fbpx
Wikipedia

Tufted capuchin

The tufted capuchin (Sapajus apella), also known as brown capuchin, black-capped capuchin, or pin monkey is a New World primate from South America and the Caribbean islands of Trinidad and Margarita. As traditionally defined, it is one of the most widespread primates in the Neotropics, but it has recently been recommended considering the black-striped, black and golden-bellied capuchins as separate species in a new genus, thereby effectively limiting the tufted capuchin to the Amazon basin and nearby regions.[1] However, the large-headed capuchin (S. a. macrocephalus), previously defined as a distinct species, has been reclassified as a subspecies of the tufted capuchin, expanding its range east to Peru & Ecuador and south to Bolivia.[3]

Tufted capuchin[1]
Scientific classification
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Primates
Suborder: Haplorhini
Infraorder: Simiiformes
Family: Cebidae
Genus: Sapajus
Species:
S. apella
Binomial name
Sapajus apella
Subspecies

Sapajus apella macrocephalus

Geographic range following taxonomy used by IUCN
Range of subspecies macrocephalus
Synonyms
  • Cebus apella Linnaeus, 1758
  • Simia apella Linnaeus, 1758
  • Simia fatuellus Linnaeus, 1766

The tufted capuchin is an omnivorous animal, mostly feeding on fruits and invertebrates, although it sometimes feeds on small vertebrates (e.g. lizards and bird chicks) and other plant parts. It can be found in many different kinds of environment, including moist tropical and subtropical forest, dry forest, and disturbed or secondary forest.

Like other capuchins, it is a social animal, forming groups of 8 to 15 individuals that are led by an alpha or dominant male.

Taxonomy and phylogeny

At one point all tufted capuchins were classified as Cebus apella.[4][5] Under such taxonomy, the range of C. apella would extend throughout much of South America from Colombia to northern Argentina. Although she didn't describe specific or subspecific nomenclature, Torres de Assumpção (1983; 1988) described differences between tufted capuchins from five distinct geographic regions of Brazil and high phenotypic variation of individuals in a sixth area that had a greater selection pressure.[6][7] In 2001, Silva Júnior proposed that the robust capuchins such (formerly the C. apella group) be placed in a separate genus, Sapajus, from the gracile capuchins (formerly the C. capucinus group) which retain the genus Cebus.[8] This was supported by Jessica Lynch Alfaro et al. in 2011.[9][10] Groves (2005) recognized six subspecies: Cebus apella apella, C. a. fatuellus, C. a. macrocephalus, C. a. margaritae, C. a. peruanus, C. a. tocantinus.[1] The IUCN follows Silva (2001) and recognise the species as monotypic, though the subspecies status of S. a. margaritae is unclear.[2]

Physical characteristics

 
Skeleton

The tufted capuchin is more powerfully built than the other capuchins, with rougher fur and a long, thick tail. It has a bundle of long, hardened hair on the forehead that can be raised as a sort of "wig". The fur is brownish gray, with the belly being somewhat lighter-colored than the rest of the body. The hands and feet are black. The tail is prehensile: strong and can be used for grasping, as an extra limb.

The tufted capuchin has a head-body length of 32 to 57 centimetres (13 to 22 in), a tail length of 38 to 56 centimetres (15 to 22 in), and a weight of 1.9 to 4.8 kilograms (4.2 to 10.6 lb), with the males generally being larger and heavier than the females.

Behaviour and ecology

 
Male tufted capuchin

The tufted capuchin is a diurnal, arboreal primate species, but it often forages on the ground to search for food or to walk longer distances between trees that are too far apart to jump.

The tufted capuchin lives in groups of two to twenty or more animals. A single group usually contains at least one adult male, but mixed groups with multiple males do also occur. In that case, one of the males is dominant. He accepts only a few monkeys in his direct surroundings, mainly younger animals and a few females. The dominant male and the group members that are close to him have the privilege to eat first in case of food scarcity, while subordinate monkeys have to wait until they are ready.

After a gestation period of 180 days, one young is born, or incidentally a twin. This young, which weighs only 200 to 250 grams, is carried on the back of its mother. The mother feeds her child for 9 months, but the young are sexually immature until its seventh year, which is quite late for a primate of its size.

Important natural enemies of the capuchin are large birds of prey. They are so afraid of those birds that they even become alarmed when a harmless bird flies over.

Diet

A recently discovered characteristic of one population of this species is that it uses stones as a tool to open hard nuts. First it chooses ripe nuts from a nut palm. It uses its teeth to strip off the nut's fibrous husk. Then it leaves the nut to dry for about a week. When the nut is dry, the monkey lays the nut on a large, flat rock or fallen tree,[11] hammering the nut with a suitable stone until the nut cracks. The hammerstones are often large enough to require lifting with both hands. The anvil rock is often pockmarked with hollows as a result of repeated use.[12][13]

Besides nuts, the capuchin also eats fruit, leaves, nuts,[14] seeds, pith,[15] insects and larvae, eggs and young birds, frogs, lizards, other reptiles,[15] rodents, mouse opossums,[15] and even bats. They are also known to chase cats.

