fbpx
Wikipedia

Identifiable victim effect

The identifiable victim effect is the tendency of individuals to offer greater aid when a specific, identifiable person ("victim") is observed under hardship, as compared to a large, vaguely defined group with the same need.[1]

The identifiable victim effect has two components. People are more inclined to help an identified victim than an unidentified one, and people are more inclined to help a single identified victim than a group of identified victims. Although helping an identified victim may be commendable, the identifiable victim effect is considered a cognitive bias. From a consequentialist point of view, the cognitive error is the failure to offer N times as much help to N unidentified victims.

The identifiable victim effect has a mirror image that is sometimes called the identifiable perpetrator effect. Research has shown that individuals are more inclined to mete out punishment, even at their own expense, when they are punishing a specific, identified perpetrator.[2]

The conceptualization of the identifiable victim effect as it is known today is commonly attributed to American economist Thomas Schelling. He wrote that harm to a particular person invokes “anxiety and sentiment, guilt and awe, responsibility and religion, [but]…most of this awesomeness disappears when we deal with statistical death”.[3]

Historical figures from Joseph Stalin to Mother Teresa are credited with statements that epitomize the identifiable victim effect. The remark "One death is a tragedy; a million deaths is a statistic" is widely, although probably incorrectly, attributed to Stalin.[4] The remark "If I look at the mass I will never act. If I look at the one, I will," is attributed to Mother Teresa.[5]

Examples edit

This article mentions many historical incidents that have been cited as examples of the identifiable victim effect. These incidents serve as illustrative examples but do not constitute evidence that the effect exists.

Arrest of Rosa Parks edit

The conviction of Rosa Parks in 1955 for refusing to give up her bus seat in favor of a White passenger inspired the Black community to boycott the Montgomery, Alabama buses for over a year. Parks appealed her conviction, but her case never reached the U.S. Supreme Court. The Court found bus segregation unconstitutional in Browder v. Gayle, a case with four plaintiffs. Parks remains a symbol of resistance to racial segregation in the United States, while the four Browder v. Gayle plaintiffs are much less well known.

'Baby Jessica' in the well edit

On October 14, 1987, 18-month old Jessica McClure fell into a narrow well in her aunt's backyard in Midland, Texas.[6] Within hours, 'Baby Jessica', as she became known, made headlines around the US. The public reacted with sympathy towards her ordeal. While teams of rescue workers, paramedics and volunteers worked to successfully rescue 'Baby Jessica' in 58 hours, the public donated hundreds of thousands of dollars to her family. Even after Jessica was discharged from the hospital, the McClure family was flooded with cards and gifts from members of the public as well as a visit from then-Vice President George H.W. Bush and a telephone call from then-President Ronald Reagan.[7]

Drowning of Alan Kurdi edit

In September, 2015, three-year-old Syrian refugee Alan (or Aylan) Kurdi drowned when he and his family tried to reach Europe by boat. A photograph of Kurdi's body caused a dramatic upturn in international concern over the refugee crisis. The picture has been credited with causing a surge in donations to charities helping migrants and refugees, with one charity, the Migrant Offshore Aid Station, recording a 15-fold increase in donations within 24 hours of its publication.[8][9]

Murder of George Floyd edit

The murder of George Floyd by a police officer in May, 2020 led to worldwide protests against police brutality.[10][11] Almost 1,000 people are killed in the U.S. by police every year,[12] and a black male is 2.5 times as likely to be killed by police as a white male.[13] The reader is invited to introspect and consider if he or she is a thousand times more outraged by a thousand annual killings than by the killing of George Floyd.[14]

For which victims is the effect strongest? edit

Meta-analysis of experimental evidence edit

A meta-analysis[15] of studies through 2015 of the identifiable victim effect found that not all studies achieve statistical significance, and the effect size, in general, is small. In an experiment more recent than the meta-analysis, identifying a victim in COVID-19 messaging had no meaningful effect on pro-health behaviors such as hand-washing, mask-wearing, and staying at home.[16]

On the other hand, extremely subtle experimental manipulations may yield significant effects. For example, Small and Loewenstein[17] found that when the victim is identified only by a number, subjects are more inclined to help the victim if they know the victim's number when they decide whether to help than if they learn the number later.

The meta-analysis[15] found that statistical significance is enhanced and the effect size is increased under the following circumstances:

  • if the identified victim is a child
  • if a photograph of the victim is shown
  • if the victim's plight is caused by poverty rather than disease or injury
  • if the victim is perceived as not responsible for his/her plight

Vividness edit

Information that identifies a victim may include vivid details such as photographs, video, and a description of the victim's predicament. The victim may be portrayed as innocent and helpless. These details evoke emotional responses and provide a sense of familiarity and social closeness. Therefore, identified victims may elicit greater support than statistical victims.[18]

Studies have found that people respond more to concrete, graphic information than abstract, statistical information.[19] Bohnet and Frey (1999)[20] and Kogut and Ritov (2005)[21] found that vividness contributes to the identifiable victim effect. Another study by Kogut and Ritov (2005)[22] found that donations to benefit a needy child increased when the name and a picture of the child were provided. Jenni and Loewenstein (1997)[1] did not observe an effect of vividness.

In 2003, Deborah Small and George Loewenstein conducted an experiment showing that the identifiable victim effect does not result from vividness alone. Victims were identified only by number. Participants selected the victim to whom they could donate by picking a victim's number out of a bag. Participants donated significantly more money if they picked the victim's number before donating rather than afterward. [17] They donated more to an already identified victim even though the victim's identity was hidden from them.

Singularity effect edit

The identifiable victim effect disappears when a group of victims, rather than a single victim, is identified.[15] In a group of two or more victims, identifying every victim makes no difference. For example, a 2005 study by Kogut and Ritov[23] asked participants how much they would be willing to donate to either a critically ill child or a group of eight critically ill children. Although identifying the individual child increased donations, identifying every child in the group of eight did not.

Paul Slovic[5] argued that our compassion fades as the number of victims increases, and eventually collapses. He and his colleagues found that experimental subjects donated less to help two starving children than to help one.[24]

When the victim can be blamed edit

Research suggests that if an individual is seen as responsible for their plight, people offer less help if the victim is identified.[25] Most research dedicated to the identifiable victim effect avoids the topic of blame, using explicitly blameless individuals, such as children suffering from an illness.[23] However, there are real-world situations where victims may be seen as to blame for their current situation. For example, in a 2011 study by Kogut, individuals were less likely to offer help to an AIDS victim if the victim had contracted AIDS through sexual contact than if the individual was born with AIDS. In other words, individuals were less likely to offer help to victims if they were seen as at least partially responsible for their plight. A meta-study conducted in 2016 supports these findings, reporting that charitable donations were highest when the victim showed little responsibility for their victimization.[15]

In such cases where victim blaming is possible, identification of individuals may not induce sympathy and may actually increase negative perception of the victim.[25] This reduction in help is even more pronounced if the individual believes in the just world hypothesis, which is the tendency for people to blame the victim for what has happened to them. This pattern of blame results from a desire to believe that the world is predictable and orderly and that those who suffer must have done something to deserve their suffering.

