fbpx
Wikipedia

Cognitive reflection test

The cognitive reflection test (CRT) is a task designed to measure a person's tendency to override an incorrect "gut" response and engage in further reflection to find a correct answer; however, the validity of the assessment as a measure of "cognitive reflection" or "intuitive thinking" is under question.[1] It was first described in 2005 by psychologist Shane Frederick. The CRT has a moderate positive correlation with measures of intelligence, such as the Intelligence Quotient test, and it correlates highly with various measures of mental heuristics.[2][3][4][5] Some research argue that the CRT is actually measuring cognitive abilities (colloquially known as intelligence).[6]

Later research showed that the CRT is a multifaceted construct: many start their response with the correct answer, while others fail to solve the test even if they reflect on their intuitive first answer. It has also been argued that suppression of the first answer is not the only factor behind the successful performance on the CRT: numeracy and reflectivity both account for performance.[7]

Basis of test edit

According to Frederick, there are two general types of cognitive activity called "system 1" and "system 2" (these terms have been first used by Keith Stanovich and Richard West[8]). System 1 is executed quickly without reflection, while system 2 requires conscious thought and effort. The cognitive reflection test has three questions that each have an obvious but incorrect response given by system 1. The correct response requires the activation of system 2. For system 2 to be activated, a person must note that their first answer is incorrect, which requires reflection on their own cognition.[2]

Correlating measures edit

The test has been found to correlate with many measures of economic thinking, such as numeracy,[7] temporal discounting, risk preference, and gambling preference.[2] It has also been correlated with measures of mental heuristics, such as the gambler's fallacy, understanding of regression to the mean, the sunk cost fallacy, and others.[3][4][5]

Keith Stanovich found that cognitive ability is not strongly correlated with CRT scores because it will only lead to better CRT performance under certain conditions. First, the test-taker must recognize the need to override their system 1 response, and then they must have available cognitive resources to carry out the override. If the test-taker does not need to inhibit system 1 for the override, then the system 2 response immediately follows. Otherwise, they must have the capacity to sustain inhibition of system 1 in order to engage the system 2 response.[9] Contrarily, some researchers have assessed the validity of the assessment, using an advanced item response theory method, and found that the CRT likely measures cognitive ability.[10] The authors of the study explain the validity of the CRT has been questioned due to the lack of validity studies and the lack of a psychometric approach.

Test questions and answers edit

The original test penned by Dr. Frederick contained only the three following questions:[2]

  1. A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1.00 more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?
  2. If it takes 5 machines 5 minutes to make 5 widgets, how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets?
  3. In a lake, there is a patch of lily pads. Every day, the patch doubles in size. If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake, how long would it take for the patch to cover half of the lake?

The intuitive answers to these questions that "system 1" gives typically are: 10 cents, 100 minutes, and 24 days; while the correct solutions are: 5 cents, 5 minutes, and 47 days.

Limitations and alternatives edit

Studies have estimated that between 44 and 51% of research participants have previously been exposed to the CRT.[11][12] Those participants that are familiar with the CRT tend to outscore those with no previous exposure, which raises questions about the validity of the measure in this population.[11][12] In an effort to combat limitations associated with familiarity, researchers have developed a variety of alternative measures of cognitive reflection.[13][14][15] Recent research, however, suggests that the CRT is robust to multiple exposure, so that despite the raw score increases in experienced participants, its correlations with other variables remain unaffected.[16]

