fbpx
Wikipedia

Flight zone

The flight zone of an animal is the area surrounding an animal that if encroached upon by a potential predator or threat, including humans, will cause alarm and escape behavior. The flight zone is determined by the animal's flight distance, sometimes called[3] flight initiation distance (FID)[4] which extends horizontally from the animal and sometimes vertically. It may also be termed[citation needed] escape distance, alert distance, flush distance, and escape flight distance.

Flight initiation distance (FID) buffer from critical wildlife area.[1][2]

Swiss zoologist Heini Hediger distinguished between flight distance (run boundary), critical distance (attack boundary), personal distance (distance separating members of non-contact species, as a pair of swans), and social distance (intraspecies communication distance).

Flight distance can be used as a measure of the willingness of an animal to take risks. Escape theory predicts that the probability of fleeing and flight distance increase as predation risk increases and decrease as escape cost increases.[5] Flight initiation distance is one measure of animals' fear responses to humans.[6]

In a study comparing 56 bird species with long flight distances, it was found these had declining populations in Europe. This indicates that standardized measures of flight distance can provide reliable information about the population consequences of risk-taking behaviour by individuals and the susceptibility of different species to increased levels of disturbance by humans.[5] A further study analyzing 75 flight initiation distance studies of 212 species found that larger species are more tolerant of humans.[6]

When the flight zone of a group of bulls was invaded by a mechanical trolley, the bulls moved away and maintained a constant distance between themselves and the trolley.[7] This indicates animals sometimes maintain a flight zone around inanimate objects.

The flight initiation distance is being used as a tool in wildlife management.[8] By studying flight zones, wildlife managers are able to reduce the impact of humans by creating buffer zones between human populations and animal habitats.[8]

The alert distance (AD) is the distance, by definition greater, within which the animal changes its behaviour in a manner enabling it to better observe the stimulus, as by raising the head in an alert posture, but does not necessarily flee unless the stimulus is also within the escape distance.[9][10][11] These measures are usually used to quantify the tolerance of wildlife to humans.

Factors influencing size edit

Animals faced with approaching predators must decide at which distance to initiate their flight, and they are expected to do so in a way that maximizes their fitness. As flight has both costs (including lost opportunity) and benefits, there will be in general an optimal flight initiation distance, defined as the first point in a predator's approach at which the benefit of flight exceeds the cost. The benefit of flight is equivalent to the cost of remaining in place or, in other words, to the risk of capture. The size of the flight zone can therefore vary according to circumstances. However, it has been shown in burrowing owls that individuals showed high repeatability in their FID.[12]

  • Behaviour of the threat: In horned lizards, FID decreased as the distance between a turning predator and prey increased, but was greater when the predator turned toward than away from the fleeing animal.[13] The FID and alert response of American robins to approaching humans was investigated; the greatest FID was when the approaching person was not on paths and was looking at the birds, while the lowest FID occurred when the person was on a path and not looking at the robins. The authors suggested this indicated that they use gaze direction to assess risk.[14]
  • Social: In lizards, FID was shorter during social encounters than when they were solitary.[13] and FID was shorter in female lizards when they were interacting with males than when they were alone; it was also shorter in males interacting with either sex.[15]
  • Distance to refuge: Gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) typically run to the nearest tree to escape from predators. As the risk of capture increases with distance from the refuge tree, squirrels feeding far from trees should have greater FID than those feeding closer by. Confirming this, FID in response to a motorized model predator (a cat) increased as distance to refuge increased.[16] Burrowing Owls breeding in territories far from roads showed larger FIDs than individuals breeding closer to roads and mated owls showed similar FIDs. Individual owls showed high repeatability in their FID.[12]
  • Training and learning: The size of the flight zone can depend upon the tameness or level of habituation of the animal. Completely tame animals have no flight zone for humans; that is, they will allow a person to approach and touch them. Wild, feral, and unbroken animals can have very large flight zones.

Wildlife management edit

Wildlife managers often use ED and FID to develop set-back distances to reduce human impacts on wildlife,[17][18][1][4] both in wildlife refuges, and, e.g., in planning areas for outdoor recreation.[19]

These measures are also important in birding and nature photography.

The FID in multiple species differs from rural to urban areas.[20] A study by Møller et al. examined 811 FIDs from 37 species of birds and determined that the FID of birds in urban areas is reduced, compared to the FID of birds in rural areas.[20] Urbanization of birds has also been shown to correlate with changes in stress physiology and anti predator behaviour.[20] This may be due to a number of factors differing in rural vs urban areas, such as; difference in predator communities, length of exposure time to humans, relative abundance of humans, and the presence/abundance of food (bird-feeders in winter for example).[20] Wildlife managers must adjust buffer zones depending on urban/rural environments.