Explanations edit

Researchers[1] [15] have proposed various underlying causes of the identifiable victim effect, which may work together to produce the effect. These possible causes are given below, and experimental tests are cited.

Emotional reactions edit

According to the affect heuristic, people make decisions based on emotions rather than objective analysis. A single identified victim may trigger a stronger emotional response than a group of unidentified victims. Several studies have found that identifying a victim evokes more sympathy[22][26] for the victim and more distress[21][26] at the victim's plight, along with more willingness to help.

Kogut and Ritov,[22] for example, asked participants how much they would donate to help a critically ill child. When they identified the child, feelings of distress at the child's plight increased along with donations. This supports the idea that altruistic acts may serve as coping mechanisms to alleviate negative emotions, such as distress or guilt.[27]

Effect of reference group size edit

Risk that is concentrated is perceived as greater than the same risk dispersed over a wider population. Identifiable victims are their own reference group; if they do not receive aid then the entire reference group will perish.[1][28] For example, Fetherstonhaugh et al[29] found that an intervention saving a fixed number of lives was considered less beneficial when more total lives were at risk.

Jenni and Loewenstein's[1] experimental subjects showed significantly more support for risk-reducing actions when a higher proportion of the reference group was at risk. This effect was so striking that Jenni and Loewenstein suggested that the identifiable victim effect could instead be called the “percentage of reference group saved effect”.

Perceived responsibility edit

People tend to feel more responsibility for victims who are psychologically closer to them, and people may feel closer to an identified victim. Indeed, several studies have found that individuals feel more responsibility for an identified victim.[26][30]

Identified vs statistical victims edit

Some victims cannot be identified because they are statistical. For example, we do not know whose lives would be saved if 10% more of the population were vaccinated against a disease. There are several theoretical reasons to suspect that identified victims are more likely to be helped than statistical ones:

  • People underestimate the importance of outcomes that are merely probable rather than certain. If the money sent to Baby Jessica when she was trapped in the well had been spent on preventative health care for children, many lives might plausibly have been saved, but Jessica certainly would have died if she had not been rescued in time.

    In one of Jenni and Loewenstein's[1] two experiments comparing certain and uncertain deaths, their subjects were significantly more concerned about certain deaths.
  • People regret losses more than they enjoy equivalent gains. In the Baby Jessica example above, the death of Jessica if she had not been rescued would have been a tragic loss, but the children's lives that might have been saved through preventative health care were framed as gains.
  • The decision to help an identified victim is made ex post, after the victim is in danger, but the decision to save a statistical victim is often made ex ante, to prevent danger to the individual.[31] People may feel a responsibility to an actual identified victim[32] but not to a possible victim of a future tragedy that might not occur. This explanation is closest to what Thomas Schelling implied in his now-famous paper.

    Jenni and Loewenstein (1997)[1] did not find evidence that ex post vs ex ante evaluation contributes to the identifiable victim effect, but Small and Lowenstein (2003)[17] did.

Indeed, researchers have generally found that identified victims are more likely to be helped than statistical ones.[17] For example, Small, Loewenstein, and Slovic[33] found that subjects donated much more money to help a single starving girl named Rokia than to relieve a famine described statistically.

Relation to other cognitive biases edit

The identifiable victim effect is a special case of a more general phenomenon: people respond to stories more readily than to facts. Kubin et al[34] found that people have more respect for their political opponents' opinions when their opponents support their opinions with personal experiences rather than facts. In keeping with the literature on the identifiable victim effect, they found that personal experiences involving harm are particularly effective.

The preference for helping a single individual rather than a group is sometimes called the singularity effect.[22][26] Indifference to the number of individuals helped is called scope neglect or scope insensitivity.

The identifiable victim effect has a mirror image that is sometimes called the identifiable perpetrator effect.[35][36] Research has shown that individuals are more inclined to mete out punishment, even at their own expense, when they are punishing a specific, identified perpetrator. They also exert more severe punishments and express stronger feelings of blame and anger. Even when the perpetrator is identified only by a number, subjects are more inclined to punish if they know the perpetrator’s number when they decide whether to punish than if they learn the number later.[2] This effect has also been called the “Goldstein effect,” after the fictional Emmanuel Goldstein, who was vilified as the supposed enemy of the state in George Orwell’s dystopian novel 1984.[37]

These two effects, of identifiable victims and identifiable perpetrators, suggest that there is a more general identifiable other effect, such that any identified individual evokes a stronger reaction than an equivalent but unidentified individual.[2][38][6]

Implications edit

Public policy and politics edit

Healthcare edit

The identifiable victim effect may also influence healthcare, both at the individual and national level.[39] On the individual level, doctors are more likely to recommend expensive, but potentially life-saving, treatments to an individual patient rather than to a group of patients. This effect is not limited to medical professionals, as laymen demonstrate this same bias towards providing more expensive treatments for individual patients.[39] On the national level, the American people are far more likely to contribute to an expensive treatment to save the life of one person rather than spend much smaller amounts on preventative measures that could save the lives of thousands per year. A function of American individualism, this nationwide bias towards expensive treatments is still prevalent today.[40]

Brady Bill edit

James S. Brady, the then-White House press secretary, was among three collateral damage victims in the attempted assassination of President Reagan in 1981. Brady was explicitly named in reports of the shooting in contrast to the other two injured, a District of Columbia police officer and a Secret Service agent. The political reaction was largely focused on the injuries of Brady which led to the enactment of the Brady Handgun Violence Prevention Act of 1993. It states that it is mandatory for firearm dealers to perform background searches on firearm purchasers.[41]

Ryan White Care Act edit

The need to tackle the problems faced by AIDS sufferers was brought to the political forefront as a result of the legal and social plight of one particular AIDS victim, Ryan White, who contracted HIV at age 13 and died of AIDS six years later. His circumstances and his campaign for greater funding for AIDS research were widely publicised in the media. Following his death in 1990, the US Congress passed the Ryan White Care Act, which funded the largest set of services for people living with AIDS in the country.[42]

Defunding peacekeeping efforts in Darfur edit

In November 2005, the U.S. Congress stripped $50 million from a bill that would have funded peacekeeping efforts in Darfur, where genocide was claiming hundreds of thousands of lives.[43]

Less than a month earlier, Rosa Parks had passed away. Her casket had lain in state at the rotunda of the U.S. Capitol, and many elected officials, including President George W. Bush, had attended her memorial service. Genocide expert Paul Slovic wrote, "We appropriately honor the one, Rosa Parks, but by turning away from the crisis in Darfur we are, implicitly, placing almost no value on the lives of millions there."[5]

George Santos' misstatements edit

Republican politician George Santos was elected to Congress in a formerly Democratic district after falsely claiming that his mother was a 9/11 casualty and his maternal grandparents were Jewish Holocaust refugees who had fled Soviet Ukraine and German-occupied Belgium. Actually, his mother, who died in 2016, was not in the United States on September 11, 2001. [44][45] His maternal grandparents actually lived in Brazil. [46] Whether Santos would have been elected if he had not made these (and other) misstatements is unknown.

