fbpx
Wikipedia

Phylogenetic autocorrelation

Phylogenetic autocorrelation also known as Galton's problem, after Sir Francis Galton who described it, is the problem of drawing inferences from cross-cultural data, due to the statistical phenomenon now called autocorrelation. The problem is now recognized as a general one that applies to all nonexperimental studies and to experimental design as well. It is most simply described as the problem of external dependencies in making statistical estimates when the elements sampled are not statistically independent. Asking two people in the same household whether they watch TV, for example, does not give you statistically independent answers. The sample size, n, for independent observations in this case is one, not two. Once proper adjustments are made that deal with external dependencies, then the axioms of probability theory concerning statistical independence will apply. These axioms are important for deriving measures of variance, for example, or tests of statistical significance.

Origin edit

In 1888, Galton was present when Sir Edward Tylor presented a paper at the Royal Anthropological Institute. Tylor had compiled information on institutions of marriage and descent for 350 cultures and examined the associations between these institutions and measures of societal complexity. Tylor interpreted his results as indications of a general evolutionary sequence, in which institutions change focus from the maternal line to the paternal line as societies become increasingly complex. Galton disagreed, pointing out that similarity between cultures could be due to borrowing, could be due to common descent, or could be due to evolutionary development; he maintained that without controlling for borrowing and common descent one cannot make valid inferences regarding evolutionary development. Galton's critique has become the eponymous Galton's Problem,[1]: 175  as named by Raoul Naroll,[2][3] who proposed the first statistical solutions.

By the early 20th century unilineal evolutionism was abandoned and along with it the drawing of direct inferences from correlations to evolutionary sequences. Galton's criticisms proved equally valid, however, for inferring functional relations from correlations. The problem of autocorrelation remained.

Solutions edit

Statistician William S. Gosset in 1914 developed methods of eliminating spurious correlation due to how position in time or space affects similarities. Today's election polls have a similar problem: the closer the poll to the election, the less individuals make up their mind independently, and the greater the unreliability of the polling results, especially the margin of error or confidence limits. The effective n of independent cases from their sample drops as the election nears. Statistical significance falls with lower effective sample size.

The problem pops up in sample surveys when sociologists want to reduce the travel time to do their interviews, and hence they divide their population into local clusters and sample the clusters randomly, then sample again within the clusters. If they interview n people in clusters of size m the effective sample size (efs) would have a lower limit of 1 + (n − 1) / m if everyone in each cluster were identical. When there are only partial similarities within clusters, the m in this formula has to be lowered accordingly. A formula of this sort is 1 + d (n − 1) where d is the intraclass correlation for the statistic in question.[4] In general, estimation of the appropriate efs depends on the statistic estimated, as for example, mean, chi-square, correlation, regression coefficient, and their variances.

For cross-cultural studies, Murdock and White[5] estimated the size of patches of similarities in their sample of 186 societies. The four variables they tested – language, economy, political integration, and descent – had patches of similarities that varied from size three to size ten. A very crude rule of thumb might be to divide the square root of the similarity-patch sizes into n, so that the effective sample sizes are 58 and 107 for these patches, respectively. Again, statistical significance falls with lower effective sample size.

In modern analysis spatial lags have been modelled in order to estimate the degree of globalization on modern societies.[6]

Spatial dependency or auto-correlation is a fundamental concept in geography. Methods developed by geographers that measure and control for spatial autocorrelation[7][8] do far more than reduce the effective n for tests of significance of a correlation. One example is the complicated hypothesis that "the presence of gambling in a society is directly proportional to the presence of a commercial money and to the presence of considerable socioeconomic differences and is inversely related to whether or not the society is a nomadic herding society." [9] Tests of this hypothesis in a sample of 60 societies failed to reject the null hypothesis. Autocorrelation analysis, however, showed a significant effect of socioeconomic differences.[10]

How prevalent is autocorrelation among the variables studied in cross-cultural research? A test by Anthon Eff on 1700 variables in the cumulative database for the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample, published in , measured Moran's I for spatial autocorrelation (distance), linguistic autocorrelation (common descent), and autocorrelation in cultural complexity (mainline evolution). "The results suggest that ... it would be prudent to test for spatial and phylogenetic autoccorrelation when conducting regression analyses with the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample."[11] The use of autocorrelation tests in exploratory data analysis is illustrated, showing how all variables in a given study can be evaluated for nonindependence of cases in terms of distance, language, and cultural complexity. The methods for estimating these autocorrelation effects are then explained and illustrated for ordinary least squares regression using again the Moran I significance measure of autocorrelation.