Some physical characteristics are very important to determine an animal's FID.[21] Eye size and brain size have a role in determining the FID.[21] FID in 107 species of birds was studied in relation to eye size and brain size and was shown that FID increases with larger eyes and decreases with larger brains.[21] Larger eyes mean that predators can be detected from further away and thus the FID would be larger compared to smaller eyes.[21] Larger brains decrease the FID compared to smaller brains, since they can better process the intent of predators and can delay their flight response for as long as possible.[21]

FID can be highly variable, but it can also be viewed as a species-specific trait.[8] A study conducted using eight species of shorebirds at six different sites in Australia was conducted to determine if FID was species specific.[8] It was demonstrated that while both the species and the site influenced the FID, there was no significant interaction between them.[8] This indicates that FID is species-specific, and while sites do influence the FID of a species, the average FID is a good reference for wildlife managers to use when creating buffer zones.[8]

While escape distance has been generally used as a measure of tolerance, other changes in animal behavior in presence of humans, such as increased vigilance time at the cost of decreased feeding time, may have significant overall impact on wildlife. Therefore, it is suggested that a more conservative measure, namely, the alert distance, should be used in determining minimum approaching distance.[19] The latter typically adds a certain buffer distance to the given tolerance measure.[4]

Animal handling edit

The flight zone is an important principle for herding, working, and mustering livestock. An animal can be stimulated to move simply by skirting its flight zone, and the animal will move in the desired direction according to the point of balance. The point of balance is usually located at the animal's shoulder according to their wide angled vision. An overstimulated animal will have a larger flight zone, for example an excited or scared animal.[22] A Cumulative Flight Zone is formed when animals move in a herd. In this situation the lead animal's and the following animals' Points of Balance, within the cumulative flight zone, must both be crossed to entice movement.[22]

The flight distance during handling is usually 1.5 to 7.6 m for beef cattle raised in a feeding operation and up to 30 m on mountain ranges.[23] Brahman cattle have a larger flight zone than most English breeds.[24] The flight zone can be thought of as the animal's personal space. The size of the flight zone is determined by the tameness of the animal; the more domesticated an animal, the smaller the zone. Fully tame animals have no flight zone.[25]

The flight zones in cattle vary depending on the situation they are experiencing.[26] Novel situations increase their flight zone, while accustomed stimuli will decrease their flight zone.[26] The flight zone is larger in the front than behind, due to the majority of their senses pointing forward.[26] As the animal becomes more relaxed in a situation or with a person its flight zone will reduce.[26] The cow's prior experiences with humans has also been shown to affect their flight zone.[26] Cows with positive handling experiences were shown to have smaller flight zones than those with negative handling experiences.[26]

Studies with sheep indicated that animals confined in a narrow alley had a smaller flight zone compared to animals confined in a wider alley.[27]

Handlers sometimes make the mistake of deeply invading the flight zone when animals are being driven down an alley or into an enclosed area such as a crowd pen. If the handler deeply penetrates the flight zone, the animals may turn back and run over them in an attempt to escape.[28] Confining a livestock animal in a crush (chute) or alley can make it feel more secure and thus reduce the size of the flight zone; however, it does not eliminate the flight zone. An animal in a livestock raceway or alley that feels threatened may panic and injure itself or other animals.[29] If handlers lean over fences around animals they penetrate the "zone of safety" and may cause the animals to rear.[30]

Husbandry edit

Animals have a tendency to move in the opposite direction when their handler walks deep into their flight zone. By crossing an animal's point of balance, within the flight zone, a handler can move the herd in a particular direction and control their speed of movement. For example, crossing the point of balance from front to back will move the animal forwards, while the opposite is also true. The handler's pace should always reflect the animal's speed when herding. Additionally, pressure should be alternated on the flight zone to reduce stress.[25] Constant pressure should never be applied.

If animals turn to face the handler he or she is considered to be no longer penetrating the flight zone.[25]

It is important that a handler does not pursue any struggling animals as this will cause undue stress. Instead the animal should be allowed to return to the group as animals naturally have herd instincts, and will follow the group's leader. A good herding practice applying this is the movement of animals through a race by maintaining a steady flow of animals, not herding in groups, this allows new animals to follow the leader calmly.[31] When yarded, animals should always have room to turn away from the handler to reduce stress. Minimal stress prevents injury to the animal and maintains good production, such as increased quality of meat and improved muscle and fat scores.[29]

Appropriate frequent penetration of the flight zone can train the animals to minimise their flight zone to the handler.[25]

Poor husbandry skills including over penetration of the flight zone results in the following behaviours: stress, panic, aggression, bolting, prey behaviour, charging, fainting, sickness and self-inflicted damage. Rough handling, such as constant flight zone pressure, can raise the heart rate of an animal. These factors are all reflective of the General Adaptation Syndrome.