Criminal justice edit

Since the identifiable victim effect can influence punishment, it has the potential to undermine the system of trial by jury.[2] Jurors, when deliberating, work with an identifiable alleged perpetrator, and thus may attach negative emotions (e.g. disgust, anger) to the individual or assign increased blame when handing down a harsh sentence. Policymakers, who are unable to see the individual offender, being almost entirely emotionally removed, may actually have intended a more lenient sentence. This may produce a harsher verdict than the legal guidelines recommend or allow. On the other extreme, jurors may feel sympathy, relating with the perpetrator on a level not experienced by policymakers, leading to a milder verdict than legally appropriate or allowable.[2]

Typically in crime investigations, law enforcement forces conceal any information regarding the identities of the suspects until they have strong evidence that the suspects are credible. When identities of suspects are revealed through description of their features or release of their images, media coverage and public discussion on the issue grows. On one side, the public discourse can become increasingly negative and hostile, or, if the perpetrator is sympathetic, support for the perpetrator may grow. This is because people experience a greater emotional reaction towards a concrete, identifiable perpetrator than an abstract, unidentifiable one.[2]

Business ethics edit

Yam and Reynolds[27] speculated that the growing anonymity of parties to business transactions may contribute to an increase in unethical business behavior. They hypothesized that because of increasing corporate size, the ubiquity of e-commerce, and the commoditization of workers, business executives are less likely to know their employees, customers, and shareholders, and therefore they might be more willing to exploit these unidentified potential victims.

In one of their experiments, they asked their subjects if they would approve of withholding a drug from the market if releasing it later would bring a greater profit. Their experimental subjects voiced more approval for withholding the drug if all of the victims of the disease the drug treats were unidentified than if one of the victims was named.

Other researchers suggested that outside observers, not only perpetrators, view unethical behavior as less unethical if the victim of the unethical behavior is unidentified.[47] This could possibly result in less public outcry against unethical practices in a globalized business environment, where the victims are often unseen.

Personality differences among potential helpers edit

Attachment anxiety edit

High levels of attachment anxiety may increase the power of identifiable victim effect.

When presented with an identified victim, individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety tend to donate more money than the average individual.[48] Research suggests that anxiously attached people experience significantly more personal distress than those securely attached when confronted with victims in need, so they donate more in order to relieve their distress.[49]

When presented with an unidentified victim, individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety tend to donate less money than the average individual. Research suggests that unidentified victims do not provoke distress, and that anxiously attached people focus on their own vulnerabilities, so that they have less inclination to help unidentified others.

Although anxiously attached people may participate in prosocial behaviors, such as donating money to a charity, researchers hypothesize that their actions are not the result of altruistic tendencies, but instead are "positively correlated with egoistic, rather than altruistic motives for helping and volunteering,"[48]: 652  and that anxiously attached people engage in pro-social behavior mainly when strenuous effort is not required.[48]

Guilt edit

Yam and Reynolds[27] investigated the propensity to victimize others. Their experimental subjects harmed unidentified others more than named others, and anticipated that harming named others would provoke more guilt than harming unidentified others. The experimenters did not test whether people who are more prone to feelings of guilt experience the identified victim effect more intensely.