The tufted capuchin looks for its food in groups. As soon as one of the group members has found something edible, he or she may make a large whistling sound, dependent upon the proximity of other individuals and abundance of the food resource so that the other monkeys know that there is something to eat.[16] The composition of the group is very well organized and is determined by rank in the hierarchy. The dominant male often resides somewhere in the middle of the group just behind the front line, so that it is safer when a predator attacks. The vanguard is composed of higher-ranked females who are tolerated by the dominant male. They have the privilege to reach the food first, but they are also the most vulnerable when a predator attacks.

Tool use and manufacture

The tufted capuchin has been observed using containers to hold water, using sticks (to dig nuts, to dip for syrup, to catch ants, to reach food), using sponges to absorb juice, using stones as hammer and chisel to penetrate a barrier[17] and using stones as hammer and anvil to crack nuts.[18] While some of these tasks are relatively simple by cognitive standards (e.g. using a stick to catch ants), others, like cracking nuts with hammer and anvil are only exceeded in complexity by chimpanzees.[19]

The potential for tool use in animals like the tufted capuchin depends on a number of conditions that would increase its likelihood of appearing in a given species. Van Schaik proposed that the occurrence of tool use would be likely in foraging species if three factors were present: manual dexterity, intelligence, and social tolerance.[20] As it applies to manual dexterity, capuchins are capable of a limited precision grip (the ability to delicately pinch and manipulate objects with the thumb and fingertips), which is not found in any other New World monkeys and only found in limited amounts in apes.[21][22] C. apella has an encephalization ratio greater than the hominids (except humans) and a neocortex ratio that is almost as large as the apes; both of these rough indicators suggest high intelligence.[23][24] Finally, the tufted capuchin forms social groups typical of a complex and tolerant society.[25][26]

The tufted capuchin has been observed manufacturing tools both in captivity and in the wild. In captivity, it has been reported as making probing sticks to reach normally inaccessible containers with syrup.[27] It is also capable of understanding the concept of "sponging" and using paper towels, monkey biscuits, sticks, leaves and straw to sop up juice and then suck on the sponge to consume the juice.[27] Research in the wild has shown that capuchin tool use is every bit as extensive as in captivity with capuchins being observed using stones to dig holes to get at tubers, an activity previously only seen in humans.[28] The practice of using stones to crack nuts has arisen spontaneously in many locations such as in the Caatinga Dry Forest[28] and Serra da Capivara National Park,[29] all in Brazil and hundreds of miles apart. It has been observed cracking various nuts and fruits such as palm nuts (Attalea and Astrocaryum spp.)[30] and jatobá fruits.(Hymenaea courbaril)[29] The tufted capuchin has even been observed using stones to dislodge other stones that would later be used as hammers or shovels, an example of a more complex tool using behavior known as second-order tool use previously only found in chimpanzees.[29] Curiously, not all tufted capuchins engage in tool use. Moura and Lee (2004)[31] suggest lack of other food sources as the key factor. Ottoni and Mannu (2001),[32] Fragaszy et al. (2004)[30] and Visalberghi et al. (2005)[33] have proposed this is likely more a factor of a monkey's terrestrial habit: the more time a monkey spends on the ground, the more likely it is to profit from (and thus engage in) tool use.

In captivity, the tufted capuchin has been seen to manufacture stone tools that produced simple flakes and cores. Some of the capuchins even used these sharpened stones to cut (in a back-and-forth motion) barriers in order to reach food.[34] The importance of this behavior is that it serves as evidence of mechanical proclivity to modify stones by using behaviors already in the monkeys' repertoires, and this behavior is seen as a precursor to stone-knapping.[21] This early and limited tool use behavior has been hypothesized as similar to pre-Homo habilis and that artifacts of that time would probably resemble those of capuchins.[34]

S. apella tool manufacture and use has been analyzed for potential clues to social learning and problem solving ability, as tool manufacture and use can often shed light on such complex cognitive abilities.[17][35] Social learning, or the ability to learn from other individuals, is a controversial topic in most nonhuman species like S. apella because of the relative difficulty of determining whether a behavior was learned from imitation or a much simpler form of social learning.[36] One way of closing the gap between concurrent tool related behaviors and their likelihood of arising from imitation is by narrowing down events that would make social learning more probable such as a preference for observing experienced tool users. In this regard, Ottoni and his team found that young capuchins tended to observe the best tool users when cracking nuts.[18]

 
An example of the Doorian Fruit, a box that can open in one of two ways (see image for more info).

Another way of isolating imitation from other simpler behaviors is to present the capuchins with a box that has food but has two different ways of opening it. The important point is that neither way should be more advantageous so that the monkey can freely choose one. In one such study, when humans opened the door in front of the monkeys using one way only, the monkeys used that method, even when they discovered the alternative on their own.[37] In another study, capuchin alphas from two separate groups were trained to open the door in a specific way, after which the monkeys were paired with subordinates who learned to open the door in the same way.[38] When capuchins are trained in the same way and this time released into their groups, the habit is once again disseminated amongst all group members even when others discover alternative ways.[39] Nevertheless, the subject of whether or not S. apella learns by imitation is still controversial, because of the inherent difficulty in teasing out unambiguous evidence of a complex cognitive process such as imitation.[37][40]