When autocorrelation is present, it can often be removed to get unbiased estimates of regression coefficients and their variances by constructing a respecified dependent variable that is "lagged" by weightings on the dependent variable on other locations, where the weights are degree of relationship. This lagged dependent variable is endogenous, and estimation requires either two-stage least squares or maximum likelihood methods.[12]

Resources edit

A public server, if used externally at http://SocSciCompute.ss.uci.edu 2016-02-20 at the Wayback Machine, offers ethnographic data, variables and tools for inference with R scripts by Dow (2007) and Eff and Dow (2009) in an NSF supported Galaxy (http://getgalaxy.org) framework (https://www.xsede.org) for instructors, students and researchers to do "CoSSci Galaxy" cross-cultural research modeling 2016-02-20 at the Wayback Machine with controls for Galton's problem using Standard Cross-Cultural Sample variables at .

Opportunities edit

In anthropology, where Tylor's problem was first recognized by the statistician Galton in 1889, it is still not widely recognized that there are standard statistical adjustments for the problem of patches of similarity in observed cases and opportunities for new discoveries using autocorrelation methods. Some cross-cultural researchers (see, e.g., Korotayev and de Munck 2003)[13] have begun to realize that evidence of diffusion, historical origin, and other sources of similarity among related societies or individuals should be renamed Galton's Opportunity and Galton's Asset rather than Galton's Problem. Researchers now use longitudinal, cross-cultural, and regional variation analysis routinely to analyze all the competing hypotheses: functional relationships, diffusion, common historical origin, multilineal evolution, co-adaptation with environment, and complex social interaction dynamics.[14]

Controversies edit

Within anthropology, the problem of phylogenetic auocorrelation is often given as a cause to reject comparative studies altogether. Since the problem is a general one, common to the sciences and statistical inference generally, this particular criticism of cross-cultural or comparative studies – and there are many – is one that, logically speaking, amounts to a rejection of science and statistics altogether. Any data collected and analyzed by ethnographers, for example, is equally subject to autocorrelation, understood in its most general sense. A critique of the anticomparative critique is not limited to statistical comparison since it would apply as well to the analysis of text. That is, the analysis and use of text in argumentation is subject to critique as to the evidential basis of inference. Reliance purely on rhetoric is no protection against critique as to the validity of argument and its evidentiary basis.

There is little doubt, however, that the community of cross-cultural researchers have been remiss in ignoring autocorrelation. Expert investigation of this question shows results that "strongly suggest that the extensive reporting of naïve chi-square independence tests using cross-cultural data sets over the past several decades has led to incorrect rejection of null hypotheses at levels much higher than the expected 5% rate."[15]: 247  The investigator concludes that "Incorrect theories that have been 'saved' by naïve chi-square tests with comparative data may yet be more rigorously tested another day."[15]: 270  Once again, the adjusted variance of a cluster sample is given as one multiplied by 1 + d (k + 1) where k is the average size of a cluster, and a more complicated correction is given for the variance of contingency table correlations with r rows and c columns. Since this critique was published in 1993, and others like it, more authors have begun to adopt corrections for Galton's problem, but the majority in the cross-cultural field have not. Consequently, a large proportion of published results that rely on naive significance tests and that adopt the P < 0.05 rather than a P < 0.005 standard are likely to be in error because they are more susceptible to type I error, which is to reject the null hypothesis when it is true.