Reasoning style edit

Research suggests that individual differences in reasoning style moderate the identifiable victim effect.[50] Two different methods of reasoning are “experiential” and “rational”. Experiential thinking (e.g. emotionally-based thinking) is automatic, contextual and fluid, and rational thinking (e.g. logically based thinking) is deliberative, analytical, and decontextualized. Experiential thinking styles may increase the power of the identifiable victim effect, and rational thinking styles may decrease the power of the identifiable victim effect. Researchers theorize that these differences result because experiential thinkers rely on emotional responses towards an issue when making a decision. In contrast, rational thinkers analyze the situation as a whole before making a decision. Thus, a person thinking rationally would respond to all victims equally, not giving preference to those specifically named or otherwise identified, just as experiential thinkers would be drawn towards the more emotionally charged identified victim.[50] However, research conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic found that identifiable victim effects on public health promoting behaviors were not only undetected, but also not mediated by behavioral tests of reasoning style.[16]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g Jenni, Karen; Loewenstein, George (1997-05-01). "Explaining the Identifiable Victim Effect" (PDF). Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 14 (3): 235–257. doi:10.1023/A:1007740225484. ISSN 0895-5646. S2CID 8498645.
  2. ^ a b c d e f Small, Deborah A.; Loewenstein, George (2005-12-01). "The devil you know: the effects of identifiability on punishment". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 18 (5): 311–318. doi:10.1002/bdm.507. ISSN 1099-0771.
  3. ^ Schelling, Thomas, C (1968). Chase (ed.). "The Life You Save May Be Your Own". Problems in Public Expenditure Analysis: 127–162.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ "A Single Death is a Tragedy; a Million Deaths is a Statistic – Quote Investigator".
  5. ^ a b c Slovic, Paul (April 2007). ""If I look at the mass I will never act": Psychic numbing and genocide" (PDF). Judgment and Decision Making. 2 (2). Society for Judgment and Decision Making: 79–95. doi:10.1017/S1930297500000061.
  6. ^ a b Loewenstein, George; Small, Deborah; Strnad, Jeff (2005-03-01), Statistical, Identifiable and Iconic Victims and Perpetrators, Rochester, NY, SSRN 678281{{citation}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  7. ^ "October 16, 1987 : Baby Jessica rescued from a well as the world watches". history.com. 2011. Retrieved 2017-02-12.
  8. ^ Henley, Jon (3 September 2015). "Britons rally to help people fleeing war and terror in Middle East". The Guardian. Retrieved 3 September 2015.
  9. ^ "Bias in the Spotlight: Identifiable Victim Effect". The Marketing Society.
  10. ^ "Protests across the globe after George Floyd's death". CNN. June 6, 2020. from the original on September 17, 2020. Retrieved September 14, 2020.
  11. ^ "George Floyd death: Violence erupts on sixth day of protests". BBC News. June 1, 2020. from the original on June 6, 2020. Retrieved June 13, 2020.
  12. ^ "Fatal Force". The Washington Post. Retrieved July 21, 2021.
  13. ^ Edwards, Frank; Lee, Hedwig; Esposito, Michael (August 20, 2019). "Risk of being killed by police use of force in the United States by age, race–ethnicity, and sex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 116 (34): 16793–16798. Bibcode:2019PNAS..11616793E. doi:10.1073/pnas.1821204116. PMC 6708348. PMID 31383756.
  14. ^ "WHY PEOPLE GET UPSET ABOUT THE WRONG STORIES". Media vs Reality. February 6, 2021.
  15. ^ a b c d e Lee, Seyoung; Feeley, Thomas Hugh (2016-07-02). "The identifiable victim effect: a meta-analytic review". Social Influence. 11 (3): 199–215. doi:10.1080/15534510.2016.1216891. ISSN 1553-4510. S2CID 152232362.
  16. ^ a b Byrd, Nick; Białek, Michał (July 2021). "Your health vs. my liberty: Philosophical beliefs dominated reflection and identifiable victim effects when predicting public health recommendation compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic". Cognition. 212: 104649. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104649. PMC 8599940. PMID 33756152.
  17. ^ a b c d Small, Deborah A.; Loewenstein, George (January 2003). "Helping a Victim or Helping the Victim: Altruism and Identifiability" (PDF). Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 26 (1): 5–16. doi:10.1023/A:1022299422219. S2CID 207550600.
  18. ^ Collins, Rebecca L.; Taylor, Shelley E.; Wood, Joanne V.; Thompson, Suzanne C. (1988-01-01). "The vividness effect: Elusive or illusory?". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 24 (1): 1–18. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(88)90041-8.
  19. ^ Nisbett, Richard; Ross, Lee (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Prentice-Hall. ISBN 978-0134451305.
  20. ^ Bohnet, Iris; Frey, Bruno S. (1999). "Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games: Comment". The American Economic Review. 89 (1): 335–339. doi:10.1257/aer.89.1.335.
  21. ^ a b Kogut, Tehila; Ritov, Ilana (2005). "The "Identified Victim" Effect: An Identified Group, or Just a Single Individual?" (PDF). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 18 (3): 157–167. doi:10.1002/bdm.492.
  22. ^ a b c d Kogut, Tehila; Ritov, Ilana (2005). "The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluation". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 97 (2): 106–116. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.02.003.
  23. ^ a b Kogut, T.; Ritov, I. (2005). "The "identified victim" effect: an identified group, or just a single individual?" (PDF). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 18 (3): 157–165. doi:10.1002/bdm.492.
  24. ^ Västfjäll, Daniel; Slovic, Paul; Mayorga, Marcus; Peters, Ellen (June 18, 2014). "Compassion Fade: Affect and Charity are Greatest for a Single Child in Need". PLOS ONE. 9 (6): e100115. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9j0115V. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0100115. PMC 4062481. PMID 24940738.
  25. ^ a b Kogut, T (2011). "Someone to blame: when identifying a victim decreases helping" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 47 (4): 748–755. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.02.011.
  26. ^ a b c d Erlandsson, A.; Björklund, F.; Bäckström, M. (2015). "Emotional reactions, perceived impact and perceived responsibility mediate the identifiable victim effect, proportion dominance effect and in-group effect respectively". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 127: 1–14. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.11.003. S2CID 17321619.
  27. ^ a b c Yam, K. C.; Reynolds, S. J. (2016). "The effects of victim anonymity on unethical behavior". Journal of Business Ethics. 136 (1): 13–22. doi:10.1007/s10551-014-2367-5. S2CID 144057330.
  28. ^ Bartels, Daniel M. (2006). "Proportion dominance: The generality and variability of favoring relative savings over absolute savings". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 100: 76–95. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.004.
  29. ^ Fetherstonhaugh, David; Slovic, Paul; Johnson, Stephen; James, Friedrich (May 1997). "Insensitivity to the Value of Human Life: A Study of Psychophysical Numbing". Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 14 (3): 283–300. doi:10.1023/A:1007744326393. S2CID 51033376.
  30. ^ Small, D.A. (2015). "On the psychology of the identifiable victim effect". In Cohen, I.G.; Daniels, N.; Eyal, N. (eds.). Identified vs. statistical lives: An interdisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–23.
  31. ^ Weinstein, M; Shepard, D; Pliskin, J (1980). "The Economic Value of Changing Mortality Probabilities: A Decision-Theoretic Approach". The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 94 (2): 373–396. doi:10.2307/1884546. JSTOR 1884546.
  32. ^ Douglas, Professor Mary; Douglas, Professor of Anthropology Mary (2013-06-17). Risk and Blame. Routledge. ISBN 9781136490040.
  33. ^ Small, Deborah A.; Loewenstein, George; Slovic, Paul (March 2007). "Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims"". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 102 (2): 143–153. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.01.005.
  34. ^ Kubin, Emily; Puryear, Curtis; Shein, Chelsea; Gray, Kurt. "Personal experiences bridge moral and political divides better than facts". PNAS. 118 (6). doi:10.1073/pnas.2008389118. PMC 8017692.
  35. ^ Dickert, Stephan (March 2008). "TWO ROUTES TO THE PERCEPTION OF NEED: THE ROLE OF AFFECTIVE VS. DELIBERATIVE INFORMATION PROCESSING IN PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR" (PDF).
  36. ^ Rowell, Arden; Bilz, Kenworthey (February 2021). "2". The Psychology of Environmental Law. NYU Press. p. 60. doi:10.18574/nyu/9781479812301.001.0001. ISBN 9781479807574. S2CID 244309090.
  37. ^ Sunstein, Cass R. (2009). Worst-Case Scenarios. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. p. 63. ISBN 978-0-674-03251-4.
  38. ^ Loewenstein, George; Small, Deborah A.; Strnad, Jeff (March 7, 2005). "Statistical, Identifiable and Iconic Victims and Perpetrators".
  39. ^ a b Redelmeir, D. A.; Tversky, A. (1990). (PDF). New England Journal of Medicine. 322 (16): 1162–1164. doi:10.1056/nejm199004193221620. PMID 2320089. S2CID 142513619. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-04-28.
  40. ^ Toufexis, A.; Bjerklie, D. (1993). "The ultimate choice". Time. 142 (9): 43–4. PMID 11645235.
  41. ^ Longley, Robert (August 23, 2016). "Brady Act Gun Buyer Background Checks". thoughtco.com.
  42. ^ . dhhr.wv.gov. Archived from the original on 2021-05-23. Retrieved 2017-03-13.
  43. ^ "Rice Appeals to Congress on Darfur". New York Times. December 17, 2005. Retrieved May 8, 2023.
  44. ^ Samu, Sheena; Milton, Tom; Brown, Erica (December 22, 2022). "Priest recalls George Santos cries of poverty — saying family could not afford a funeral for his mother". WCBS-TV. from the original on December 23, 2022. Retrieved December 24, 2022.
  45. ^ Paybarah, Azi (January 18, 2023). "Records show Rep. George Santos's mother wasn't in New York on 9/11". The Washington Post. from the original on January 18, 2023. Retrieved January 18, 2023.
  46. ^ Silverstein, Andrew (December 21, 2022). "Congressman-elect George Santos lied about grandparents fleeing anti-Jewish persecution during WWII". The Forward. from the original on December 21, 2022. Retrieved December 21, 2022.
  47. ^ Gino, F.; Shu, L. L.; Bazerman, M. H. (2010). "Nameless + harmless = blameless: When seemingly irrelevant factors influence judgment of (un)ethical behavior". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 111 (2): 93–101. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.11.001.
  48. ^ a b c Kogut, T.; Kogut, E. (2013). "Exploring the relationship between adult attachment style and the identifiable victim effect in helping behavior" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 49 (4): 651–660. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.011.
  49. ^ Mikulincer, M.; Gillath, O.; Halevy, V.; Avihou, N.; Avidan, S.; et al. (2001). "Attachment theory and reactions to others' needs: Evidence that activation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (6): 1205–1224. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1205. PMID 11761318.
  50. ^ a b Friedrich, J.; McGuire, A. (2010). "Individual differences in reasoning style as a moderator of the identifiable victim effect". Social Influence. 5 (3): 182–201. doi:10.1080/15534511003707352. S2CID 143401716.