Problem solving

Tool use and manufacture can also shed light on the many aspects of the tufted capuchin's cognitive abilities by determining how it solves some problems. Some non-primates manufacture and use objects as tools. Crows are known to make hook-tools for catching insects,[41] but such activities lack the behavioral plasticity of tool use as evidenced in tufted capuchins who found new ways to use tools that other species could not.[42] But this plasticity in tool use, while suggesting greater complexity and cognitive ability, does not suggest that the monkeys understand cause and effect. It instead implies they are only able to learn from successful efforts but not from failures, nor are they able to refine and improve much.[43] Its ability to repeat successes, coupled with its complex repertoire of behavioral events helps to explain the tufted capuchin's extensive repertoire of innovative behaviors besides tool use.[43]

Distribution and habitat

This species lives in the northern Amazon rainforest of the Guyanas, Venezuela and Brazil and to the west of the Rio Negro, as far north as the Orinoco in Venezuela. It is also found in eastern Colombia, Ecuador, Bolivia, Peru, including the upper Andean Magdalena valley in Colombia. An introduced breeding population is well established in the northwestern peninsula of the island of Trinidad in the Republic of Trinidad and Tobago. The subspecies/population on Margarita Island in Venezuela, S. a. margaritae, is considered Critically Endangered by the IUCN Red List.[44] It can be found in a large variety of forest types, mainly in tropical rainforests (up till a height of 2700 m), but also in more open forests.

The distribution overlaps with that of other species of capuchins, such as the white-fronted capuchin (Cebus albifrons).

