fbpx
Wikipedia

Cognitive ecology

Cognitive ecology is the study of cognitive phenomena within social and natural contexts.[1] It is an integrative perspective drawing from aspects of ecological psychology, cognitive science, evolutionary ecology and anthropology. Notions of domain-specific modules in the brain and the cognitive biases they create are central to understanding the enacted nature of cognition within a cognitive ecological framework. This means that cognitive mechanisms not only shape the characteristics of thought, but they dictate the success of culturally transmitted ideas. Because culturally transmitted concepts can often inform ecological decision-making behaviors, group-level trends in cognition (i.e., culturally salient concepts) are hypothesized to address ecologically relevant challenges.[2]

Theoretical basis edit

Cognitive ecology explores the interactive relationship between organism-environment interactions and its impact on cognitive phenomena.[1] Human cognition in this framework is multimodal and viewed similarly to enactivist perspectives on cognitive processing. For cultural concepts, this emphasizes cognitive distribution across an ecosystem, which is predicated on models of the extended mind thesis.

Ecological psychology edit

While the multi-faceted nature of cognitive ecology is a consequence of its interdisciplinary history, it primarily derives from early work in ecological psychology. Paradigm shifts from behaviorist orientations of psychology to cognition, or the "cognitive revolution",[3][4] gave rise to the ecological psychology approach, which distanced itself from mainstream cognitivist views by breaking down the common mind-environment dichotomy of psychological theory.[5]

One particularly influential progenitor of this work was ecological psychologist James Gibson, whose legacy is marked by his ideas on ecological and social affordances. These are the opportunistic features of environmental objects that can be exploited for human use, and are therefore particularly perceptible (e.g., a knob affords twisting, an agreeable social cue affords a warm reaction).[6] Gibson argued further that organisms cannot be disentangled from their environments, and that their cognitive constraints were consequences of a limited set of environmental invariants which shaped them over evolutionary time.[5][7] An illustrative example for Gibson is the human capacity for three-dimensional visual perception, which he argues is a cognitive concept resulting from the way that people interact with their environment.[8]

Another foreshadowed element of cognitive ecological theory comes from ecological anthropologist Gregory Bateson, who considered the notion of informational feedback loops between mind and environment, particularly their role in generating meaning and awareness of one's surroundings. In an essay, he speculates on how an observer might best delineate the "self" of a blind man. In his treatment, he questions whether one may arbitrarily choose to carve out the man's informational processing loop at his brain or his hands or his walking stick without offering an incomplete view of his cognitive process.[9] This discussion of concept remains influential in modern cognitive ecological considerations of the densely interconnected elements of ecology that play relevant roles in cognition.[1]

Enactivism edit

An enactive perspective of cognition is fundamental to a cognitive ecological view.[10] Rather than a passive interpretation of internally represented information, cognition is considered to be an active process involving the transformation of information into meaningful relationships between the organism and its environment.[11] For humans then, a perceived environment is only constructed insofar as cognitive constraints will allow. In other words, they "enact a world" by building perspectives out of ecological information, using their evolved cognitive equipment.[12]

Extended cognition edit

Cognitive ecology borrows ideas from views of extended cognition, as articulated by Chalmers and Clark (1998). They argue that humans cognitively utilize elements of their environment to aid the cognitive process and further entangle the mind-environment relationship as a result. They illustrate their claim with a hypothetical example of two people who achieve the same navigational success through a museum by different means; a person with Alzheimer's may use a notebook with written directions, while another may use her memory. The primary difference between the two people is that the former outsourced his memory to readily available external representations of information about the museum, whereas the latter relied on internal representations. A variant of this concept they also consider is socially extended cognition, which is a similar outsourcing of cognitive representations into other peoples' minds.[13] These ideas elaborate a cognitive interpretation of broader anthropological notions maintaining that humans are a species deeply entangled in social and material elements of culture.[14]