Some cross-cultural researchers reject the seriousness of the problem of autocorrelation because, they argue, estimates of correlations and means may be unbiased even if autocorrelation, weak or strong, is present. Without investigating autocorrelation, however, they may still mis-estimate statistics dealing with relationships among variables. In regression analysis, for example, examining the patterns of autocorrelated residuals may give important clues to third factors that may affect the relationships among variables but that have not been included in the regression model. Second, if there are clusters of similar and related societies in the sample, measures of variance will be underestimated, leading to spurious statistical conclusions. for example, exaggerating the statistical significance of correlations. Third, the underestimation of variance makes it difficult to test for replication of results from two different samples, as the results will more often be rejected as similar.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Stocking, George W. Jr. (1968). "Edward Burnett Tylor." International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. David L. Sills, editor, New York, Mcmillan Company: v.16, pp. 170–177.
  2. ^ Raoul Naroll (1961). "Two solutions to Galton's Problem". Philosophy of Science. 28: 15–29. doi:10.1086/287778. S2CID 121671403.
  3. ^ Raoul Naroll (1965). "Galton's problem: The logic of cross cultural research". Social Research. 32: 428–451.
  4. ^ (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2006-04-14. Retrieved 2006-11-01.
  5. ^ George P. Murdock and Douglas R. White (1969). "Standard cross-cultural sample". Ethnology. 9: 329–369.
  6. ^ Jahn, Detlef (2006). "Globalization as Galton's Problem: The Missing Link in the Analysis of the Diffusion Patterns in Welfare State Development" (PDF). International Organization. 60 (2): 401–431. doi:10.1017/s0020818306060127. S2CID 154976704. abstract
  7. ^ Cliff, A.D., and J.K. Ord. 1973. Spatial Autocorrelation. London: Pion Press.
  8. ^ Cliff, A.D. and J.K. Ord. 1981. Spatial Processes. London: Pion Press.
  9. ^ Pryor, Frederick (1976). "The Diffusion Possibility Method: A More General and Simpler Solution to Galton's Problem". American Ethnologist. 3 (4). American Anthropological Association: 731–749. doi:10.1525/ae.1976.3.4.02a00100.
  10. ^ Malcolm M. Dow, Michael L. Burton, Douglas R. White, and Karl P. Reitz (1984). "Galton's problem as network autocorrelation". American Ethnologist. 11 (4): 754–770. doi:10.1525/ae.1984.11.4.02a00080. S2CID 143111431.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ E. Anthon Eff (2004). "Does Mr. Galton still have a Problem? Autocorrelation in the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample" (PDF). World Cultures. 15 (2): 153–170.
  12. ^ Anselin, Luc. 1988. Spatial Econometrics: Methods and Models. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  13. ^ Andrey Korotayev and Victor de Munck (2003). "Galton's Asset and Flower's Problem: Cultural Networks and Cultural Units in Cross-Cultural Research". American Anthropologist. 105 (2): 353–358. doi:10.1525/aa.2003.105.2.353.
  14. ^ Mace, Ruth; Pagel, Mark (1994). "The Comparative Method in Anthropology". Current Anthropology. 35 (5): 549–564. doi:10.1086/204317. S2CID 146297584.
  15. ^ a b Malcolm M. Dow (1993). "Saving the theory: on chi-square tests with cross-cultural survey data". Cross-Cultural Research. 27 (3–4): 247–276. doi:10.1177/106939719302700305. S2CID 122509821.

Further reading edit

  • Dow, M. M. (2007). "Galton's Problem as multiple network autocorrelation effects" (PDF). Cross-Cultural Research. 41 (4): 336–363. doi:10.1177/1069397107305452. S2CID 143230639.
  • Eff, E. Anthon and Malcolm M. Dow. 2009. "How to Deal with Missing Data and Galton's Problem in Cross-Cultural Survey Research: A Primer for R." Structure and Dynamics: eJournal of Anthropological and Related Sciences 3(3):223–252. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7cm1f10b
  • Oztan, B. Tolga. 2016. Evolution of Cooperation: Comparative Study of Kinship Behavior. PhD Thesis, UC Irvine. Mathematical Behavioral Sciences. http://intersci.ss.uci.edu/wiki/pdf/latest/thesisJan2Tolga2015.pdf (extensive treatment of Dow–Eff solution to Galton's problem).
  • IntersciWiki. 2007. Using Autocorrelation in model specification (including software and tutorial)
  • IntersciWiki. 2009. Galton's problem and Autocorrelation (bibliography)
  • Student (W. S. Gosset) (1914). "The elimination of spurious correlation due to position in time or space". Biometrika. 10 (1): 179–181. doi:10.2307/2331746. JSTOR 2331746.
  • Tylor, Edward E. (1889). "On a Method of Investigating the Development of Institutions Applied to the Laws of Marriage and Descent". Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute. 18 (3): 245–72. doi:10.2307/2842423. hdl:2027/hvd.32044097779680. JSTOR 2842423.
  • Witkowski, Stanley (1974). "Galton's opportunity – hologeistic study of historical processes". Behavior Science Research. 9 (1): 11–15. doi:10.1177/106939717400900105. S2CID 144398651.