identifiable, victim, effect, identifiable, victim, effect, tendency, individuals, offer, greater, when, specific, identifiable, person, victim, observed, under, hardship, compared, large, vaguely, defined, group, with, same, need, identifiable, victim, effect. The identifiable victim effect is the tendency of individuals to offer greater aid when a specific identifiable person victim is observed under hardship as compared to a large vaguely defined group with the same need 1 The identifiable victim effect has two components People are more inclined to help an identified victim than an unidentified one and people are more inclined to help a single identified victim than a group of identified victims Although helping an identified victim may be commendable the identifiable victim effect is considered a cognitive bias From a consequentialist point of view the cognitive error is the failure to offer N times as much help to N unidentified victims The identifiable victim effect has a mirror image that is sometimes called the identifiable perpetrator effect Research has shown that individuals are more inclined to mete out punishment even at their own expense when they are punishing a specific identified perpetrator 2 The conceptualization of the identifiable victim effect as it is known today is commonly attributed to American economist Thomas Schelling He wrote that harm to a particular person invokes anxiety and sentiment guilt and awe responsibility and religion but most of this awesomeness disappears when we deal with statistical death 3 Historical figures from Joseph Stalin to Mother Teresa are credited with statements that epitomize the identifiable victim effect The remark One death is a tragedy a million deaths is a statistic is widely although probably incorrectly attributed to Stalin 4 The remark If I look at the mass I will never act If I look at the one I will is attributed to Mother Teresa 5 Contents 1 Examples 1 1 Arrest of Rosa Parks 1 2 Baby Jessica in the well 1 3 Drowning of Alan Kurdi 1 4 Murder of George Floyd 2 For which victims is the effect strongest 2 1 Meta analysis of experimental evidence 2 2 Vividness 2 3 Singularity effect 2 4 When the victim can be blamed 3 Explanations 3 1 Emotional reactions 3 2 Effect of reference group size 3 3 Perceived responsibility 3 4 Identified vs statistical victims 4 Relation to other cognitive biases 5 Implications 5 1 Public policy and politics 5 1 1 Healthcare 5 1 1 1 Brady Bill 5 1 1 2 Ryan White Care Act 5 1 2 Defunding peacekeeping efforts in Darfur 5 1 3 George Santos misstatements 5 1 4 Criminal justice 5 2 Business ethics 6 Personality differences among potential helpers 6 1 Attachment anxiety 6 2 Guilt 6 3 Reasoning style 7 See also 8 ReferencesExamples editThis article mentions many historical incidents that have been cited as examples of the identifiable victim effect These incidents serve as illustrative examples but do not constitute evidence that the effect exists Arrest of Rosa Parks edit The conviction of Rosa Parks in 1955 for refusing to give up her bus seat in favor of a White passenger inspired the Black community to boycott the Montgomery Alabama buses for over a year Parks appealed her conviction but her case never reached the U S Supreme Court The Court found bus segregation unconstitutional in Browder v Gayle a case with four plaintiffs Parks remains a symbol of resistance to racial segregation in the United States while the four Browder v Gayle plaintiffs are much less well known Baby Jessica in the well edit On October 14 1987 18 month old Jessica McClure fell into a narrow well in her aunt s backyard in Midland Texas 6 Within hours Baby Jessica as she became known made headlines around the US The public reacted with sympathy towards her ordeal While teams of rescue workers paramedics and volunteers worked to successfully rescue Baby Jessica in 58 hours the public donated hundreds of thousands of dollars to her family Even after Jessica was discharged from the hospital the McClure family was flooded with cards and gifts from members of the public as well as a visit from then Vice President George H W Bush and a telephone call from then President Ronald Reagan 7 Drowning of Alan Kurdi edit In September 2015 three year old Syrian refugee Alan or Aylan Kurdi drowned when he and his family tried to reach Europe by boat A photograph of Kurdi s body caused a dramatic upturn in international concern over the refugee crisis The picture has been credited with causing a surge in donations to charities helping migrants and refugees with one charity the Migrant Offshore Aid Station recording a 15 fold increase in donations within 24 hours of its publication 8 9 Murder of George Floyd edit The murder of George Floyd by a police officer in May 2020 led to worldwide protests against police brutality 10 11 Almost 1 000 people are killed in the U S by police every year 12 and a black male is 2 5 times as likely to be killed by police as a white male 13 The reader is invited to introspect and consider if he or she is a thousand times more outraged by a thousand annual killings than by the killing of George Floyd 14 For which victims is the effect strongest editMeta analysis of experimental evidence edit A meta analysis 15 of studies through 2015 of the identifiable victim effect found that not all studies achieve statistical significance and the effect size in general is small In an experiment more recent than the meta analysis identifying a victim in COVID 19 messaging had no meaningful effect on pro health behaviors such as hand washing mask wearing and staying at home 16 On the other hand extremely subtle experimental manipulations may yield significant effects For example Small and Loewenstein 17 found that when the victim is identified only by a number subjects are more inclined to help the victim if they know the victim s number when they decide whether to help than if they learn the number later The meta analysis 15 found that statistical significance is enhanced and the effect size is increased under the following circumstances if the identified victim is a child if a photograph of the victim is shown if the victim s plight is caused by poverty rather than disease or injury if the victim is perceived as not responsible for his her plightVividness edit Information that identifies a victim may include vivid details such as photographs video and a description of the victim s predicament The victim may be portrayed as innocent and helpless These details evoke emotional responses and provide a sense of familiarity and social closeness Therefore identified victims may elicit greater support than statistical victims 18 Studies have found that people respond more to concrete graphic information than abstract statistical information 19 Bohnet and Frey 1999 20 and Kogut and Ritov 2005 21 found that vividness contributes to the identifiable victim effect Another study by Kogut and Ritov 2005 22 found that donations to benefit a needy child increased when the name and a picture of the child were provided Jenni and Loewenstein 1997 1 did not observe an effect of vividness In 2003 Deborah Small and George Loewenstein conducted an experiment showing that the identifiable victim effect does not result from vividness alone Victims were identified only by number Participants selected the victim to whom they could donate by picking a victim s number out of a bag Participants donated significantly more money if they picked the victim s number before donating rather than afterward 17 They donated more to an already identified victim even though the victim s identity was hidden from them Singularity effect edit The identifiable victim effect disappears when a group of victims rather than a single victim is identified 15 In a group of two or more victims identifying every victim makes no difference For example a 2005 study by Kogut and Ritov 23 asked participants how much they would be willing to donate to either a critically ill child or a group of eight critically ill children Although identifying the individual child increased donations identifying every child in the group of eight did not Paul Slovic 5 argued that our compassion fades as the number of victims increases and eventually collapses He and his colleagues found that experimental subjects donated less to help two starving children than to help one 24 When the victim can be blamed edit Research suggests that if an individual is seen as responsible for