Another limitation is due to a lack of strong psychometric properties and scarcity of validity studies in the literature.[17] The CRT was not designed in a manner that aligns with standards of the industry such as the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing which was developed by the American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Blacksmith, Nikki; Yang, Yongwei; Behrend, Tara S.; Ruark, Gregory A. (2019). "Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 32 (5): 599–612. doi:10.1002/bdm.2133. ISSN 1099-0771. S2CID 197706996.
  2. ^ a b c d Frederick, Shane (2005). "Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making". Journal of Economic Perspectives. 19 (4): 25–42. doi:10.1257/089533005775196732.
  3. ^ a b Oechssler, Jörg; Roider, Andreas; Schmitz, Patrick W. (2009). "Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases" (PDF). Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 72 (1): 147–152. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018. ISSN 0167-2681.
  4. ^ a b Hoppe, Eva I.; Kusterer, David J. (2011). "Behavioral biases and cognitive reflection". Economics Letters. 110 (2): 97–100. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2010.11.015. ISSN 0165-1765.
  5. ^ a b Toplak, Maggie (4 May 2011). "The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks" (PDF). Memory and Cognition. 39 (7): 1275–1289. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0104-1. PMID 21541821. Retrieved 30 May 2014.
  6. ^ Blacksmith, Nikki; Yang, Yongwei; Behrend, Tara S.; Ruark, Gregory A. (2019). "Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 32 (5): 599–612. doi:10.1002/bdm.2133. ISSN 1099-0771. S2CID 197706996.
  7. ^ a b Szaszi, B., Szollosi, A., Palfi, B., Aczél B., (2017) The cognitive reflection test revisited: exploring the ways individuals solve the test, Thinking and Reasoning, https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13546783.2017.1292954
  8. ^ Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645-665. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00003435
  9. ^ Stanovich, Keith E., & West, Richard F. (2008). "On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability." Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 94(4), 672-695. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672 http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/94/4/672
  10. ^ Blacksmith, Nikki; Yang, Yongwei; Behrend, Tara S.; Ruark, Gregory A. (2019). "Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 32 (5): 599–612. doi:10.1002/bdm.2133. ISSN 1099-0771. S2CID 197706996.
  11. ^ a b Haigh, Matthew (2016). "Has the Standard Cognitive Reflection Test Become a Victim of Its Own Success?". Advances in Cognitive Psychology. 12 (3): 145–149. doi:10.5709/acp-0193-5. PMC 5225989. PMID 28115997.
  12. ^ a b Stieger, Stefan; Reips, Ulf-Dietrich (2016-09-06). "A limitation of the Cognitive Reflection Test: familiarity". PeerJ. 4: e2395. doi:10.7717/peerj.2395. ISSN 2167-8359. PMC 5018679. PMID 27651989.
  13. ^ Primi, Caterina; Morsanyi, Kinga; Chiesi, Francesca; Donati, Maria Anna; Hamilton, Jayne (2016-12-01). "The Development and Testing of a New Version of the Cognitive Reflection Test Applying Item Response Theory (IRT)". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 29 (5): 453–469. doi:10.1002/bdm.1883. ISSN 1099-0771. S2CID 56252490.
  14. ^ Toplak, Maggie E.; West, Richard F.; Stanovich, Keith E. (2014-04-03). "Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test". Thinking & Reasoning. 20 (2): 147–168. doi:10.1080/13546783.2013.844729. ISSN 1354-6783. S2CID 53340418.
  15. ^ Thomson, Keela S.; Oppenheimer, Daniel M. (2016). "Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test". Judgment and Decision Making. 11: 99–113. doi:10.1017/S1930297500007622. S2CID 146924609.
  16. ^ Bialek, Michal; Pennycook, Gordon (2017-08-28). "The Cognitive Reflection Test is robust to multiple exposures". Behavior Research Methods. 50 (5): 1953–1959. doi:10.3758/s13428-017-0963-x. PMID 28849403.
  17. ^ Blacksmith, Nikki; Yang, Yongwei; Behrend, Tara S.; Ruark, Gregory A. (2019). "Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test". Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 32 (5): 599–612. doi:10.1002/bdm.2133. ISSN 1099-0771. S2CID 197706996.