phylogenetic, autocorrelation, also, known, galton, problem, after, francis, galton, described, problem, drawing, inferences, from, cross, cultural, data, statistical, phenomenon, called, autocorrelation, problem, recognized, general, that, applies, nonexperim. Phylogenetic autocorrelation also known as Galton s problem after Sir Francis Galton who described it is the problem of drawing inferences from cross cultural data due to the statistical phenomenon now called autocorrelation The problem is now recognized as a general one that applies to all nonexperimental studies and to experimental design as well It is most simply described as the problem of external dependencies in making statistical estimates when the elements sampled are not statistically independent Asking two people in the same household whether they watch TV for example does not give you statistically independent answers The sample size n for independent observations in this case is one not two Once proper adjustments are made that deal with external dependencies then the axioms of probability theory concerning statistical independence will apply These axioms are important for deriving measures of variance for example or tests of statistical significance Contents 1 Origin 2 Solutions 3 Resources 4 Opportunities 5 Controversies 6 See also 7 References 8 Further readingOrigin editIn 1888 Galton was present when Sir Edward Tylor presented a paper at the Royal Anthropological Institute Tylor had compiled information on institutions of marriage and descent for 350 cultures and examined the associations between these institutions and measures of societal complexity Tylor interpreted his results as indications of a general evolutionary sequence in which institutions change focus from the maternal line to the paternal line as societies become increasingly complex Galton disagreed pointing out that similarity between cultures could be due to borrowing could be due to common descent or could be due to evolutionary development he maintained that without controlling for borrowing and common descent one cannot make valid inferences regarding evolutionary development Galton s critique has become the eponymous Galton s Problem 1 175 as named by Raoul Naroll 2 3 who proposed the first statistical solutions By the early 20th century unilineal evolutionism was abandoned and along with it the drawing of direct inferences from correlations to evolutionary sequences Galton s criticisms proved equally valid however for inferring functional relations from correlations The problem of autocorrelation remained Solutions editStatistician William S Gosset in 1914 developed methods of eliminating spurious correlation due to how position in time or space affects similarities Today s election polls have a similar problem the closer the poll to the election the less individuals make up their mind independently and the greater the unreliability of the polling results especially the margin of error or confidence limits The effective n of independent cases from their sample drops as the election nears Statistical significance falls with lower effective sample size The problem pops up in sample surveys when sociologists want to reduce the travel time to do their interviews and hence they divide their population into local clusters and sample the clusters randomly then sample again within the clusters If they interview n people in clusters of size m the effective sample size efs would have a lower limit of 1 n 1 m if everyone in each cluster were identical When there are only partial similarities within clusters the m in this formula has to be lowered accordingly A formula of this sort is 1 d n 1 where d is the intraclass correlation for the statistic in question 4 In general estimation of the appropriate efs depends on the statistic estimated as for example mean chi square correlation regression coefficient and their variances For cross cultural studies Murdock and White 5 estimated the size of patches of similarities in their sample of 186 societies The four variables they tested language economy political integration and descent had patches of similarities that varied from size three to size ten A very crude rule of thumb might be to divide the square root of the similarity patch sizes into n so that the effective sample sizes are 58 and 107 for these patches respectively Again statistical significance falls with lower effective sample size In modern analysis spatial lags have been modelled in order to estimate the degree of globalization on modern societies 6 Spatial dependency or auto correlation is a fundamental concept in geography Methods developed by geographers that measure and control for spatial autocorrelation 7 8 do far more than reduce the effective n for tests of significance of a correlation One example is the complicated hypothesis that the presence of gambling in a society is directly proportional to the presence of a commercial money and to the presence of considerable socioeconomic differences and is inversely related to whether or not the society is a nomadic herding society 9 Tests of this hypothesis in a sample of 