their plight people offer less help if the victim is identified 25 Most research dedicated to the identifiable victim effect avoids the topic of blame using explicitly blameless individuals such as children suffering from an illness 23 However there are real world situations where victims may be seen as to blame for their current situation For example in a 2011 study by Kogut individuals were less likely to offer help to an AIDS victim if the victim had contracted AIDS through sexual contact than if the individual was born with AIDS In other words individuals were less likely to offer help to victims if they were seen as at least partially responsible for their plight A meta study conducted in 2016 supports these findings reporting that charitable donations were highest when the victim showed little responsibility for their victimization 15 In such cases where victim blaming is possible identification of individuals may not induce sympathy and may actually increase negative perception of the victim 25 This reduction in help is even more pronounced if the individual believes in the just world hypothesis which is the tendency for people to blame the victim for what has happened to them This pattern of blame results from a desire to believe that the world is predictable and orderly and that those who suffer must have done something to deserve their suffering Explanations editResearchers 1 15 have proposed various underlying causes of the identifiable victim effect which may work together to produce the effect These possible causes are given below and experimental tests are cited Emotional reactions edit According to the affect heuristic people make decisions based on emotions rather than objective analysis A single identified victim may trigger a stronger emotional response than a group of unidentified victims Several studies have found that identifying a victim evokes more sympathy 22 26 for the victim and more distress 21 26 at the victim s plight along with more willingness to help Kogut and Ritov 22 for example asked participants how much they would donate to help a critically ill child When they identified the child feelings of distress at the child s plight increased along with donations This supports the idea that altruistic acts may serve as coping mechanisms to alleviate negative emotions such as distress or guilt 27 Effect of reference group size edit Risk that is concentrated is perceived as greater than the same risk dispersed over a wider population Identifiable victims are their own reference group if they do not receive aid then the entire reference group will perish 1 28 For example Fetherstonhaugh et al 29 found that an intervention saving a fixed number of lives was considered less beneficial when more total lives were at risk Jenni and Loewenstein s 1 experimental subjects showed significantly more support for risk reducing actions when a higher proportion of the reference group was at risk This effect was so striking that Jenni and Loewenstein suggested that the identifiable victim effect could instead be called the percentage of reference group saved effect Perceived responsibility edit People tend to feel more responsibility for victims who are psychologically closer to them and people may feel closer to an identified victim Indeed several studies have found that individuals feel more responsibility for an identified victim 26 30 Identified vs statistical victims edit Some victims cannot be identified because they are statistical For example we do not know whose lives would be saved if 10 more of the population were vaccinated against a disease There are several theoretical reasons to suspect that identified victims are more likely to be helped than statistical ones People underestimate the importance of outcomes that are merely probable rather than certain If the money sent to Baby Jessica when she was trapped in the well had been spent on preventative health care for children many lives might plausibly have been saved but Jessica certainly would have died if she had not been rescued in time In one of Jenni and Loewenstein s 1 two experiments comparing certain and uncertain deaths their subjects were significantly more concerned about certain deaths People regret losses more than they enjoy equivalent gains In the Baby Jessica example above the death of Jessica if she had not been rescued would have been a tragic loss but the children s lives that might have been saved through preventative health care were framed as gains The decision to help an identified victim is made ex post after the victim is in danger but the decision to save a statistical victim is often made ex ante to prevent danger to the individual 31 People may feel a responsibility to an actual identified victim 32 but not to a possible victim of a future tragedy that might not occur This explanation is closest to what Thomas Schelling implied in his now famous paper Jenni and Loewenstein 1997 1 did not find evidence that ex post vs ex ante evaluation contributes to the identifiable victim effect but Small and Lowenstein 2003 17 did Indeed researchers have generally found that identified victims are more likely to be helped than statistical ones 17 For example Small Loewenstein and Slovic 33 found that subjects donated much more money to help a single starving girl named Rokia than to relieve a famine described statistically Relation to other cognitive biases editThe identifiable victim effect is a special case of a more general phenomenon people respond to stories more readily than to facts Kubin et al 34 found that people have more respect for their political opponents opinions when their opponents support their opinions with personal experiences rather than facts In keeping with the literature on the identifiable victim effect they found that personal experiences involving harm are particularly effective The preference for helping a single individual rather than a group is sometimes called the singularity effect 22 26 Indifference to the number of individuals helped is called scope neglect or scope insensitivity The identifiable victim effect has a mirror image that is sometimes called the identifiable perpetrator effect 35 36 Research has shown that individuals are more inclined to mete out punishment even at their own expense when they are punishing a specific identified perpetrator They also exert more severe punishments and express stronger feelings of blame and anger Even when the perpetrator is identified only by a number subjects are more inclined to punish if they know the perpetrator s number when they decide whether to punish than if they learn the number later 2 This effect has also been called the Goldstein effect after the fictional Emmanuel Goldstein who was vilified as the supposed enemy of the state in George Orwell s dystopian novel 1984 37 These two effects of identifiable victims and identifiable perpetrators suggest that there is a more general identifiable other effect such that any identified individual evokes a stronger reaction than an equivalent but unidentified individual 2 38 6 Implications editPublic policy and politics edit Healthcare edit The identifiable victim effect may also influence healthcare both at the individual and national level 39 On the individual level doctors are more likely to recommend expensive but potentially life saving treatments to an individual patient rather than to a group of patients This effect is not limited to medical professionals as laymen demonstrate this same bias towards providing more expensive treatments for individual patients 39 On the national level the American people are far more likely to contribute to an expensive treatment to save the life of one person rather than spend much smaller amounts on preventative measures that could save the lives of thousands per year A function of American individualism this nationwide bias towards expensive treatments is still prevalent today 40 Brady Bill edit James S Brady the then White House press secretary was among three collateral damage victims in the attempted assassination of President Reagan in 1981 Brady was explicitly named in reports of the shooting in contrast to the other two injured a District of Columbia police officer and a Secret Service agent The political reaction was largely focused on the injuries of Brady which led to the enactment of the Brady Handgun Violence Prevention Act of 1993 It states that it is mandatory for firearm dealers to perform