Distributed cognition edit

Distributed cognition is an important model of the extended mind thesis for cognitive ecological theory put forth by Edwin Hutchins.[15][16] This conceptualizes human groups as active networks with cognitive properties of their own, much like neural networks themselves yield emergent cognitive properties. For a social group, cognitive properties are disseminated into an individual's surrounding network.[17] The cognitive properties of a group, Hutchins notes, is completely distinct from those of an individual.[1] Distributed cognition is fundamentally contingent on and emergent from trending ideas among a collection of brains and artefacts.[18]

This is conceptually similar to models of collective cognition in other social animal groups, which use agent based models to understanding insect swarming, fish schooling, bird flocking and baboon pack behaviors.[19][20][21] Collective cognition in social animal groups is adaptive because the group can amplify its overall responsiveness to ecological cues.[20] Likewise, the computational power of a human group can be more effective than that of even its best individuals.[22] This idea is echoed by anthropologists noting the collective intentionality of cultural institutions.[23]

Existing models of cultural learning dynamics seem to articulate the mechanisms by which information is acquired by and distributed within groups. In particular, cultural evolution theorists assert that individual learning is required for tracking environmental dynamics,[24] but this information is retained in culture by social learning.[25] For Hutchins, this theoretical similarity is not a coincidence. After describing distributed cognitive networks and their relationships with ecological dynamics as "cognitive ecosystems", he defines culture as a "shorthand way of referring to a complex cognitive ecosystem."[1]

Applications to cultural concepts edit

Religious beliefs edit

Religious behaviors typically exist in the form of ritual and correspond to religious god concepts.[26] These behaviors are phenotypic outcomes of god concepts that are ultimately subject to natural selection.[27][28] Cognitive ecologists who study religion predict that god concepts across cultures can be linked to coordination solutions for local socioecological challenges, such as large-scale cooperation, intragroup cohesion and commitment, and resource management.[2][29] For example, an omniscient and morally punitive "Big God" may be adaptive for large-scale populations by motivating prosocial behavior,[30] whereas gods associated with small-scale societies are often concerned about the stability of local resources.[31]

Economic exchange edit

Social contracts and their associated fairness norms are thought by many economists to be contingent on means of production. A hunter-gatherer society, for instance, may operate at an equilibrium where each person contributes to the best of his or her ability and receives according to need. But if this society were to shift toward larger-scale agricultural practices, this equilibrium would be destabilized by increases in free riding and general temptations to profit by defecting.[32] This has been supported empirically in cross-cultural studies using experimental economic game data, which showed a wide range of variance in fairness expectations between populations based on culturally-specific exchange concepts.[33][34]