References

  1. ^ a b c Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 137. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^ a b Boubli, J.P., Stevenson, P.R., Palacios, E., de la Torre, S., Ravetta, A.L., Messias, M.R., Carvalho, A.S. & Mittermeier, R.A. 2021. Sapajus apella (amended version of 2020 assessment). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2021: e.T172351505A192594550. https://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2021-1.RLTS.T172351505A192594550.en. Downloaded on 26 August 2021.
  3. ^ Lima, Marcela G. M.; Silva-Júnior, José de Sousa e; Černý, David; Buckner, Janet C.; Aleixo, Alexandre; Chang, Jonathan; Zheng, Jimmy; Alfaro, Michael E.; Martins, Amely; Di Fiore, Anthony; Boubli, Jean P. (1 July 2018). "A phylogenomic perspective on the robust capuchin monkey (Sapajus) radiation: First evidence for extensive population admixture across South America". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 124: 137–150. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2018.02.023. ISSN 1055-7903. PMID 29545109. S2CID 4589398.
  4. ^ Hershkovitz, P. (1949). "Mammals of northern Colombia. Preliminary report No. 4: Monkeys (primates), with taxonomic revisions of some forms". Proceedings of the United States National Museum. 98 (3232): 1–323. doi:10.5479/si.00963801.98-3232.323.
  5. ^ Hershkovitz, P. (1955). "Notes on American monkeys of the genus Cebus". Journal of Mammalogy. 36 (3): 449–452. doi:10.2307/1375688. JSTOR 1375688.
  6. ^ Torres de Assumpcao, C. (1983). An ecological study of the primates of southeastern Brazil, with a reappraisal of Cebus apella races (PhD). University of Edinburgh.
  7. ^ Torres de Assumpcao, C. (1988). "Resultados preliminares de reavaliacao das racas do macaco-prego Cebus apella (Primates: Cebidae)". Revista Nordestina de Biologia. 6: 15–28.
  8. ^ Silva, J.S. Jr. (2001). Especiacao nos macacos-pregos e caiararas, género Cebus Erxleben, 1777 (Primates, Cebidae) (PhD). Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro.
  9. ^ Lynch Alfaro, J.W.; et al. (2011). (PDF). Journal of Biogeography. 39 (2): 272–288. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2699.2011.02609.x. S2CID 13791283. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 February 2015.
  10. ^ Lynch Alfaro, J.W.; Silva, J.S. Jr.; Rylands, A.B. (2012). "How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus". American Journal of Primatology. 74 (4): 1–14. doi:10.1002/ajp.22007. PMID 22328205. S2CID 18840598.
  11. ^ Madden, M.; Fragaszy, D.M.; Izar, P.; Hinley, A.J.; Ottoni, E.B.; Visalberghi, E. (2007). (PDF). Geospatial Today (5): 26–35. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 July 2011.
  12. ^ Discovery "Life" series, "Challenges of Life" episode, David Attenborough
  13. ^ Waga, I.C.; Dacier, A.K.; Pinha, P.S.; Tavares, M.C.H. (2005). "Spontaneous tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the Cerrado". Folia Primatologica. 77 (5): 337–344. doi:10.1159/000093698. PMID 16912501. S2CID 19546828.
  14. ^ https://sta.uwi.edu/fst/lifesciences/sites/default/files/lifesciences/images/Cebus_apella%20-%20Tufted%20Capuchin.pdf[bare URL PDF]
  15. ^ a b c "Cebus apella (Brown capuchin)".
  16. ^ Di Bitetti, M.S. (2005). "Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus". Animal Behaviour. 69 (4): 911–919. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.021. S2CID 53178841.
  17. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C. (1995). "The subsistence technology of capuchins". International Journal of Primatology. 16 (1): 899–906. doi:10.1007/BF02736075. S2CID 7703148.
  18. ^ a b Ottoni, E.; Dogo de Resende, B.; Izar, P. (2005). "Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills". Animal Cognition. 8 (4): 215–219. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8. PMID 15719240. S2CID 51862020.
  19. ^ Westergaard, Gregory Charles (1999). "Structural analysis of tool-use by tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Animal Cognition. 2 (3): 141–145. doi:10.1007/s100710050034. S2CID 40181377.
  20. ^ van Schaik, C.; D.R; M.M. (1999). "The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture" (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 36 (6): 719–741. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0304. PMID 10330335.
  21. ^ a b Cummins-Sebree, S.E.; Fragaszy, D.M. (2005). Capuchins as stone-knappers?: an evaluation of the evidence. Stone knapping: the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour. Cambridge, England: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. p. 171. ISBN 978-1-902937-34-2.
  22. ^ Costello, M.; Fragaszy, D. (1988). "Prehension in Cebus and Saimiri: Grip type and hand preference". American Journal of Primatology. 15 (3): 235–245. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350150306. PMID 31968893. S2CID 86556774.
  23. ^ Rilling, J.; Insel, T.R. (August 1999). "The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging". Journal of Human Evolution. 37 (2): 191–223. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0313. PMID 10444351.
  24. ^ Dunbar, R. (1998). "The social brain hypothesis". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 6 (5): 178–190. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-8. S2CID 9555943.
  25. ^ Izar, P. (2004). "Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in a southeastern Atlantic forest". Behaviour. 141 (1): 71–99. doi:10.1163/156853904772746619.
  26. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Fragaszy, D. (1995). "The behaviour of capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, with novel food: the role of social context". Animal Behaviour. 49 (4): 1089–1095. doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0137. S2CID 53191563.
  27. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C.; Fragaszy, D. (1987). "The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 101 (2): 159–168. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.101.2.159.
  28. ^ a b Moura, A.C.; Lee, PC (2004). "Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest". Science. 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. PMID 15591195. S2CID 36793833.
  29. ^ a b c Mannu, M.; Ottoni, E. (2009). "The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (3): 242–251. doi:10.1002/ajp.20642. PMID 19051323. S2CID 3479020.
  30. ^ a b Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579. S2CID 16222308.
  31. ^ Moura, A.C. de A.; Lee, P.C. (2004). "Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest". Science. 306 (5703): 1909. doi:10.1126/science.1102558. PMID 15591195. S2CID 36793833.
  32. ^ Ottoni, E.B.; Mannu, M. (2001). "Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts". International Journal of Primatology. 22 (3): 347–358. doi:10.1023/A:1010747426841. S2CID 29768277.
  33. ^ Visalberghi, E.; et al. (2005). "Terrestriality and tool use". Science. 308 (5724): 951–952. doi:10.1126/science.308.5724.951c. PMID 15890860. S2CID 29819009.
  34. ^ a b Westergaard, G.C.; Suomi, S.J. (December 1995). "The stone tools of capuchins (Cebus apella)". International Journal of Primatology. 16 (6): 1017–1024. doi:10.1007/BF02696114. S2CID 33211414.
  35. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Trinca, L. (1989). "Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding". Primates. 30 (4): 511–521. doi:10.1007/BF02380877. S2CID 19285572.
  36. ^ Fragaszy, D.; E. Visalberghi (1989). "Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 103 (2): 159–170. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.159. PMID 2736909.
  37. ^ a b Custance, D.; Whiten, A.; Fredman, T. (1999). "Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113 (1): 13–22. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.13.
  38. ^ Dindo, M.; Thierry first 2=B.; Whiten, A. (2008). "Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 275 (1631): 187–93. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1318. PMC 2596191. PMID 17971322.
  39. ^ Dindo, M.; Whiten, A.; De Waal, F. (2009). Ferrari, P.F. (ed.). "In-group conformity sustains different foraging traditions in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". PLOS ONE. 4 (11): e7858. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.7858D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007858. PMC 2773420. PMID 19924242.
  40. ^ Visalberghi, E.; Fragaszy, D.M. (2002). "'Do monkeys ape?' - Ten years after.". In Nehaniv, C.L.; Dautenhahn, K. (eds.). Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press. p. 494. ISBN 978-0-262-04203-1.
  41. ^ Hunt, G. (1996). "Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows". Nature. 379 (6562): 249–251. Bibcode:1996Natur.379..249H. doi:10.1038/379249a0. S2CID 4352835.
  42. ^ Cummins-Sebree, S.E.; Fragaszy, D. (2005). "Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 119 (9): 210–219. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.210. PMID 15982164.
  43. ^ a b Visalberghi, E.; Limognelli, L. (1994). "Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 108 (1): 511–521. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15. PMID 8174341.
  44. ^ Ceballos-Mago, N.; Lynch Alfaro, J.W. (2020). "Sapajus apella ssp. margaritae". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2020: e.T4079A70611951. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-3.RLTS.T4079A70611951.en. Retrieved 18 November 2021.

Further reading

  • Janson, Charles H. (2001) "Capuchin-like Monkeys". in: MacDonald, D. (red.), The New Encyclopedia of Mammals, Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 344–353.
  • Nowak, R.M. (1999) Walker's Primates of the World. Baltimore & London: the Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 113–115.