60 societies failed to reject the null hypothesis Autocorrelation analysis however showed a significant effect of socioeconomic differences 10 How prevalent is autocorrelation among the variables studied in cross cultural research A test by Anthon Eff on 1700 variables in the cumulative database for the Standard Cross Cultural Sample published in World Cultures measured Moran s I for spatial autocorrelation distance linguistic autocorrelation common descent and autocorrelation in cultural complexity mainline evolution The results suggest that it would be prudent to test for spatial and phylogenetic autoccorrelation when conducting regression analyses with the Standard Cross Cultural Sample 11 The use of autocorrelation tests in exploratory data analysis is illustrated showing how all variables in a given study can be evaluated for nonindependence of cases in terms of distance language and cultural complexity The methods for estimating these autocorrelation effects are then explained and illustrated for ordinary least squares regression using again the Moran I significance measure of autocorrelation When autocorrelation is present it can often be removed to get unbiased estimates of regression coefficients and their variances by constructing a respecified dependent variable that is lagged by weightings on the dependent variable on other locations where the weights are degree of relationship This lagged dependent variable is endogenous and estimation requires either two stage least squares or maximum likelihood methods 12 Resources editA public server if used externally at http SocSciCompute ss uci edu Archived 2016 02 20 at the Wayback Machine offers ethnographic data variables and tools for inference with R scripts by Dow 2007 and Eff and Dow 2009 in an NSF supported Galaxy http getgalaxy org framework https www xsede org for instructors students and researchers to do CoSSci Galaxy cross cultural research modeling Archived 2016 02 20 at the Wayback Machine with controls for Galton s problem using Standard Cross Cultural Sample variables at https web archive org web 20160402201432 https dl dropboxusercontent com u 9256203 SCCScodebook txt Opportunities editIn anthropology where Tylor s problem was first recognized by the statistician Galton in 1889 it is still not widely recognized that there are standard statistical adjustments for the problem of patches of similarity in observed cases and opportunities for new discoveries using autocorrelation methods Some cross cultural researchers see e g Korotayev and de Munck 2003 13 have begun to realize that evidence of diffusion historical origin and other sources of similarity among related societies or individuals should be renamed Galton s Opportunity and Galton s Asset rather than Galton s Problem Researchers now use longitudinal cross cultural and regional variation analysis routinely to analyze all the competing hypotheses functional relationships diffusion common historical origin multilineal evolution co adaptation with environment and complex social interaction dynamics 14 Controversies editThe neutrality of this article is disputed Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met November 2017 Learn how and when to remove this message Within anthropology the problem of phylogenetic auocorrelation is often given as a cause to reject comparative studies altogether Since the problem is a general one common to the sciences and statistical inference generally this particular criticism of cross cultural or comparative studies and there are many is one that logically speaking amounts to a rejection of science and statistics altogether Any data collected and analyzed by ethnographers for example is equally subject to autocorrelation understood in its most general sense A critique of the anticomparative critique is not limited to statistical comparison since it would apply as well to the analysis of text That is the analysis and use of text in argumentation is subject to critique as to the evidential basis of inference Reliance purely on rhetoric is no protection against critique as to the validity of argument and its evidentiary basis There is little doubt however that the community of cross cultural researchers have been remiss in ignoring autocorrelation Expert investigation of this question shows results that strongly suggest that the extensive reporting of naive chi square independence tests using cross cultural data sets over the past several decades has led to incorrect rejection of null hypotheses at levels much higher than the expected 5 rate 15 247 The investigator concludes that Incorrect theories that have been saved by naive chi square tests with comparative data may yet be more rigorously tested another day 15 270 Once again the adjusted variance of a cluster sample is given as one multiplied by 1 d k 1 where k is the average size of a cluster and a more complicated correction is given for the variance of contingency table correlations with r rows and c columns Since this critique was published in 1993 and others like it more authors have begun to adopt corrections for Galton s problem but the majority in