background searches on firearm purchasers 41 Ryan White Care Act edit The need to tackle the problems faced by AIDS sufferers was brought to the political forefront as a result of the legal and social plight of one particular AIDS victim Ryan White who contracted HIV at age 13 and died of AIDS six years later His circumstances and his campaign for greater funding for AIDS research were widely publicised in the media Following his death in 1990 the US Congress passed the Ryan White Care Act which funded the largest set of services for people living with AIDS in the country 42 Defunding peacekeeping efforts in Darfur edit In November 2005 the U S Congress stripped 50 million from a bill that would have funded peacekeeping efforts in Darfur where genocide was claiming hundreds of thousands of lives 43 Less than a month earlier Rosa Parks had passed away Her casket had lain in state at the rotunda of the U S Capitol and many elected officials including President George W Bush had attended her memorial service Genocide expert Paul Slovic wrote We appropriately honor the one Rosa Parks but by turning away from the crisis in Darfur we are implicitly placing almost no value on the lives of millions there 5 George Santos misstatements edit Republican politician George Santos was elected to Congress in a formerly Democratic district after falsely claiming that his mother was a 9 11 casualty and his maternal grandparents were Jewish Holocaust refugees who had fled Soviet Ukraine and German occupied Belgium Actually his mother who died in 2016 was not in the United States on September 11 2001 44 45 His maternal grandparents actually lived in Brazil 46 Whether Santos would have been elected if he had not made these and other misstatements is unknown Criminal justice edit Since the identifiable victim effect can influence punishment it has the potential to undermine the system of trial by jury 2 Jurors when deliberating work with an identifiable alleged perpetrator and thus may attach negative emotions e g disgust anger to the individual or assign increased blame when handing down a harsh sentence Policymakers who are unable to see the individual offender being almost entirely emotionally removed may actually have intended a more lenient sentence This may produce a harsher verdict than the legal guidelines recommend or allow On the other extreme jurors may feel sympathy relating with the perpetrator on a level not experienced by policymakers leading to a milder verdict than legally appropriate or allowable 2 Typically in crime investigations law enforcement forces conceal any information regarding the identities of the suspects until they have strong evidence that the suspects are credible When identities of suspects are revealed through description of their features or release of their images media coverage and public discussion on the issue grows On one side the public discourse can become increasingly negative and hostile or if the perpetrator is sympathetic support for the perpetrator may grow This is because people experience a greater emotional reaction towards a concrete identifiable perpetrator than an abstract unidentifiable one 2 Business ethics edit Yam and Reynolds 27 speculated that the growing anonymity of parties to business transactions may contribute to an increase in unethical business behavior They hypothesized that because of increasing corporate size the ubiquity of e commerce and the commoditization of workers business executives are less likely to know their employees customers and shareholders and therefore they might be more willing to exploit these unidentified potential victims In one of their experiments they asked their subjects if they would approve of withholding a drug from the market if releasing it later would bring a greater profit Their experimental subjects voiced more approval for withholding the drug if all of the victims of the disease the drug treats were unidentified than if one of the victims was named Other researchers suggested that outside observers not only perpetrators view unethical behavior as less unethical if the victim of the unethical behavior is unidentified 47 This could possibly result in less public outcry against unethical practices in a globalized business environment where the victims are often unseen Personality differences among potential helpers editAttachment anxiety edit High levels of attachment anxiety may increase the power of identifiable victim effect When presented with an identified victim individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety tend to donate more money than the average individual 48 Research suggests that anxiously attached people experience significantly more personal distress than those securely attached when confronted with victims in need so they donate more in order to relieve their distress 49 When presented with an unidentified victim individuals with high levels of attachment anxiety tend to donate less money than the average individual Research suggests that unidentified victims do not provoke distress and that anxiously attached people focus on their own vulnerabilities so that they have less inclination to help unidentified others Although anxiously attached people may participate in prosocial behaviors such as donating money to a charity researchers hypothesize that their actions are not the result of altruistic tendencies but instead are positively correlated with egoistic rather than altruistic motives for helping and volunteering 48 652 and that anxiously attached people engage in pro social behavior mainly when strenuous effort is not required 48 Guilt edit Yam and Reynolds 27 investigated the propensity to victimize others Their experimental subjects harmed unidentified others more than named others and anticipated that harming named others would provoke more guilt than harming unidentified others The experimenters did not test whether people who are more prone to feelings of guilt experience the identified victim effect more intensely Reasoning style edit Research suggests that individual differences in reasoning style moderate the identifiable victim effect 50 Two different methods of reasoning are experiential and rational Experiential thinking e g emotionally based thinking is automatic contextual and fluid and rational thinking e g logically based thinking is deliberative analytical and decontextualized Experiential thinking styles may increase the power of the identifiable victim effect and rational thinking styles may decrease the power of the identifiable victim effect Researchers theorize that these differences result because experiential thinkers rely on emotional responses towards an issue when making a decision In contrast rational thinkers analyze the situation as a whole before making a decision Thus a person thinking rationally would respond to all victims equally not giving preference to those specifically named or otherwise identified just as experiential thinkers would be drawn towards the more emotionally charged identified victim 50 However research conducted during the COVID 19 pandemic found that identifiable victim effects on public health promoting behaviors were not only undetected but also not mediated by behavioral tests of reasoning style 16 See also editCompassion fade List of cognitive biases Moral psychologyReferences edit a b c d e f g Jenni Karen Loewenstein George 1997 05 01 Explaining the Identifiable Victim Effect PDF Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 14 3 235 257 doi 10 1023 A 1007740225484 ISSN 0895 5646 S2CID 8498645 a b c d e f Small Deborah A Loewenstein George 2005 12 01 The devil you know the effects of identifiability on punishment Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18 5 311 318 doi 10 1002 bdm 507 ISSN 1099 0771 Schelling Thomas C 1968 Chase ed The Life You Save May Be Your Own Problems in Public Expenditure Analysis 127 162 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link A Single Death is a Tragedy a Million Deaths is a Statistic Quote Investigator a b c Slovic Paul April 2007 If I look at the mass I will never act Psychic numbing and genocide PDF Judgment and Decision Making 2 2 Society for Judgment and Decision Making 79 95 doi 10 1017 S1930297500000061 a b Loewenstein George Small Deborah Strnad Jeff 2005 03 01 Statistical Identifiable and Iconic Victims and Perpetrators Rochester NY SSRN 678281 a href Template Citation html title Template Citation citation a CS1 maint