External links

  • Information about tufted capuchins at Animal Diversity Web
  • Primate Info Net Cebus apella Factsheet
  • Associação Mãe-da-lua

tufted, capuchin, tufted, capuchin, sapajus, apella, also, known, brown, capuchin, black, capped, capuchin, monkey, world, primate, from, south, america, caribbean, islands, trinidad, margarita, traditionally, defined, most, widespread, primates, neotropics, r. The tufted capuchin Sapajus apella also known as brown capuchin black capped capuchin or pin monkey is a New World primate from South America and the Caribbean islands of Trinidad and Margarita As traditionally defined it is one of the most widespread primates in the Neotropics but it has recently been recommended considering the black striped black and golden bellied capuchins as separate species in a new genus thereby effectively limiting the tufted capuchin to the Amazon basin and nearby regions 1 However the large headed capuchin S a macrocephalus previously defined as a distinct species has been reclassified as a subspecies of the tufted capuchin expanding its range east to Peru amp Ecuador and south to Bolivia 3 Tufted capuchin 1 Conservation statusLeast Concern IUCN 3 1 2 Scientific classificationKingdom AnimaliaPhylum ChordataClass MammaliaOrder PrimatesSuborder HaplorhiniInfraorder SimiiformesFamily CebidaeGenus SapajusSpecies S apellaBinomial nameSapajus apella Linnaeus 1758 SubspeciesSapajus apella macrocephalusGeographic range following taxonomy used by IUCNRange of subspecies macrocephalusSynonymsCebus apella Linnaeus 1758 Simia apella Linnaeus 1758 Simia fatuellus Linnaeus 1766The tufted capuchin is an omnivorous animal mostly feeding on fruits and invertebrates although it sometimes feeds on small vertebrates e g lizards and bird chicks and other plant parts It can be found in many different kinds of environment including moist tropical and subtropical forest dry forest and disturbed or secondary forest Like other capuchins it is a social animal forming groups of 8 to 15 individuals that are led by an alpha or dominant male Contents 1 Taxonomy and phylogeny 2 Physical characteristics 3 Behaviour and ecology 3 1 Diet 3 2 Tool use and manufacture 3 3 Problem solving 4 Distribution and habitat 5 References 6 Further reading 7 External linksTaxonomy and phylogeny EditAt one point all tufted capuchins were classified as Cebus apella 4 5 Under such taxonomy the range of C apella would extend throughout much of South America from Colombia to northern Argentina Although she didn t describe specific or subspecific nomenclature Torres de Assumpcao 1983 1988 described differences between tufted capuchins from five distinct geographic regions of Brazil and high phenotypic variation of individuals in a sixth area that had a greater selection pressure 6 7 In 2001 Silva Junior proposed that the robust capuchins such formerly the C apella group be placed in a separate genus Sapajus from the gracile capuchins formerly the C capucinus group which retain the genus Cebus 8 This was supported by Jessica Lynch Alfaro et al in 2011 9 10 Groves 2005 recognized six subspecies Cebus apella apella C a fatuellus C a macrocephalus C a margaritae C a peruanus C a tocantinus 1 The IUCN follows Silva 2001 and recognise the species as monotypic though the subspecies status of S a margaritae is unclear 2 Physical characteristics Edit Skeleton The tufted capuchin is more powerfully built than the other capuchins with rougher fur and a long thick tail It has a bundle of long hardened hair on the forehead that can be raised as a sort of wig The fur is brownish gray with the belly being somewhat lighter colored than the rest of the body The hands and feet are black The tail is prehensile strong and can be used for grasping as an extra limb The tufted capuchin has a head body length of 32 to 57 centimetres 13 to 22 in a tail length of 38 to 56 centimetres 15 to 22 in and a weight of 1 9 to 4 8 kilograms 4 2 to 10 6 lb with the males generally being larger and heavier than the females Behaviour and ecology Edit Male tufted capuchin The tufted capuchin is a diurnal arboreal primate species but it often forages on the ground to search for food or to walk longer distances between trees that are too far apart to jump The tufted capuchin lives in groups of two to twenty or more animals A single group usually contains at least one adult male but mixed groups with multiple males do also occur In that case one of the males is dominant He accepts only a few monkeys in his direct surroundings mainly younger animals and a few females The dominant male and the group members that are close to him have the privilege to eat first in case of food scarcity while subordinate monkeys have to wait until they are ready After a gestation period of 180 days one young is born or incidentally a twin This young which weighs only 200 to 250 grams is carried on the back of its mother The mother feeds her child for 9 months but the young are sexually immature until its seventh year which is quite late for a primate of its size Important natural enemies of the capuchin are large birds of prey They are so afraid of those birds that they even become alarmed when a harmless bird flies over Diet Edit A recently discovered characteristic of one population of this species is that it uses stones as a tool to open hard nuts First it chooses ripe nuts from a nut palm It uses its teeth to strip off the nut s fibrous husk Then it leaves the nut to dry for about a week When the nut is dry the monkey lays the nut on a large flat rock or fallen tree 11 hammering the nut with a suitable stone until the nut cracks The hammerstones are often large enough to require lifting with both hands The anvil rock is often pockmarked with hollows as a result of repeated use 12 13 Besides nuts the capuchin also eats fruit leaves nuts 14 seeds pith 15 insects and larvae eggs and young birds frogs lizards other reptiles 15 rodents mouse opossums 15 and