General adaptation syndrome edit

The general adaptation syndrome (GAS) is a three-phase response to stress in animals.

  • The first phase is the fight or flight response – the animal flight zone is included in this. Over penetration of the animal flight zone causes stimulation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). The SNS produces localised adjustments and responses; this includes the excretion of large quantities of epinephrine from the medulla of the adrenal gland.[32] Epinephrine is commonly known as adrenaline. Adrenaline increases the supply of oxygen to vital organs and decreases supply to others.[33] Frequent subjection to fight or flight situations causes severe endocrine disorders.
  • The second phase is Adaptation and Resistance. It is the idea that recurring subjection builds natural immunity,[33] and common handler movement and herding minimises an animals flight zone.
  • The third phase is Exhaustion. Strong, constant and over frequent stimulation of an animals flight zone may lead to death, decreases production and lower quality of life. According to the GAS, a full recovery from exhaustion is possible over time.[33]

Sample values edit

Sample escape distances (mostly mean) from humans:

Birds of Europe

 
Because many birds flee from humans, ornithologists and birdwatchers sometimes use digiscoping equipment, allowing them to take pictures from long distances.
Species ED [m] Ref.
Brent goose (130–1000) 319 [10]
Northern pintail (100–500) 294 [10]
Grey heron 255 [11]
Mallard (60–400) 236 [10]
Northern lapwing 162 [11]
Dunlin (15–450) 70 [10]
Eurasian coot 68 [11]
Whinchat 20–30 [34]
Eurasian blackbird 10 [34]
Blue tit 10 [34]

Factors affecting escape distances for birds edit

 
Fleeing blackbird

Escape distance may differ significantly depending on many circumstances. Body size is the best known general factor influencing interspecies differences. Very frequently large species are more timid than small species, because size affects how rapidly a bird can take off.[11][10] Surprisingly, an analysis of hundreds of studies found that larger birds are more tolerant of humans, despite substantial research that shows larger animals are less tolerant of humans.[6]

Among the affecting factors:

Flight Zone in Lizards edit

It is shown that the Lizard Urosaurus ornatus where different colored males have differing FID values due to differences in hormonal levels.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Bentrup G. (2008). "Conservation buffers: design guidelines for buffers, corridors, and greenways". Gen. Tech. Rep. Asheville, NC: USDA, Forest Service, Southern Research Station. SRS-109.
  2. ^ Bentrup, G. (2008). "Flight Initiation Distance Buffers". USDA National Agroforestry Center. Archived from the original on 12 December 2012. Retrieved 6 September 2012.
  3. ^ Grandin, Temple; Deesing, Mark (2014). Genetics and Behavior During Handling, Restraint, and Herding. Elsevier Inc. p. 121.
  4. ^ a b c d e Bentrup, G. (2008). "Flight Initiation Distance Buffers". USDA National Agroforestry Center. Archived from the original on 12 December 2012. Retrieved 6 September 2012.
  5. ^ a b Moller, A.P. (2008). "Flight distance and population trends in European breeding birds". Behavioral Ecology. 19 (6): 1095–1102. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn103.
  6. ^ a b c Stuart Wolpert (16 November 2015). "Why are some wild animals more tolerant to human interaction than others?". UCLA. Retrieved 30 December 2016.
  7. ^ Kilgour, R., (1971). Animal handling in works, pertinent behaviour studies. 13th Meat Industry Research Conference, Hamilton, New Zealand. pp. 9–12
  8. ^ a b c d e f Blumstein, D. T.; Anthony, L. L.; Harcourt, R.; Ross, G. (2003). "Testing a key assumption of wildlife buffer zones: is flight initiation distance a species-specific trait?". Biological Conservation. 110 (1): 97–100. doi:10.1016/s0006-3207(02)00180-5.
  9. ^ a b Ruddock M., Whitfield D. P. (2007). (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on February 28, 2013. Retrieved September 4, 2012.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Laursen K.; Kahlert J.; Frikke; J. (2005). "Factors affecting escape distances of staging waterbirds" (PDF). Wildlife Biology. 11 (1): 13–19. doi:10.2981/0909-6396(2005)11[13:faedos]2.0.co;2. S2CID 86208796. Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Bregnballe T.; Aaen K.; Fox A. D. (2009). "Escape distances from human pedestrians by staging waterbirds in a Danish wetland" (PDF). Wildfowl (Special Issue 2): 115–130. Retrieved 4 September 2012.[permanent dead link]
  12. ^ a b Carrete, M.; Tella, J.L. (2009). "Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls: a new hypothesis on disturbance-induced habitat selection". Biology Letters. 6 (2): 167–170. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0739. PMC 2865052. PMID 19864278.
  13. ^ a b Cooper, W.E. (2000). "Plesiomorphic escape decisions in cryptic horned lizards (Phrynosoma) having highly derived antipredatory defenses". Retrieved 20 April 2013.
  14. ^ Eason, P.K.; Sherman, P.T; Rankin, O.; Coleman, B. (2006). "Factors affecting flight initiation distance in American robin". The Journal of Wildlife Management. 70 (6): 1796–1800. doi:10.2193/0022-541x(2006)70[1796:fafidi]2.0.co;2. S2CID 85926285.
  15. ^ Cooper, W.E. (2009). "Flight initiation distance decreases during social activity in lizards (Sceloporus virgatus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 63 (12): 1765–1771. doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0799-1. S2CID 24549272.
  16. ^ Dill, L.M.; Houtman, R. (1989). "The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)" (PDF). Canadian Journal of Zoology. 67: 233–235. doi:10.1139/z89-033.
  17. ^ a b Fernandez-Juricic E.; Jimenez M. D.; Lucas E. (2001). "Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance- implications for park design" (PDF). Environmental Conservation. 28 (3): 263–269. doi:10.1017/S0376892901000273. S2CID 44189097. Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  18. ^ a b Blumstein D. T. (Oct 2003). "Flight-Initiation Distance in Birds Is Dependent on Intruder Starting Distance" (PDF). The Journal of Wildlife Management. Allen Press. 67 (4): 852–857. doi:10.2307/3802692. JSTOR 3802692. Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  19. ^ a b Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance: implications for park design
  20. ^ a b c d Møller, A. P.; Tryjanowski, P; Díaz, M; Kwieciński, Z; Indykiewicz, P; Mitrus, C; Golawski, A; Polakowski, M (2015). "Urban habitats and feeders both contribute to flight initiation distance reduction in birds". Behavioral Ecology. 26 (3): 861–865. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv024.
  21. ^ a b c d e Møller, A. P.; Erritzøe, J. (2013). "Predator-prey interactions, flight initiation distance and brain size". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 26 (1): 23–42. doi:10.1111/jeb.12272. PMID 25990564. S2CID 41897304.
  22. ^ a b Grandin, Temple. "Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling". Vision, Hearing, and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs. American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  23. ^ Grandin, T (1980). "Observations of cattle behavior applied to the design of cattle-handling facilities". Appl. Anim. Ethol. 6: 19–31. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(80)90091-7.
  24. ^ Grandin, T (1978). "Observations of the spatial relationships between people and cattle during handling". Proc. Western Sect., Am. Soc. Anim. Sci. 29: 76–79.
  25. ^ a b c d Grandin, Temple. "Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling". Vision, Hearing, and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs. American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  26. ^ a b c d e f Moran, J; Doyle, R (2015). Cow talk: understanding dairy cow behaviour to improve their welfare on Asian farms. Clayton South, Vic: CSIRO Publishing. pp. 48–49.
  27. ^ Hutson, G.D. (1982). "Flight distance in Merino sheep". Animal Production. 35 (2): 231–235. doi:10.1017/s0003356100027409.
  28. ^ Grandin T. (1989). "Behavioral principles of livestock handling". Retrieved 22 April 2013.
  29. ^ a b Chambers, P.G.; Grandin, T.; Heinz, G.; Srisuvan, T. (2001). "Guidelines for Humane Handling, Transport and Slaughter of Livestock". Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations – Regional Office for Asia and the Pacific. Retrieved 22 April 2013.
  30. ^ Grandin, T. (1983). Handling and processing feedlot cattle. In: G.B. Thompson and C.C. O'Mary (Eds) The Feedlot, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. pp. 213–235
  31. ^ Grandin, Temple. "Understanding Flight Zones and Point of Balance". ProWay. ProWay Livestock Equipment. Retrieved 30 September 2020.
  32. ^ Loewy, Arthur. "Human Nervous System". Britannica. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  33. ^ a b c Unknown, Unknown. . Oracle Education Foundation. Archived from the original on 19 October 2013. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  34. ^ a b c d e f g h Gotzman J.; Desselberger J. (1979). Z lornetką wśród ptaków [With binoculars among birds]. Nasze hobby [Our hobby] (in Polish) (1st ed.). Warsaw: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Rolnicze i Leśne (PWRiL).