This shift in fairness expectations has also been implicated in archaeological data. In particular, the relaxed sharing norms hypothesized to be built upon periods of successful maize exploitation in the pre-Hispanic Pueblo Southwest seemed to be eroded by decreases in agricultural success. In other words, when crops began to fail and supply became low, cultural exchange norms became more stringent, kin-based and based on reciprocity.[35]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e Hutchins, Edwin (2010-10-01). "Cognitive Ecology". Topics in Cognitive Science. 2 (4): 705–715. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01089.x. ISSN 1756-8765. PMID 25164051.
  2. ^ a b Botero, Carlos A.; Gardner, Beth; Kirby, Kathryn R.; Bulbulia, Joseph; Gavin, Michael C.; Gray, Russell D. (25 November 2014). "The ecology of religious beliefs". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (47): 16784–16789. Bibcode:2014PNAS..11116784B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1408701111. PMC 4250141. PMID 25385605.
  3. ^ Miller, George A (March 2003). "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. PMID 12639696. S2CID 206129621.
  4. ^ Neisser, Ulric (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall. ISBN 978-0131396678.
  5. ^ a b Heft, Harry (2013). "An ecological approach to psychology". Review of General Psychology. 17 (2): 162–167. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.400.9646. doi:10.1037/a0032928. S2CID 147335659.
  6. ^ Chemero, Anthony (2003). "An outline of a theory of affordances". Ecological Psychology. 15 (2): 181–195. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.475.4827. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1502_5. S2CID 143720358.
  7. ^ Gibson, James J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception ([Nachdr.]. ed.). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0898599596.
  8. ^ Gibson, James (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1848725782.
  9. ^ Bateson, Gregory (2000). Steps to an ecology of mind (University of Chicago Press ed.). Chicago [u.a.]: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-03905-3.
  10. ^ Palacios, AG; Bozinovic, F (2003). "An "enactive" approach to integrative and comparative biology: thoughts on the table". Biological Research. 36 (1): 101–5. doi:10.4067/s0716-97602003000100008. PMID 12795209.
  11. ^ Noë, Alva (2004). Action in perception (Paperback ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0262140881.
  12. ^ Stewart, John; Gapenne, Olivier; Di Paolo, Ezequiel A. (2014). Enaction: toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge, Massachusetts. ISBN 978-0-262-52601-2. OCLC 880401178.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  13. ^ Clark, A.; Chalmers, D. (1 January 1998). "The Extended Mind". Analysis. 58 (1): 7–19. doi:10.1093/analys/58.1.7.
  14. ^ Hodder, Ian (2012). Entangled. ; An Archaeology of the Relationships Between Humans and Things (first publ. ed.). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons, Incorporated. ISBN 978-0-470-67212-9.
  15. ^ Hutchins, Edwin (2000). "Distributed cognition". International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences.
  16. ^ Hutchins, Edwin (1996). Cognition in the wild (8. pr. ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press (A Bradford Book). ISBN 9780262581462.
  17. ^ Heylighen, Heath; Van Overwalle (2003). "The Emergence of Distributed Cognition: a conceptual framework". Proceedings of Collective Intentionality IV.
  18. ^ Rogers, Ellis (1994). "Distributed Cognition: an alternative framework for analysing and explaining collaborative working". Journal of Information Technology. 9 (2): 119–128. doi:10.1177/026839629400900203. S2CID 219981758.
  19. ^ Strandburg-Peshkin, A.; Farine, D. R.; Couzin, I. D.; Crofoot, M. C. (18 June 2015). "Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons". Science. 348 (6241): 1358–1361. Bibcode:2015Sci...348.1358S. doi:10.1126/science.aaa5099. PMC 4801504. PMID 26089514.