even bats They are also known to chase cats The tufted capuchin looks for its food in groups As soon as one of the group members has found something edible he or she may make a large whistling sound dependent upon the proximity of other individuals and abundance of the food resource so that the other monkeys know that there is something to eat 16 The composition of the group is very well organized and is determined by rank in the hierarchy The dominant male often resides somewhere in the middle of the group just behind the front line so that it is safer when a predator attacks The vanguard is composed of higher ranked females who are tolerated by the dominant male They have the privilege to reach the food first but they are also the most vulnerable when a predator attacks Tool use and manufacture Edit The tufted capuchin has been observed using containers to hold water using sticks to dig nuts to dip for syrup to catch ants to reach food using sponges to absorb juice using stones as hammer and chisel to penetrate a barrier 17 and using stones as hammer and anvil to crack nuts 18 While some of these tasks are relatively simple by cognitive standards e g using a stick to catch ants others like cracking nuts with hammer and anvil are only exceeded in complexity by chimpanzees 19 The potential for tool use in animals like the tufted capuchin depends on a number of conditions that would increase its likelihood of appearing in a given species Van Schaik proposed that the occurrence of tool use would be likely in foraging species if three factors were present manual dexterity intelligence and social tolerance 20 As it applies to manual dexterity capuchins are capable of a limited precision grip the ability to delicately pinch and manipulate objects with the thumb and fingertips which is not found in any other New World monkeys and only found in limited amounts in apes 21 22 C apella has an encephalization ratio greater than the hominids except humans and a neocortex ratio that is almost as large as the apes both of these rough indicators suggest high intelligence 23 24 Finally the tufted capuchin forms social groups typical of a complex and tolerant society 25 26 The tufted capuchin has been observed manufacturing tools both in captivity and in the wild In captivity it has been reported as making probing sticks to reach normally inaccessible containers with syrup 27 It is also capable of understanding the concept of sponging and using paper towels monkey biscuits sticks leaves and straw to sop up juice and then suck on the sponge to consume the juice 27 Research in the wild has shown that capuchin tool use is every bit as extensive as in captivity with capuchins being observed using stones to dig holes to get at tubers an activity previously only seen in humans 28 The practice of using stones to crack nuts has arisen spontaneously in many locations such as in the Caatinga Dry Forest 28 and Serra da Capivara National Park 29 all in Brazil and hundreds of miles apart It has been observed cracking various nuts and fruits such as palm nuts Attalea and Astrocaryum spp 30 and jatoba fruits Hymenaea courbaril 29 The tufted capuchin has even been observed using stones to dislodge other stones that would later be used as hammers or shovels an example of a more complex tool using behavior known as second order tool use previously only found in chimpanzees 29 Curiously not all tufted capuchins engage in tool use Moura and Lee 2004 31 suggest lack of other food sources as the key factor Ottoni and Mannu 2001 32 Fragaszy et al 2004 30 and Visalberghi et al 2005 33 have proposed this is likely more a factor of a monkey s terrestrial habit the more time a monkey spends on the ground the more likely it is to profit from and thus engage in tool use In captivity the tufted capuchin has been seen to manufacture stone tools that produced simple flakes and cores Some of the capuchins even used these sharpened stones to cut in a back and forth motion barriers in order to reach food 34 The importance of this behavior is that it serves as evidence of mechanical proclivity to modify stones by using behaviors already in the monkeys repertoires and this behavior is seen as a precursor to stone knapping 21 This early and limited tool use behavior has been hypothesized as similar to pre Homo habilis and that artifacts of that time would probably resemble those of capuchins 34 S apella tool manufacture and use has been analyzed for potential clues to social learning and problem solving ability as tool manufacture and use can often shed light on such complex cognitive abilities 17 35 Social learning or the ability to learn from other individuals is a controversial topic in most nonhuman species like S apella because of the relative difficulty of determining whether a behavior was learned from imitation or a much simpler form of social learning 36 One way of closing the gap between concurrent tool related behaviors and their likelihood of arising from imitation is by narrowing down events that would make social learning more probable such as a preference for observing experienced tool users In this regard Ottoni and his team found that young capuchins tended to observe the best tool users when cracking nuts 18 An example of the Doorian Fruit a box that can open in one of two ways see image for more info Another way of isolating imitation from other simpler behaviors is to present the capuchins with a box that has food but has two different ways of opening it The important point is that neither way should be more advantageous so that the monkey can freely choose one In one such study when humans opened the door in front of the monkeys using one way only the monkeys used that method even when they discovered the alternative on their own 37 In another study capuchin alphas from two separate groups were trained to open the door in a specific way after which the monkeys were paired with subordinates who learned to open