location missing publisher link October 16 1987 Baby Jessica rescued from a well as the world watches history com 2011 Retrieved 2017 02 12 Henley Jon 3 September 2015 Britons rally to help people fleeing war and terror in Middle East The Guardian Retrieved 3 September 2015 Bias in the Spotlight Identifiable Victim Effect The Marketing Society Protests across the globe after George Floyd s death CNN June 6 2020 Archived from the original on September 17 2020 Retrieved September 14 2020 George Floyd death Violence erupts on sixth day of protests BBC News June 1 2020 Archived from the original on June 6 2020 Retrieved June 13 2020 Fatal Force The Washington Post Retrieved July 21 2021 Edwards Frank Lee Hedwig Esposito Michael August 20 2019 Risk of being killed by police use of force in the United States by age race ethnicity and sex Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 116 34 16793 16798 Bibcode 2019PNAS 11616793E doi 10 1073 pnas 1821204116 PMC 6708348 PMID 31383756 WHY PEOPLE GET UPSET ABOUT THE WRONG STORIES Media vs Reality February 6 2021 a b c d e Lee Seyoung Feeley Thomas Hugh 2016 07 02 The identifiable victim effect a meta analytic review Social Influence 11 3 199 215 doi 10 1080 15534510 2016 1216891 ISSN 1553 4510 S2CID 152232362 a b Byrd Nick Bialek Michal July 2021 Your health vs my liberty Philosophical beliefs dominated reflection and identifiable victim effects when predicting public health recommendation compliance during the COVID 19 pandemic Cognition 212 104649 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2021 104649 PMC 8599940 PMID 33756152 a b c d Small Deborah A Loewenstein George January 2003 Helping a Victim or Helping the Victim Altruism and Identifiability PDF Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26 1 5 16 doi 10 1023 A 1022299422219 S2CID 207550600 Collins Rebecca L Taylor Shelley E Wood Joanne V Thompson Suzanne C 1988 01 01 The vividness effect Elusive or illusory Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 24 1 1 18 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 88 90041 8 Nisbett Richard Ross Lee 1980 Human Inference Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0134451305 Bohnet Iris Frey Bruno S 1999 Social distance and other regarding behavior in dictator games Comment The American Economic Review 89 1 335 339 doi 10 1257 aer 89 1 335 a b Kogut Tehila Ritov Ilana 2005 The Identified Victim Effect An Identified Group or Just a Single Individual PDF Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18 3 157 167 doi 10 1002 bdm 492 a b c d Kogut Tehila Ritov Ilana 2005 The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluation Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 97 2 106 116 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2005 02 003 a b Kogut T Ritov I 2005 The identified victim effect an identified group or just a single individual PDF Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18 3 157 165 doi 10 1002 bdm 492 Vastfjall Daniel Slovic Paul Mayorga Marcus Peters Ellen June 18 2014 Compassion Fade Affect and Charity are Greatest for a Single Child in Need PLOS ONE 9 6 e100115 Bibcode 2014PLoSO 9j0115V doi 10 1371 journal pone 0100115 PMC 4062481 PMID 24940738 a b Kogut T 2011 Someone to blame when identifying a victim decreases helping PDF Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 4 748 755 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2011 02 011 a b c d Erlandsson A Bjorklund F Backstrom M 2015 Emotional reactions perceived impact and perceived responsibility mediate the identifiable victim effect proportion dominance effect and in group effect respectively Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 127 1 14 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2014 11 003 S2CID 17321619 a b c Yam K C Reynolds S J 2016 The effects of victim anonymity on unethical behavior Journal of Business Ethics 136 1 13 22 doi 10 1007 s10551 014 2367 5 S2CID 144057330 Bartels Daniel M 2006 Proportion dominance The generality and variability of favoring relative savings over absolute savings Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 100 76 95 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2005 10 004 Fetherstonhaugh David Slovic Paul Johnson Stephen James Friedrich May 1997 Insensitivity to the Value of Human Life A Study of Psychophysical Numbing Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 14 3 283 300 doi 10 1023 A 1007744326393 S2CID 51033376 Small D A 2015 On the psychology of the identifiable victim effect In Cohen I G Daniels N Eyal N eds Identified vs statistical lives An interdisciplinary perspective Oxford University Press pp 13 23 Weinstein M Shepard D Pliskin J 1980 The Economic Value of Changing Mortality Probabilities A Decision Theoretic Approach The Quarterly Journal of Economics 94 2 373 396 doi 10 2307 1884546 JSTOR 1884546 Douglas Professor Mary Douglas Professor of Anthropology Mary 2013 06 17 Risk and Blame Routledge ISBN 9781136490040 Small Deborah A Loewenstein George Slovic Paul March 2007 Sympathy and callousness The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 102 2 143 153 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2006 01 005 Kubin Emily Puryear Curtis Shein Chelsea Gray Kurt Personal experiences bridge moral and political divides better than facts PNAS 118 6 doi 10 1073 pnas 2008389118 PMC 8017692 Dickert Stephan March 2008 TWO ROUTES TO THE PERCEPTION OF NEED THE ROLE OF AFFECTIVE VS DELIBERATIVE INFORMATION PROCESSING IN PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR PDF Rowell Arden Bilz Kenworthey February 2021 2 The Psychology of Environmental Law NYU Press p 60 doi 10 18574 nyu 9781479812301 001 0001 ISBN 9781479807574 S2CID 244309090 Sunstein Cass R 2009 Worst Case Scenarios Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press p 63 ISBN 978 0 674 03251 4 Loewenstein George Small Deborah A Strnad Jeff March 7 2005 Statistical Identifiable and Iconic Victims and Perpetrators a b Redelmeir D A Tversky A 1990 Discrepancy between medical decisions for individual patients and for groups PDF New England Journal of Medicine 322 16 1162 1164 doi 10 1056 nejm199004193221620 PMID 2320089 S2CID 142513619 Archived from the original PDF on 2017 04 28 Toufexis A Bjerklie D 1993 The ultimate choice Time 142 9 43 4 PMID 11645235 Longley Robert August 23 2016 Brady Act Gun Buyer Background Checks thoughtco com Ryan White and Care Act History dhhr wv gov Archived from the original on 2021 05 23 Retrieved 2017 03 13 Rice Appeals to Congress on Darfur New York Times December 17 2005 Retrieved May 8 2023 Samu Sheena Milton Tom Brown Erica December 22 2022 Priest recalls George Santos cries of poverty saying family could not afford a funeral for his mother WCBS TV Archived from the original on December 23 2022 Retrieved December 24 2022 Paybarah Azi January 18 2023 Records show Rep George Santos s mother wasn t in New York on 9 11 The Washington Post Archived from the original on January 18 2023 Retrieved January 18 2023 Silverstein Andrew December 21 2022 Congressman elect George Santos lied about grandparents fleeing anti Jewish persecution during WWII The Forward Archived from the original on December 21 2022 Retrieved December 21 2022 Gino F Shu L L Bazerman M H 2010 Nameless harmless blameless When seemingly irrelevant factors influence judgment of un ethical behavior Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 111 2 93 101 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2009 11 001 a b c Kogut T Kogut E 2013 Exploring the relationship between adult attachment style and the identifiable victim effect in helping behavior PDF Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49 4 651 660 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2013 02 011 Mikulincer M Gillath O Halevy V Avihou N Avidan S et al 2001 Attachment theory and reactions to others needs Evidence that activation of the sense of attachment security promotes empathic responses Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 6 1205 1224 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 6 1205 PMID 11761318 a b Friedrich J McGuire A 2010 Individual differences in reasoning style as a moderator of the identifiable victim effect Social Influence 5 3 182 201 doi 10 1080 15534511003707352 S2CID 143401716 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Identifiable victim effect amp oldid 1200251399, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.