cognitive, reflection, test, cognitive, reflection, test, task, designed, measure, person, tendency, override, incorrect, response, engage, further, reflection, find, correct, answer, however, validity, assessment, measure, cognitive, reflection, intuitive, th. The cognitive reflection test CRT is a task designed to measure a person s tendency to override an incorrect gut response and engage in further reflection to find a correct answer however the validity of the assessment as a measure of cognitive reflection or intuitive thinking is under question 1 It was first described in 2005 by psychologist Shane Frederick The CRT has a moderate positive correlation with measures of intelligence such as the Intelligence Quotient test and it correlates highly with various measures of mental heuristics 2 3 4 5 Some research argue that the CRT is actually measuring cognitive abilities colloquially known as intelligence 6 Later research showed that the CRT is a multifaceted construct many start their response with the correct answer while others fail to solve the test even if they reflect on their intuitive first answer It has also been argued that suppression of the first answer is not the only factor behind the successful performance on the CRT numeracy and reflectivity both account for performance 7 Contents 1 Basis of test 2 Correlating measures 3 Test questions and answers 4 Limitations and alternatives 5 See also 6 ReferencesBasis of test editAccording to Frederick there are two general types of cognitive activity called system 1 and system 2 these terms have been first used by Keith Stanovich and Richard West 8 System 1 is executed quickly without reflection while system 2 requires conscious thought and effort The cognitive reflection test has three questions that each have an obvious but incorrect response given by system 1 The correct response requires the activation of system 2 For system 2 to be activated a person must note that their first answer is incorrect which requires reflection on their own cognition 2 Correlating measures editThe test has been found to correlate with many measures of economic thinking such as numeracy 7 temporal discounting risk preference and gambling preference 2 It has also been correlated with measures of mental heuristics such as the gambler s fallacy understanding of regression to the mean the sunk cost fallacy and others 3 4 5 Keith Stanovich found that cognitive ability is not strongly correlated with CRT scores because it will only lead to better CRT performance under certain conditions First the test taker must recognize the need to override their system 1 response and then they must have available cognitive resources to carry out the override If the test taker does not need to inhibit system 1 for the override then the system 2 response immediately follows Otherwise they must have the capacity to sustain inhibition of system 1 in order to engage the system 2 response 9 Contrarily some researchers have assessed the validity of the assessment using an advanced item response theory method and found that the CRT likely measures cognitive ability 10 The authors of the study explain the validity of the CRT has been questioned due to the lack of validity studies and the lack of a psychometric approach Test questions and answers editThe original test penned by Dr Frederick contained only the three following questions 2 A bat and a ball cost 1 10 in total The bat costs 1 00 more than the ball How much does the ball cost If it takes 5 machines 5 minutes to make 5 widgets how long would it take 100 machines to make 100 widgets In a lake there is a patch of lily pads Every day the patch doubles in size If it takes 48 days for the patch to cover the entire lake how long would it take for the patch to cover half of the lake The intuitive answers to these questions that system 1 gives typically are 10 cents 100 minutes and 24 days while the correct solutions are 5 cents 5 minutes and 47 days Limitations and alternatives editStudies have estimated that between 44 and 51 of research participants have previously been exposed to the CRT 11 12 Those participants that are familiar with the CRT tend to outscore those with no previous exposure which raises questions about the validity of the measure in this population 11 12 In an effort to combat limitations associated with familiarity researchers have developed a variety of alternative measures of cognitive reflection 13 14 15 Recent research however suggests that the CRT is robust to multiple exposure so that despite the raw score increases in experienced participants its correlations with other variables remain unaffected 16 Another limitation is due to a lack of strong psychometric properties and scarcity of validity studies in the literature 17 The CRT was not designed in a manner that aligns with standards of the industry such as the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing which was developed by the American Educational Research Association American Psychological Association amp National Council on Measurement in Education See also editDual process theory it gives more background about system 1 and system 2 thinking Thinking Fast and Slow book by Daniel Kahneman Trick questionReferences edit Blacksmith Nikki Yang Yongwei Behrend Tara S Ruark Gregory A 2019 Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 32 5 599 612 doi 10 1002 bdm 2133 ISSN 1099 0771 S2CID 197706996 a b c d Frederick Shane 2005 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making Journal of Economic Perspectives 19 4 25 42 doi 10 1257 089533005775196732 a b Oechssler Jorg Roider Andreas Schmitz Patrick W 2009 Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases PDF Journal of Economic Behavior amp Organization 72 1 147 152 doi 10 1016 j jebo 2009 04 018 ISSN 0167 2681 a b Hoppe Eva I Kusterer David J 2011 Behavioral biases and cognitive reflection Economics Letters 110 2 97 100 doi 10 1016 j econlet 2010 11 015 ISSN 0165 1765 a b Toplak Maggie 4 May 2011 The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics and biases tasks PDF Memory and Cognition 39 7 1275 1289 doi 10 3758 s13421 011 0104 1 PMID 21541821 Retrieved 30 May 2014 Blacksmith Nikki Yang Yongwei Behrend Tara S Ruark Gregory A 2019 Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 32 5 599 612 doi 10 1002 bdm 2133 ISSN 1099 0771 S2CID 197706996 a b Szaszi B Szollosi A Palfi B Aczel B 2017 The cognitive reflection test revisited exploring the ways individuals solve the test Thinking and Reasoning https www tandfonline com doi abs 10 1080 13546783 2017 1292954 Stanovich K E amp West R F 2000 Individual differences in reasoning Implications for the rationality debate Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 5 645 665 https doi org 10 1017 s0140525x00003435 Stanovich Keith E amp West Richard F 2008 On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability Personality Processes and Individual Differences 94 4 672 695 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 94 4 672 http psycnet apa org journals psp 94 4 672 Blacksmith Nikki Yang Yongwei Behrend Tara S Ruark Gregory A 2019 Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 32 5 599 612 doi 10 1002 bdm 2133 ISSN 1099 0771 S2CID 197706996 a b Haigh Matthew 2016 Has the Standard Cognitive Reflection Test Become a Victim of Its Own Success Advances in Cognitive Psychology 12 3 145 149 doi 10 5709 acp 0193 5 PMC 5225989 PMID 28115997 a b Stieger Stefan Reips Ulf Dietrich 2016 09 06 A limitation of the Cognitive Reflection Test familiarity PeerJ 4 e2395 doi 10 7717 peerj 2395 ISSN 2167 8359 PMC 5018679 PMID 27651989 Primi Caterina Morsanyi Kinga Chiesi Francesca Donati Maria Anna Hamilton Jayne 2016 12 01 The Development and Testing of a New Version of the Cognitive Reflection Test Applying Item Response Theory IRT Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 29 5 453 469 doi 10 1002 bdm 1883 ISSN 1099 0771 S2CID 56252490 Toplak Maggie E West Richard F Stanovich Keith E 2014 04 03 Assessing miserly information processing An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test Thinking amp Reasoning 20 2 147 168 doi 10 1080 13546783 2013 844729 ISSN 1354 6783 S2CID 53340418 Thomson Keela S Oppenheimer Daniel M 2016 Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test Judgment and Decision Making 11 99 113 doi 10 1017 S1930297500007622 S2CID 146924609 Bialek Michal Pennycook Gordon 2017 08 28 The Cognitive Reflection Test is robust to multiple exposures Behavior Research Methods 50 5 1953 1959 doi 10 3758 s13428 017 0963 x PMID 28849403 Blacksmith Nikki Yang Yongwei Behrend Tara S Ruark Gregory A 2019 Assessing the validity of inferences from scores on the cognitive reflection test Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 32 5 599 612 doi 10 1002 bdm 2133 ISSN 1099 0771 S2CID 197706996 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Cognitive reflection test amp oldid 1183989004, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.