  20. ^ a b Couzin, Iain D. (January 2009). "Collective cognition in animal groups". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 13 (1): 36–43. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.278.5663. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.002. PMID 19058992. S2CID 15175317.
  21. ^ Couzin, Iain (15 February 2007). "Collective minds". Nature. 445 (7129): 715. Bibcode:2007Natur.445..715C. doi:10.1038/445715a. PMID 17301775.
  22. ^ Clément, Romain J. G.; Krause, Stefan; von Engelhardt, Nikolaus; Faria, Jolyon J.; Krause, Jens; Kurvers, Ralf H. J. M.; de Polavieja, GonzaloÂ. G. (17 October 2013). "Collective Cognition in Humans: Groups Outperform Their Best Members in a Sentence Reconstruction Task". PLOS ONE. 8 (10): e77943. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...877943C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077943. PMC 3798465. PMID 24147101.
  23. ^ Tomasello, Michael; Melis, Alicia P.; Tennie, Claudio; Wyman, Emily; Herrmann, Esther (December 2012). "Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation" (PDF). Current Anthropology. 53 (6): 673–692. doi:10.1086/668207. S2CID 210221053.
  24. ^ Henrich, Joseph; McElreath, Richard (19 May 2003). "The evolution of cultural evolution". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 12 (3): 123–135. doi:10.1002/evan.10110. S2CID 14302229.
  25. ^ Boyd, R.; Richerson, P. J.; Henrich, J. (20 June 2011). "The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (Supplement_2): 10918–10925. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10810918B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1100290108. PMC 3131818. PMID 21690340.
  26. ^ Purzycki & Sosis (2011). "Our Gods: Variation in Supernatural Minds". In Ulrich, Frey (ed.). Essential Building Blocks of Human Nature. Bibcode:2011ebbh.book.....F.
  27. ^ Codding, Brian F.; Bird, Douglas W. (April 2015). "Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological science". Journal of Archaeological Science. 56: 9–20. Bibcode:2015JArSc..56....9C. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2015.02.027.
  28. ^ Cronk, Lee (1991). "Human Behavioral Ecology". Annual Review of Anthropology. 20 (1): 25–53. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.20.1.25.
  29. ^ Purzycki; McNamara (2016). "An Ecological Theory of Gods' Minds". Advances in Religion, Cognitive Science and Experimental Philosophy.
  30. ^ Norenzayan, Ara; Shariff, Azim F.; Gervais, Will M.; Willard, Aiyana K.; McNamara, Rita A.; Slingerland, Edward; Henrich, Joseph (2 December 2014). "The cultural evolution of prosocial religions". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 39: e1. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14001356. PMID 26785995.
  31. ^ Purzycki, Benjamin Grant (13 April 2016). "The Evolution of Gods' Minds in the Tyva Republic". Current Anthropology. 57: S88–S104. doi:10.1086/685729. S2CID 33452734.
  32. ^ Binmore, Ken (22 December 2005). "Economic man – or straw man?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (6): 817–818. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05230145. S2CID 53375617.
  33. ^ Henrich, Boyd; Bowles; Camerer; Fehr; Gintis; McElreath; Aivard; Barr; Ensminger; Henrich; Hill; Gil-White; Gurven; Marlowe; Patton; Tracer (2005). ""Economic man" in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (6): 795–815, discussion 815–55. doi:10.1017/s0140525x05000142. PMID 16372952. S2CID 3194574.
  34. ^ Gachter, S.; Herrmann, B.; Thoni, C. (2 August 2010). "Culture and cooperation". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 365 (1553): 2651–2661. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0135. PMC 2936171. PMID 20679109.
  35. ^ Bocinsky, R. K.; Rush, J.; Kintigh, K. W.; Kohler, T. A. (1 April 2016). "Exploration and exploitation in the macrohistory of the pre-Hispanic Pueblo Southwest". Science Advances. 2 (4): e1501532. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E1532B. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1501532. PMC 4820384. PMID 27051879.