the cross cultural field have not Consequently a large proportion of published results that rely on naive significance tests and that adopt the P lt 0 05 rather than a P lt 0 005 standard are likely to be in error because they are more susceptible to type I error which is to reject the null hypothesis when it is true Some cross cultural researchers reject the seriousness of the problem of autocorrelation because they argue estimates of correlations and means may be unbiased even if autocorrelation weak or strong is present Without investigating autocorrelation however they may still mis estimate statistics dealing with relationships among variables In regression analysis for example examining the patterns of autocorrelated residuals may give important clues to third factors that may affect the relationships among variables but that have not been included in the regression model Second if there are clusters of similar and related societies in the sample measures of variance will be underestimated leading to spurious statistical conclusions for example exaggerating the statistical significance of correlations Third the underestimation of variance makes it difficult to test for replication of results from two different samples as the results will more often be rejected as similar See also editList of cultures in the standard cross cultural sampleReferences edit Stocking George W Jr 1968 Edward Burnett Tylor International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences David L Sills editor New York Mcmillan Company v 16 pp 170 177 Raoul Naroll 1961 Two solutions to Galton s Problem Philosophy of Science 28 15 29 doi 10 1086 287778 S2CID 121671403 Raoul Naroll 1965 Galton s problem The logic of cross cultural research Social Research 32 428 451 Sample Size and Design Effect PDF Archived from the original PDF on 2006 04 14 Retrieved 2006 11 01 George P Murdock and Douglas R White 1969 Standard cross cultural sample Ethnology 9 329 369 Jahn Detlef 2006 Globalization as Galton s Problem The Missing Link in the Analysis of the Diffusion Patterns in Welfare State Development PDF International Organization 60 2 401 431 doi 10 1017 s0020818306060127 S2CID 154976704 abstract Cliff A D and J K Ord 1973 Spatial Autocorrelation London Pion Press Cliff A D and J K Ord 1981 Spatial Processes London Pion Press Pryor Frederick 1976 The Diffusion Possibility Method A More General and Simpler Solution to Galton s Problem American Ethnologist 3 4 American Anthropological Association 731 749 doi 10 1525 ae 1976 3 4 02a00100 Malcolm M Dow Michael L Burton Douglas R White and Karl P Reitz 1984 Galton s problem as network autocorrelation American Ethnologist 11 4 754 770 doi 10 1525 ae 1984 11 4 02a00080 S2CID 143111431 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link E Anthon Eff 2004 Does Mr Galton still have a Problem Autocorrelation in the Standard Cross Cultural Sample PDF World Cultures 15 2 153 170 Anselin Luc 1988 Spatial Econometrics Methods and Models Dordrecht Kluwer Academic Publishers Andrey Korotayev and Victor de Munck 2003 Galton s Asset and Flower s Problem Cultural Networks and Cultural Units in Cross Cultural Research American Anthropologist 105 2 353 358 doi 10 1525 aa 2003 105 2 353 Mace Ruth Pagel Mark 1994 The Comparative Method in Anthropology Current Anthropology 35 5 549 564 doi 10 1086 204317 S2CID 146297584 a b Malcolm M Dow 1993 Saving the theory on chi square tests with cross cultural survey data Cross Cultural Research 27 3 4 247 276 doi 10 1177 106939719302700305 S2CID 122509821 Further reading editDow M M 2007 Galton s Problem as multiple network autocorrelation effects PDF Cross Cultural Research 41 4 336 363 doi 10 1177 1069397107305452 S2CID 143230639 Eff E Anthon and Malcolm M Dow 2009 How to Deal with Missing Data and Galton s Problem in Cross Cultural Survey Research A Primer for R Structure and Dynamics eJournal of Anthropological and Related Sciences 3 3 223 252 https escholarship org uc item 7cm1f10b Oztan B Tolga 2016 Evolution of Cooperation Comparative Study of Kinship Behavior PhD Thesis UC Irvine Mathematical Behavioral Sciences http intersci ss uci edu wiki pdf latest thesisJan2Tolga2015 pdf extensive treatment of Dow Eff solution to Galton s problem IntersciWiki 2007 Using Autocorrelation in model specification including software and tutorial IntersciWiki 2009 Galton s problem and Autocorrelation bibliography Student W S Gosset 1914 The elimination of spurious correlation due to position in time or space Biometrika 10 1 179 181 doi 10 2307 2331746 JSTOR 2331746 Tylor Edward E 1889 On a Method of Investigating the Development of Institutions Applied to the Laws of Marriage and Descent Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 18 3 245 72 doi 10 2307 2842423 hdl 2027 hvd 32044097779680 JSTOR 2842423 Witkowski Stanley 1974 Galton s opportunity hologeistic study of historical processes Behavior Science Research 9 1 11 15 doi 10 1177 106939717400900105 S2CID 144398651 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Phylogenetic autocorrelation amp oldid 1197498029, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.