the door in the same way 38 When capuchins are trained in the same way and this time released into their groups the habit is once again disseminated amongst all group members even when others discover alternative ways 39 Nevertheless the subject of whether or not S apella learns by imitation is still controversial because of the inherent difficulty in teasing out unambiguous evidence of a complex cognitive process such as imitation 37 40 Problem solving Edit Tool use and manufacture can also shed light on the many aspects of the tufted capuchin s cognitive abilities by determining how it solves some problems Some non primates manufacture and use objects as tools Crows are known to make hook tools for catching insects 41 but such activities lack the behavioral plasticity of tool use as evidenced in tufted capuchins who found new ways to use tools that other species could not 42 But this plasticity in tool use while suggesting greater complexity and cognitive ability does not suggest that the monkeys understand cause and effect It instead implies they are only able to learn from successful efforts but not from failures nor are they able to refine and improve much 43 Its ability to repeat successes coupled with its complex repertoire of behavioral events helps to explain the tufted capuchin s extensive repertoire of innovative behaviors besides tool use 43 Distribution and habitat EditThis species lives in the northern Amazon rainforest of the Guyanas Venezuela and Brazil and to the west of the Rio Negro as far north as the Orinoco in Venezuela It is also found in eastern Colombia Ecuador Bolivia Peru including the upper Andean Magdalena valley in Colombia An introduced breeding population is well established in the northwestern peninsula of the island of Trinidad in the Republic of Trinidad and Tobago The subspecies population on Margarita Island in Venezuela S a margaritae is considered Critically Endangered by the IUCN Red List 44 It can be found in a large variety of forest types mainly in tropical rainforests up till a height of 2700 m but also in more open forests The distribution overlaps with that of other species of capuchins such as the white fronted capuchin Cebus albifrons References Edit a b c Groves C P 2005 Wilson D E Reeder D M eds Mammal Species of the World A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference 3rd ed Baltimore Johns Hopkins University Press p 137 ISBN 0 801 88221 4 OCLC 62265494 a b Boubli J P Stevenson P R Palacios E de la Torre S Ravetta A L Messias M R Carvalho A S amp Mittermeier R A 2021 Sapajus apella amended version of 2020 assessment The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2021 e T172351505A192594550 https dx doi org 10 2305 IUCN UK 2021 1 RLTS T172351505A192594550 en Downloaded on 26 August 2021 Lima Marcela G M Silva Junior Jose de Sousa e Cerny David Buckner Janet C Aleixo Alexandre Chang Jonathan Zheng Jimmy Alfaro Michael E Martins Amely Di Fiore Anthony Boubli Jean P 1 July 2018 A phylogenomic perspective on the robust capuchin monkey Sapajus radiation First evidence for extensive population admixture across South America Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 124 137 150 doi 10 1016 j ympev 2018 02 023 ISSN 1055 7903 PMID 29545109 S2CID 4589398 Hershkovitz P 1949 Mammals of northern Colombia Preliminary report No 4 Monkeys primates with taxonomic revisions of some forms Proceedings of the United States National Museum 98 3232 1 323 doi 10 5479 si 00963801 98 3232 323 Hershkovitz P 1955 Notes on American monkeys of the genus Cebus Journal of Mammalogy 36 3 449 452 doi 10 2307 1375688 JSTOR 1375688 Torres de Assumpcao C 1983 An ecological study of the primates of southeastern Brazil with a reappraisal ofCebus apellaraces PhD University of Edinburgh Torres de Assumpcao C 1988 Resultados preliminares de reavaliacao das racas do macaco prego Cebus apella Primates Cebidae Revista Nordestina de Biologia 6 15 28 Silva J S Jr 2001 Especiacao nos macacos pregos e caiararas generoCebusErxleben 1777 Primates Cebidae PhD Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro Lynch Alfaro J W et al 2011 Explosive Pleistocene range expansion leads to widespread Amazonian sympatry between robust and gracile capuchin monkeys PDF Journal of Biogeography 39 2 272 288 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2699 2011 02609 x S2CID 13791283 Archived from the original PDF on 26 February 2015 Lynch Alfaro J W Silva J S Jr Rylands A B 2012 How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys An argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus American Journal of Primatology 74 4 1 14 doi 10 1002 ajp 22007 PMID 22328205 S2CID 18840598 Madden M Fragaszy D M Izar P Hinley A J Ottoni E B Visalberghi E 2007 Modelling the monkey habitat in Brazil PDF Geospatial Today 5 26 35 Archived from the original PDF on 17 July 2011 Discovery Life series Challenges of Life episode David Attenborough Waga I C Dacier A K Pinha P S Tavares M C H 2005 Spontaneous tool use by wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus in the Cerrado Folia Primatologica 77 5 337 344 doi 10 1159 000093698 PMID 16912501 S2CID 19546828 https sta uwi edu fst lifesciences sites default files lifesciences images Cebus apella 20 20Tufted 20Capuchin pdf bare URL PDF a b c Cebus apella Brown capuchin Di Bitetti M S 2005 Food associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys Cebus apella nigritus Animal Behaviour 69 4 911 919 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2004 05 021 S2CID 53178841 a b Westergaard G C 1995 The subsistence technology of capuchins International Journal of Primatology 16 1 899 906 doi 10 1007 BF02736075 S2CID 7703148 a b Ottoni E Dogo de Resende B Izar P 2005 Watching the best nutcrackers what capuchin monkeys Cebus apella know about others tool using skills Animal Cognition 8 4 215 219 doi 10 1007 s10071 004 0245 8 PMID 15719240 S2CID 51862020 Westergaard Gregory Charles 