cognitive, ecology, study, cognitive, phenomena, within, social, natural, contexts, integrative, perspective, drawing, from, aspects, ecological, psychology, cognitive, science, evolutionary, ecology, anthropology, notions, domain, specific, modules, brain, co. Cognitive ecology is the study of cognitive phenomena within social and natural contexts 1 It is an integrative perspective drawing from aspects of ecological psychology cognitive science evolutionary ecology and anthropology Notions of domain specific modules in the brain and the cognitive biases they create are central to understanding the enacted nature of cognition within a cognitive ecological framework This means that cognitive mechanisms not only shape the characteristics of thought but they dictate the success of culturally transmitted ideas Because culturally transmitted concepts can often inform ecological decision making behaviors group level trends in cognition i e culturally salient concepts are hypothesized to address ecologically relevant challenges 2 Contents 1 Theoretical basis 1 1 Ecological psychology 1 2 Enactivism 2 Extended cognition 2 1 Distributed cognition 3 Applications to cultural concepts 3 1 Religious beliefs 3 2 Economic exchange 4 See also 5 ReferencesTheoretical basis editCognitive ecology explores the interactive relationship between organism environment interactions and its impact on cognitive phenomena 1 Human cognition in this framework is multimodal and viewed similarly to enactivist perspectives on cognitive processing For cultural concepts this emphasizes cognitive distribution across an ecosystem which is predicated on models of the extended mind thesis Ecological psychology edit While the multi faceted nature of cognitive ecology is a consequence of its interdisciplinary history it primarily derives from early work in ecological psychology Paradigm shifts from behaviorist orientations of psychology to cognition or the cognitive revolution 3 4 gave rise to the ecological psychology approach which distanced itself from mainstream cognitivist views by breaking down the common mind environment dichotomy of psychological theory 5 One particularly influential progenitor of this work was ecological psychologist James Gibson whose legacy is marked by his ideas on ecological and social affordances These are the opportunistic features of environmental objects that can be exploited for human use and are therefore particularly perceptible e g a knob affords twisting an agreeable social cue affords a warm reaction 6 Gibson argued further that organisms cannot be disentangled from their environments and that their cognitive constraints were consequences of a limited set of environmental invariants which shaped them over evolutionary time 5 7 An illustrative example for Gibson is the human capacity for three dimensional visual perception which he argues is a cognitive concept resulting from the way that people interact with their environment 8 Another foreshadowed element of cognitive ecological theory comes from ecological anthropologist Gregory Bateson who considered the notion of informational feedback loops between mind and environment particularly their role in generating meaning and awareness of one s surroundings In an essay he speculates on how an observer might best delineate the self of a blind man In his treatment he questions whether one may arbitrarily choose to carve out the man s informational processing loop at his brain or his hands or his walking stick without offering an incomplete view of his cognitive process 9 This discussion of concept remains influential in modern cognitive ecological considerations of the densely interconnected elements of ecology that play relevant roles in cognition 1 Enactivism edit An enactive perspective of cognition is fundamental to a cognitive ecological view 10 Rather than a passive interpretation of internally represented information cognition is considered to be an active process involving the transformation of information into meaningful relationships between the organism and its environment 11 For humans then a perceived environment is only constructed insofar as cognitive constraints will allow In other words they enact a world by building perspectives out of ecological information using their evolved cognitive equipment 12 Extended cognition editCognitive ecology borrows ideas from views of extended cognition as articulated by Chalmers and Clark 1998 They argue that humans cognitively utilize elements of their environment to aid the cognitive process and further entangle the mind environment relationship as a result They illustrate their claim with a hypothetical example of two people who achieve the same navigational success through a museum by different means a person with Alzheimer s may use a notebook with written directions while another may use her memory The primary difference between the two people is that the former outsourced his memory to readily available external representations of information about the museum whereas the latter relied on internal representations A variant of this concept they also consider is socially extended cognition which is a similar outsourcing of cognitive representations into other peoples minds 13 These ideas elaborate a cognitive interpretation of broader anthropological notions maintaining that humans are a species deeply entangled in