Further reading edit

  • Cooper, W.E.; Frederick, W.G. (2007). "Optimal flight initiation distance". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 244 (1): 59–67. Bibcode:2007JThBi.244...59C. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.07.011. PMID 16949619.

flight, zone, flight, distance, redirects, here, aircraft, flight, distance, flight, length, flight, zone, animal, area, surrounding, animal, that, encroached, upon, potential, predator, threat, including, humans, will, cause, alarm, escape, behavior, flight, . Flight distance redirects here For aircraft flight distance see flight length The flight zone of an animal is the area surrounding an animal that if encroached upon by a potential predator or threat including humans will cause alarm and escape behavior The flight zone is determined by the animal s flight distance sometimes called 3 flight initiation distance FID 4 which extends horizontally from the animal and sometimes vertically It may also be termed citation needed escape distance alert distance flush distance and escape flight distance Flight initiation distance FID buffer from critical wildlife area 1 2 Swiss zoologist Heini Hediger distinguished between flight distance run boundary critical distance attack boundary personal distance distance separating members of non contact species as a pair of swans and social distance intraspecies communication distance Flight distance can be used as a measure of the willingness of an animal to take risks Escape theory predicts that the probability of fleeing and flight distance increase as predation risk increases and decrease as escape cost increases 5 Flight initiation distance is one measure of animals fear responses to humans 6 In a study comparing 56 bird species with long flight distances it was found these had declining populations in Europe This indicates that standardized measures of flight distance can provide reliable information about the population consequences of risk taking behaviour by individuals and the susceptibility of different species to increased levels of disturbance by humans 5 A further study analyzing 75 flight initiation distance studies of 212 species found that larger species are more tolerant of humans 6 When the flight zone of a group of bulls was invaded by a mechanical trolley the bulls moved away and maintained a constant distance between themselves and the trolley 7 This indicates animals sometimes maintain a flight zone around inanimate objects The flight initiation distance is being used as a tool in wildlife management 8 By studying flight zones wildlife managers are able to reduce the impact of humans by creating buffer zones between human populations and animal habitats 8 The alert distance AD is the distance by definition greater within which the animal changes its behaviour in a manner enabling it to better observe the stimulus as by raising the head in an alert posture but does not necessarily flee unless the stimulus is also within the escape distance 9 10 11 These measures are usually used to quantify the tolerance of wildlife to humans Contents 1 Factors influencing size 2 Wildlife management 3 Animal handling 3 1 Husbandry 3 2 General adaptation syndrome 4 Sample values 5 Factors affecting escape distances for birds 6 Flight Zone in Lizards 7 See also 8 References 9 Further readingFactors influencing size editAnimals faced with approaching predators must decide at which distance to initiate their flight and they are expected to do so in a way that maximizes their fitness As flight has both costs including lost opportunity and benefits there will be in general an optimal flight initiation distance defined as the first point in a predator s approach at which the benefit of flight exceeds the cost The benefit of flight is equivalent to the cost of remaining in place or in other words to the risk of capture The size of the flight zone can therefore vary according to circumstances However it has been shown in burrowing owls that individuals showed high repeatability in their FID 12 Behaviour of the threat In horned lizards FID decreased as the distance between a turning predator and prey increased but was greater when the predator turned toward than away from the fleeing animal 13 The FID and alert response of American robins to approaching humans was investigated the greatest FID was when the approaching person was not on paths and was looking at the birds while the lowest FID occurred when the person was on a path and not looking at the robins The authors suggested this indicated that they use gaze direction to assess risk 14 Social In lizards FID was shorter during social encounters than when they were solitary 13 and FID was shorter in female lizards when they were interacting with males than when they were alone it was also shorter in males interacting with either sex 15 Distance to refuge Gray squirrels Sciurus carolinensis typically run to the nearest tree to escape from predators As the risk of capture increases with distance from the refuge tree squirrels feeding far from trees should have greater FID than those feeding closer by Confirming this FID in response to a motorized model predator a cat increased as distance to refuge increased 16 Burrowing Owls breeding in territories far from roads showed larger FIDs than individuals breeding closer to roads and mated owls showed similar FIDs Individual owls showed high repeatability in their FID 12 Training and learning The size of the flight zone can depend upon the tameness or level of habituation of the animal Completely tame animals have no flight zone for humans that is they will allow a person to approach and touch them Wild feral and unbroken animals can have very large flight zones Wildlife management editWildlife managers