1999 Structural analysis of tool use by tufted capuchins Cebus apella and chimpanzees Pan troglodytes Animal Cognition 2 3 141 145 doi 10 1007 s100710050034 S2CID 40181377 van Schaik C D R M M 1999 The conditions for tool use in primates implications for the evolution of material culture PDF Journal of Human Evolution 36 6 719 741 doi 10 1006 jhev 1999 0304 PMID 10330335 a b Cummins Sebree S E Fragaszy D M 2005 Capuchins as stone knappers an evaluation of the evidence Stone knapping the necessary conditions for a uniquely hominin behaviour Cambridge England McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research p 171 ISBN 978 1 902937 34 2 Costello M Fragaszy D 1988 Prehension in Cebus and Saimiri Grip type and hand preference American Journal of Primatology 15 3 235 245 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350150306 PMID 31968893 S2CID 86556774 Rilling J Insel T R August 1999 The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging Journal of Human Evolution 37 2 191 223 doi 10 1006 jhev 1999 0313 PMID 10444351 Dunbar R 1998 The social brain hypothesis Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 6 5 178 190 doi 10 1002 SICI 1520 6505 1998 6 5 lt 178 AID EVAN5 gt 3 0 CO 2 8 S2CID 9555943 Izar P 2004 Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in a southeastern Atlantic forest Behaviour 141 1 71 99 doi 10 1163 156853904772746619 Visalberghi E Fragaszy D 1995 The behaviour of capuchin monkeys Cebus apella with novel food the role of social context Animal Behaviour 49 4 1089 1095 doi 10 1006 anbe 1995 0137 S2CID 53191563 a b Westergaard G C Fragaszy D 1987 The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 101 2 159 168 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 101 2 159 a b Moura A C Lee PC 2004 Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest Science 306 5703 1909 doi 10 1126 science 1102558 PMID 15591195 S2CID 36793833 a b c Mannu M Ottoni E 2009 The enhanced tool kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga tool making associative use and secondary tools American Journal of Primatology 71 3 242 251 doi 10 1002 ajp 20642 PMID 19051323 S2CID 3479020 a b Fragaszy D Izar P Visalberghi E Ottoni E B de Oliveira M G 2004 Wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus use anvils and stone pounding tools American Journal of Primatology 64 4 359 366 doi 10 1002 ajp 20085 PMID 15580579 S2CID 16222308 Moura A C de A Lee P C 2004 Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest Science 306 5703 1909 doi 10 1126 science 1102558 PMID 15591195 S2CID 36793833 Ottoni E B Mannu M 2001 Semifree ranging tufted capuchins Cebus apella spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts International Journal of Primatology 22 3 347 358 doi 10 1023 A 1010747426841 S2CID 29768277 Visalberghi E et al 2005 Terrestriality and tool use Science 308 5724 951 952 doi 10 1126 science 308 5724 951c PMID 15890860 S2CID 29819009 a b Westergaard G C Suomi S J December 1995 The stone tools of capuchins Cebus apella International Journal of Primatology 16 6 1017 1024 doi 10 1007 BF02696114 S2CID 33211414 Visalberghi E Trinca L 1989 Tool use in capuchin monkeys Distinguishing between performing and understanding Primates 30 4 511 521 doi 10 1007 BF02380877 S2CID 19285572 Fragaszy D E Visalberghi 1989 Social influences on the acquisition of tool using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 103 2 159 170 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 103 2 159 PMID 2736909 a b Custance D Whiten A Fredman T 1999 Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 113 1 13 22 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 113 1 13 Dindo M Thierry first 2 B Whiten A 2008 Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Proceedings of the Royal Society B 275 1631 187 93 doi 10 1098 rspb 2007 1318 PMC 2596191 PMID 17971322 Dindo M Whiten A De Waal F 2009 Ferrari P F ed In group conformity sustains different foraging traditions in capuchin monkeys Cebus apella PLOS ONE 4 11 e7858 Bibcode 2009PLoSO 4 7858D doi 10 1371 journal pone 0007858 PMC 2773420 PMID 19924242 Visalberghi E Fragaszy D M 2002 Do monkeys ape Ten years after In Nehaniv C L Dautenhahn K eds Imitation in animals and artifacts MIT Press p 494 ISBN 978 0 262 04203 1 Hunt G 1996 Manufacture and use of hook tools by New Caledonian crows Nature 379 6562 249 251 Bibcode 1996Natur 379 249H doi 10 1038 379249a0 S2CID 4352835 Cummins Sebree S E Fragaszy D 2005 Choosing and using tools Capuchins Cebus apella use a different metric than tamarins Saguinus oedipus Journal of Comparative Psychology 119 9 210 219 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 119 2 210 PMID 15982164 a b Visalberghi E Limognelli L 1994 Lack of comprehension of cause effect relations in tool using capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 108 1 511 521 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 108 1 15 PMID 8174341 Ceballos Mago N Lynch Alfaro J W 2020 Sapajus apella ssp margaritae IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020 e T4079A70611951 doi 10 2305 IUCN UK 2020 3 RLTS T4079A70611951 en Retrieved 18 November 2021 Further reading EditNotes on the taxonomy and distributions of the tufted capuchin monkeys Cebus Cebidae of South America Janson Charles H 2001 Capuchin like Monkeys in MacDonald D red The New Encyclopedia of Mammals Oxford Oxford University Press p 344 353 Nowak R M 1999 Walker s Primates of the World Baltimore amp London the Johns Hopkins University Press p 113 115 External links Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to Cebus apella Wikispecies has information related to Tufted caphuchin Information about tufted capuchins at Animal Diversity Web Primate Info Net Cebus apella Factsheet Associacao Mae da lua Brown Capuchin Cebus apella Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Tufted capuchin amp oldid 1114053018, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.