social and material elements of culture 14 Distributed cognition edit Distributed cognition is an important model of the extended mind thesis for cognitive ecological theory put forth by Edwin Hutchins 15 16 This conceptualizes human groups as active networks with cognitive properties of their own much like neural networks themselves yield emergent cognitive properties For a social group cognitive properties are disseminated into an individual s surrounding network 17 The cognitive properties of a group Hutchins notes is completely distinct from those of an individual 1 Distributed cognition is fundamentally contingent on and emergent from trending ideas among a collection of brains and artefacts 18 This is conceptually similar to models of collective cognition in other social animal groups which use agent based models to understanding insect swarming fish schooling bird flocking and baboon pack behaviors 19 20 21 Collective cognition in social animal groups is adaptive because the group can amplify its overall responsiveness to ecological cues 20 Likewise the computational power of a human group can be more effective than that of even its best individuals 22 This idea is echoed by anthropologists noting the collective intentionality of cultural institutions 23 Existing models of cultural learning dynamics seem to articulate the mechanisms by which information is acquired by and distributed within groups In particular cultural evolution theorists assert that individual learning is required for tracking environmental dynamics 24 but this information is retained in culture by social learning 25 For Hutchins this theoretical similarity is not a coincidence After describing distributed cognitive networks and their relationships with ecological dynamics as cognitive ecosystems he defines culture as a shorthand way of referring to a complex cognitive ecosystem 1 Applications to cultural concepts editReligious beliefs edit Main article Cognitive ecology of religion Religious behaviors typically exist in the form of ritual and correspond to religious god concepts 26 These behaviors are phenotypic outcomes of god concepts that are ultimately subject to natural selection 27 28 Cognitive ecologists who study religion predict that god concepts across cultures can be linked to coordination solutions for local socioecological challenges such as large scale cooperation intragroup cohesion and commitment and resource management 2 29 For example an omniscient and morally punitive Big God may be adaptive for large scale populations by motivating prosocial behavior 30 whereas gods associated with small scale societies are often concerned about the stability of local resources 31 Economic exchange edit Social contracts and their associated fairness norms are thought by many economists to be contingent on means of production A hunter gatherer society for instance may operate at an equilibrium where each person contributes to the best of his or her ability and receives according to need But if this society were to shift toward larger scale agricultural practices this equilibrium would be destabilized by increases in free riding and general temptations to profit by defecting 32 This has been supported empirically in cross cultural studies using experimental economic game data which showed a wide range of variance in fairness expectations between populations based on culturally specific exchange concepts 33 34 This shift in fairness expectations has also been implicated in archaeological data In particular the relaxed sharing norms hypothesized to be built upon periods of successful maize exploitation in the pre Hispanic Pueblo Southwest seemed to be eroded by decreases in agricultural success In other words when crops began to fail and supply became low cultural exchange norms became more stringent kin based and based on reciprocity 35 See also editCognitive ecology of individual recognition in colonial birdsReferences edit a b c d e Hutchins Edwin 2010 10 01 Cognitive Ecology Topics in Cognitive Science 2 4 705 715 doi 10 1111 j 1756 8765 2010 01089 x ISSN 1756 8765 PMID 25164051 a b Botero Carlos A Gardner Beth Kirby Kathryn R Bulbulia Joseph Gavin Michael C Gray Russell D 25 November 2014 The ecology of religious beliefs Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 47 16784 16789 Bibcode 2014PNAS 11116784B doi 10 1073 pnas 1408701111 PMC 4250141 PMID 25385605 Miller George A March 2003 The cognitive revolution a historical perspective Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 3 141 144 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00029 9 PMID 12639696 S2CID 206129621 Neisser Ulric 1967 Cognitive psychology New York Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0131396678 a b Heft Harry 2013 An ecological approach to psychology Review of General Psychology 17 2 162 167 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 400 9646 doi 10 1037 a0032928 S2CID 147335659 Chemero Anthony 2003 An outline of a theory of affordances Ecological Psychology 15 2 181 195 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 475 4827 doi 10 1207 s15326969eco1502 5 S2CID 143720358 Gibson James J 1986 The ecological approach to visual perception Nachdr ed New York Psychology Press ISBN 978 0898599596 Gibson James 1979 The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception Psychology Press ISBN 978 1848725782 