often use ED and FID to develop set back distances to reduce human impacts on wildlife 17 18 1 4 both in wildlife refuges and e g in planning areas for outdoor recreation 19 These measures are also important in birding and nature photography The FID in multiple species differs from rural to urban areas 20 A study by Moller et al examined 811 FIDs from 37 species of birds and determined that the FID of birds in urban areas is reduced compared to the FID of birds in rural areas 20 Urbanization of birds has also been shown to correlate with changes in stress physiology and anti predator behaviour 20 This may be due to a number of factors differing in rural vs urban areas such as difference in predator communities length of exposure time to humans relative abundance of humans and the presence abundance of food bird feeders in winter for example 20 Wildlife managers must adjust buffer zones depending on urban rural environments Some physical characteristics are very important to determine an animal s FID 21 Eye size and brain size have a role in determining the FID 21 FID in 107 species of birds was studied in relation to eye size and brain size and was shown that FID increases with larger eyes and decreases with larger brains 21 Larger eyes mean that predators can be detected from further away and thus the FID would be larger compared to smaller eyes 21 Larger brains decrease the FID compared to smaller brains since they can better process the intent of predators and can delay their flight response for as long as possible 21 FID can be highly variable but it can also be viewed as a species specific trait 8 A study conducted using eight species of shorebirds at six different sites in Australia was conducted to determine if FID was species specific 8 It was demonstrated that while both the species and the site influenced the FID there was no significant interaction between them 8 This indicates that FID is species specific and while sites do influence the FID of a species the average FID is a good reference for wildlife managers to use when creating buffer zones 8 While escape distance has been generally used as a measure of tolerance other changes in animal behavior in presence of humans such as increased vigilance time at the cost of decreased feeding time may have significant overall impact on wildlife Therefore it is suggested that a more conservative measure namely the alert distance should be used in determining minimum approaching distance 19 The latter typically adds a certain buffer distance to the given tolerance measure 4 Animal handling editThe flight zone is an important principle for herding working and mustering livestock An animal can be stimulated to move simply by skirting its flight zone and the animal will move in the desired direction according to the point of balance The point of balance is usually located at the animal s shoulder according to their wide angled vision An overstimulated animal will have a larger flight zone for example an excited or scared animal 22 A Cumulative Flight Zone is formed when animals move in a herd In this situation the lead animal s and the following animals Points of Balance within the cumulative flight zone must both be crossed to entice movement 22 The flight distance during handling is usually 1 5 to 7 6 m for beef cattle raised in a feeding operation and up to 30 m on mountain ranges 23 Brahman cattle have a larger flight zone than most English breeds 24 The flight zone can be thought of as the animal s personal space The size of the flight zone is determined by the tameness of the animal the more domesticated an animal the smaller the zone Fully tame animals have no flight zone 25 The flight zones in cattle vary depending on the situation they are experiencing 26 Novel situations increase their flight zone while accustomed stimuli will decrease their flight zone 26 The flight zone is larger in the front than behind due to the majority of their senses pointing forward 26 As the animal becomes more relaxed in a situation or with a person its flight zone will reduce 26 The cow s prior experiences with humans has also been shown to affect their flight zone 26 Cows with positive handling experiences were shown to have smaller flight zones than those with negative handling experiences 26 Studies with sheep indicated that animals confined in a narrow alley had a smaller flight zone compared to animals confined in a wider alley 27 Handlers sometimes make the mistake of deeply invading the flight zone when animals are being driven down an alley or into an enclosed area such as a crowd pen If the handler deeply penetrates the flight zone the animals may turn back and run over them in an attempt to escape 28 Confining a livestock animal in a crush chute or alley can make it feel more secure and thus reduce the size of the flight zone however it does not eliminate the flight zone An animal in a livestock raceway or alley that feels threatened may panic and injure itself or other animals 29 If handlers lean over fences around animals they penetrate the zone of safety and may cause the animals to rear 30 Husbandry edit Animals have a tendency to move in the opposite direction when their handler walks deep into their flight zone By crossing an animal s point of balance within the flight zone a handler can move the herd in a particular direction and control their speed of movement For example crossing the point of balance from front to back will move the animal forwards while the opposite is also true The handler s pace should always reflect the animal s speed when herding Additionally pressure should be alternated on the flight zone to reduce stress 25 Constant pressure should never be applied If animals turn to face the handler he or she is considered to be no longer penetrating the flight zone 25 It is important that a handler does not pursue any struggling animals as this will cause undue stress Instead the animal should be allowed to return to the group as animals naturally have herd instincts and will follow the group s leader A good herding practice applying this is the movement of animals through a race by maintaining a steady flow of animals not herding in groups this allows new animals to follow the leader calmly 31 When yarded animals should always have room to turn away from the handler to reduce stress Minimal stress prevents injury to the animal and maintains good production such as increased quality of meat and improved muscle and fat scores 29 Appropriate frequent penetration of the flight zone can train the animals to minimise their flight zone to the handler 25 Poor husbandry skills including over penetration of the flight zone results in the following behaviours stress panic aggression bolting prey behaviour charging fainting sickness and self inflicted damage Rough handling such as constant flight zone pressure can raise the heart rate of an animal These factors are all reflective of the General Adaptation Syndrome General adaptation syndrome edit The general adaptation syndrome GAS is a three phase response to stress in animals The first phase is the fight or flight response the animal flight zone is included in this Over penetration of the animal flight zone causes stimulation of the sympathetic nervous system SNS The SNS produces localised adjustments and responses this includes the excretion of large quantities of epinephrine from the medulla of the adrenal gland 32 Epinephrine is commonly known as adrenaline Adrenaline increases the supply of oxygen to vital organs and decreases supply to others 33 Frequent subjection to fight or flight situations causes severe endocrine disorders The second phase is Adaptation and Resistance It is the idea that recurring subjection builds natural immunity 33 and common handler movement and herding minimises an animals flight zone The third phase is Exhaustion Strong constant and over frequent stimulation of an animals flight zone may lead to death decreases production and lower quality of life According to the GAS a full recovery from exhaustion is possible over time 33 Sample values editSample escape distances mostly mean from humans Birds of Europe nbsp Because many birds flee from humans ornithologists and birdwatchers sometimes use digiscoping equipment allowing them to take pictures from long distances Species ED m Ref Brent goose 130 1000 319 10 Northern pintail 100 500 294 10 Grey heron 255 11 Mallard 60 400 236 10 Northern lapwing 162 11 Dunlin 15 450 70 10 Eurasian coot 68 11 Whinchat 20 30 34 Eurasian blackbird 10 34 Blue tit 10 34 Birds of North America 4 Species FID m Golden eagle 105 390Rough legged hawk 53 884Bald eagle 50 884American golden plover 201Great blue heron 201Merlin 18 183Prairie falcon 18 183Great egret 101Western Meadowlark 30American robin 9 Mammals of North America 4 Species FID m Pronghorn Antilocapra americana 235Mule deer Odocoileus hemionus 149 250Elk Cervus canadensis 85 201Bison Bison bison 101Factors affecting escape distances for birds edit nbsp Fleeing blackbirdEscape distance may differ significantly depending on many circumstances Body size is the best known general factor influencing interspecies differences Very frequently large species are more timid than small species because size affects how rapidly a bird can take off 11 10 Surprisingly an analysis of hundreds of studies found that larger birds are more tolerant of humans despite substantial research that shows larger animals are less tolerant of humans 6 Among the affecting factors Species 10 9 18 17 11 34 for example mallard Anas platyrhynchos has shorter ED than pintail Anas acuta 10 Age young birds are less shy for example dunlin Calidris alpina citation needed Habituation to walking people 10 Mallards Anas platyrhynchos or Canada Geese Branta canadensis are less shy in a park than somewhere in the wild Tits and Nuthatches near the feeder or in the park are less shy than in the wild Season For example wintering bullfinches Pyrrhula pyrrhula have shorter EDs than breeding ones 34 Origin of birds sometimes wintering birds from the north perhaps not knowing people are less shy than native for example nutcracker Nucifraga caryocatactes 34 Given bird individual citation needed Color of clothes of an observer and observer s behaviour 34 Hunting status quarry species have longer EDs than non quarry increasing during hunting season or after days with hunting 10 11 Flock size 11 Flock composition for example birds in mixed flocks of mallard Anas platyrhynchos and teal Anas crecca react at longer distances than those in single species flocks for either species 11 Or dunlin Calidris alpina in flocks with other waders 34 Visibility of the stimulus to the birds 11 Wind force 10 Vegetation height 11 Flight Zone in Lizards editIt is shown that the Lizard Urosaurus ornatus where different colored males have differing FID values due to differences in hormonal levels See also editBirdwatching Digiscoping Fight or flight response Herding Nature photography Personal space Wildlife management Wildlife photographyReferences edit a b Bentrup G 2008 Conservation buffers design guidelines for buffers corridors and greenways Gen Tech Rep Asheville NC USDA Forest Service Southern Research Station SRS 109 Bentrup G 2008 Flight Initiation Distance Buffers USDA National Agroforestry Center Archived from the original on 12 December 2012 Retrieved 6 September 2012 Grandin Temple Deesing Mark 2014 Genetics and Behavior During Handling Restraint and Herding Elsevier Inc p 121 a b c d e Bentrup G 2008 Flight Initiation Distance