Bateson Gregory 2000 Steps to an ecology of mind University of Chicago Press ed Chicago u a University of Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 03905 3 Palacios AG Bozinovic F 2003 An enactive approach to integrative and comparative biology thoughts on the table Biological Research 36 1 101 5 doi 10 4067 s0716 97602003000100008 PMID 12795209 Noe Alva 2004 Action in perception Paperback ed Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0262140881 Stewart John Gapenne Olivier Di Paolo Ezequiel A 2014 Enaction toward a new paradigm for cognitive science Cambridge Massachusetts ISBN 978 0 262 52601 2 OCLC 880401178 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link Clark A Chalmers D 1 January 1998 The Extended Mind Analysis 58 1 7 19 doi 10 1093 analys 58 1 7 Hodder Ian 2012 Entangled An Archaeology of the Relationships Between Humans and Things first publ ed Oxford John Wiley amp Sons Incorporated ISBN 978 0 470 67212 9 Hutchins Edwin 2000 Distributed cognition International Encyclopedia of the Social amp Behavioral Sciences Hutchins Edwin 1996 Cognition in the wild 8 pr ed Cambridge Mass The MIT Press A Bradford Book ISBN 9780262581462 Heylighen Heath Van Overwalle 2003 The Emergence of Distributed Cognition a conceptual framework Proceedings of Collective Intentionality IV Rogers Ellis 1994 Distributed Cognition an alternative framework for analysing and explaining collaborative working Journal of Information Technology 9 2 119 128 doi 10 1177 026839629400900203 S2CID 219981758 Strandburg Peshkin A Farine D R Couzin I D Crofoot M C 18 June 2015 Shared decision making drives collective movement in wild baboons Science 348 6241 1358 1361 Bibcode 2015Sci 348 1358S doi 10 1126 science aaa5099 PMC 4801504 PMID 26089514 a b Couzin Iain D January 2009 Collective cognition in animal groups Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 1 36 43 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 278 5663 doi 10 1016 j tics 2008 10 002 PMID 19058992 S2CID 15175317 Couzin Iain 15 February 2007 Collective minds Nature 445 7129 715 Bibcode 2007Natur 445 715C doi 10 1038 445715a PMID 17301775 Clement Romain J G Krause Stefan von Engelhardt Nikolaus Faria Jolyon J Krause Jens Kurvers Ralf H J M de Polavieja GonzaloA G 17 October 2013 Collective Cognition in Humans Groups Outperform Their Best Members in a Sentence Reconstruction Task PLOS ONE 8 10 e77943 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 877943C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0077943 PMC 3798465 PMID 24147101 Tomasello Michael Melis Alicia P Tennie Claudio Wyman Emily Herrmann Esther December 2012 Two Key Steps in the Evolution of Human Cooperation PDF Current Anthropology 53 6 673 692 doi 10 1086 668207 S2CID 210221053 Henrich Joseph McElreath Richard 19 May 2003 The evolution of cultural evolution Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 12 3 123 135 doi 10 1002 evan 10110 S2CID 14302229 Boyd R Richerson P J Henrich J 20 June 2011 The cultural niche Why social learning is essential for human adaptation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 Supplement 2 10918 10925 Bibcode 2011PNAS 10810918B doi 10 1073 pnas 1100290108 PMC 3131818 PMID 21690340 Purzycki amp Sosis 2011 Our Gods Variation in Supernatural Minds In Ulrich Frey ed Essential Building Blocks of Human Nature Bibcode 2011ebbh book F Codding Brian F Bird Douglas W April 2015 Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological science Journal of Archaeological Science 56 9 20 Bibcode 2015JArSc 56 9C doi 10 1016 j jas 2015 02 027 Cronk Lee 1991 Human Behavioral Ecology Annual Review of Anthropology 20 1 25 53 doi 10 1146 annurev anthro 20 1 25 Purzycki McNamara 2016 An Ecological Theory of Gods Minds Advances in Religion Cognitive Science and Experimental Philosophy Norenzayan Ara Shariff Azim F Gervais Will M Willard Aiyana K McNamara Rita A Slingerland Edward Henrich Joseph 2 December 2014 The cultural evolution of prosocial religions Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39 e1 doi 10 1017 S0140525X14001356 PMID 26785995 Purzycki Benjamin Grant 13 April 2016 The Evolution of Gods Minds in the Tyva Republic Current Anthropology 57 S88 S104 doi 10 1086 685729 S2CID 33452734 Binmore Ken 22 December 2005 Economic man or straw man Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 6 817 818 doi 10 1017 S0140525X05230145 S2CID 53375617 Henrich Boyd Bowles Camerer Fehr Gintis McElreath Aivard Barr Ensminger Henrich Hill Gil White Gurven Marlowe Patton Tracer 2005 Economic man in cross cultural perspective Behavioral experiments in 15 small scale societies PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 6 795 815 discussion 815 55 doi 10 1017 s0140525x05000142 PMID 16372952 S2CID 3194574 Gachter S Herrmann B Thoni C 2 August 2010 Culture and cooperation Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 365 1553 2651 2661 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0135 PMC 2936171 PMID 20679109 Bocinsky R K Rush J Kintigh K W Kohler T A 1 April 2016 Exploration and exploitation in the macrohistory of the pre Hispanic Pueblo Southwest Science Advances 2 4 e1501532 Bibcode 2016SciA 2E1532B doi 10 1126 sciadv 1501532 PMC 4820384 PMID 27051879 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Cognitive ecology amp oldid 1183916380, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.