Buffers USDA National Agroforestry Center Archived from the original on 12 December 2012 Retrieved 6 September 2012 a b Moller A P 2008 Flight distance and population trends in European breeding birds Behavioral Ecology 19 6 1095 1102 doi 10 1093 beheco arn103 a b c Stuart Wolpert 16 November 2015 Why are some wild animals more tolerant to human interaction than others UCLA Retrieved 30 December 2016 Kilgour R 1971 Animal handling in works pertinent behaviour studies 13th Meat Industry Research Conference Hamilton New Zealand pp 9 12 a b c d e f Blumstein D T Anthony L L Harcourt R Ross G 2003 Testing a key assumption of wildlife buffer zones is flight initiation distance a species specific trait Biological Conservation 110 1 97 100 doi 10 1016 s0006 3207 02 00180 5 a b Ruddock M Whitfield D P 2007 A Review of Disturbance Distances in Selected Bird Species A report from Natural Research Projects Ltd to Scottish Natural Heritage PDF Archived from the original PDF on February 28 2013 Retrieved September 4 2012 a b c d e f g h i j k Laursen K Kahlert J Frikke J 2005 Factors affecting escape distances of staging waterbirds PDF Wildlife Biology 11 1 13 19 doi 10 2981 0909 6396 2005 11 13 faedos 2 0 co 2 S2CID 86208796 Retrieved 4 September 2012 a b c d e f g h i j k Bregnballe T Aaen K Fox A D 2009 Escape distances from human pedestrians by staging waterbirds in a Danish wetland PDF Wildfowl Special Issue 2 115 130 Retrieved 4 September 2012 permanent dead link a b Carrete M Tella J L 2009 Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls a new hypothesis on disturbance induced habitat selection Biology Letters 6 2 167 170 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2009 0739 PMC 2865052 PMID 19864278 a b Cooper W E 2000 Plesiomorphic escape decisions in cryptic horned lizards Phrynosoma having highly derived antipredatory defenses Retrieved 20 April 2013 Eason P K Sherman P T Rankin O Coleman B 2006 Factors affecting flight initiation distance in American robin The Journal of Wildlife Management 70 6 1796 1800 doi 10 2193 0022 541x 2006 70 1796 fafidi 2 0 co 2 S2CID 85926285 Cooper W E 2009 Flight initiation distance decreases during social activity in lizards Sceloporus virgatus Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63 12 1765 1771 doi 10 1007 s00265 009 0799 1 S2CID 24549272 Dill L M Houtman R 1989 The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel Sciurus carolinensis PDF Canadian Journal of Zoology 67 233 235 doi 10 1139 z89 033 a b Fernandez Juricic E Jimenez M D Lucas E 2001 Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance implications for park design PDF Environmental Conservation 28 3 263 269 doi 10 1017 S0376892901000273 S2CID 44189097 Retrieved 4 September 2012 a b Blumstein D T Oct 2003 Flight Initiation Distance in Birds Is Dependent on Intruder Starting Distance PDF The Journal of Wildlife Management Allen Press 67 4 852 857 doi 10 2307 3802692 JSTOR 3802692 Retrieved 4 September 2012 a b Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance implications for park design a b c d Moller A P Tryjanowski P Diaz M Kwiecinski Z Indykiewicz P Mitrus C Golawski A Polakowski M 2015 Urban habitats and feeders both contribute to flight initiation distance reduction in birds Behavioral Ecology 26 3 861 865 doi 10 1093 beheco arv024 a b c d e Moller A P Erritzoe J 2013 Predator prey interactions flight initiation distance and brain size Journal of Evolutionary Biology 26 1 23 42 doi 10 1111 jeb 12272 PMID 25990564 S2CID 41897304 a b Grandin Temple Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling Vision Hearing and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists Retrieved 7 October 2013 Grandin T 1980 Observations of cattle behavior applied to the design of cattle handling facilities Appl Anim Ethol 6 19 31 doi 10 1016 0304 3762 80 90091 7 Grandin T 1978 Observations of the spatial relationships between people and cattle during handling Proc Western Sect Am Soc Anim Sci 29 76 79 a b c d Grandin Temple Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling Vision Hearing and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists Retrieved 7 October 2013 a b c d e f Moran J Doyle R 2015 Cow talk understanding dairy cow behaviour to improve their welfare on Asian farms Clayton South Vic CSIRO Publishing pp 48 49 Hutson G D 1982 Flight distance in Merino sheep Animal Production 35 2 231 235 doi 10 1017 s0003356100027409 Grandin T 1989 Behavioral principles of livestock handling Retrieved 22 April 2013 a b Chambers P G Grandin T Heinz G Srisuvan T 2001 Guidelines for Humane Handling Transport and Slaughter of Livestock Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations Regional Office for Asia and the Pacific Retrieved 22 April 2013 Grandin T 1983 Handling and processing feedlot cattle In G B Thompson and C C O Mary Eds The Feedlot Lea amp Febiger Philadelphia pp 213 235 Grandin Temple Understanding Flight Zones and Point of Balance ProWay ProWay Livestock Equipment Retrieved 30 September 2020 Loewy Arthur Human Nervous System Britannica Retrieved 7 October 2013 a b c Unknown Unknown General Adaptation Syndrome Oracle Education Foundation Archived from the original on 19 October 2013 Retrieved 7 October 2013 a b c d e f g h Gotzman J Desselberger J 1979 Z lornetka wsrod ptakow With binoculars among birds Nasze hobby Our hobby in Polish 1st ed Warsaw Panstwowe Wydawnictwo Rolnicze i Lesne PWRiL Further reading editCooper W E Frederick W G 2007 Optimal flight initiation distance Journal of Theoretical Biology 244 1 59 67 Bibcode 2007JThBi 244 59C doi 10 1016 j jtbi 2006 07 011 PMID 16949619 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Flight zone amp oldid 1175383732, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.