fbpx
Wikipedia

Brodmann area 45

Brodmann area 45 (BA45), is part of the frontal cortex in the human brain. It is situated on the lateral surface, inferior to BA9 and adjacent to BA46.

Brodmann area 45
Details
Part ofCerebral cortex of frontal lobe in the human brain
Arterymiddle cerebral artery
Identifiers
LatinArea triangularis
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1777
FMA68642
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
[edit on Wikidata]

This area in humans occupies the triangular part of inferior frontal gyrus (H) and, surrounding the anterior horizontal limb of the lateral sulcus (H), a portion of the orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus (H). Bounded caudally by the anterior ascending limb of the lateral sulcus (H), it borders on the insula in the depth of the lateral sulcus.

In terms of cytoarchitecture, it is bounded caudally by the opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 44 (BA44)), rostrodorsally by the middle frontal area 46 (BA46), and ventrally by the orbital part of inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann area 47 BA47).

Functions edit

The left-hemisphere Brodmann area 44 and Brodmann area 45 make up Broca's area, a region that is active in semantic tasks,[1] such as semantic decision tasks (determining whether a word represents an abstract or a concrete entity) and generation tasks (generating a verb associated with a noun).

The precise role of BA45 in semantic tasks remains controversial. For some researchers, its role would be to subserve semantic retrieval or semantic working memory processes. Under this view, BA44 and BA45 would together guide recovery of semantic information and evaluate the recovered information with regard to the criterion appropriate to a given context.[2][3] A slightly modified account of this view is that activation of BA45 is needed only under controlled semantic retrieval, when strong stimulus-stimulus associations are absent.[4] For other researchers, BA45's role is not restricted to semantics per se, but to all activities that require task-relevant representations from among competing representations.[5]Lesions of the BA45 lead to the characteristic findings of expressive aphasia in individuals who are left hemispheric dominant.

Research findings edit

Asymmetry and language dominance edit

 
Triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus. Shown in red.

A strong correlation has been found between speech-language and the anatomically asymmetric pars triangularis. Foundas, et al. showed that language function can be localized to one region of the brain, as Paul Broca had done before them, but they also supported the idea that one side of the brain is more involved with language than the other. The human brain has two hemispheres, and each one looks similar to the other; that is, it looks like one hemisphere is a mirror image of the other. However, Foundas, et al. found that the pars triangularis in Broca's area is actually larger than the same region in the right side of the brain. This "leftward asymmetry" corresponded both in form and function, which means that the part of the brain that is active during language processing is larger. In almost all the test subjects, this was the left side. In fact, the only subject tested that had right-hemispheric language dominance was found to have a rightward asymmetry of the pars triangularis.[6]

Certain other researchers, however, have found no volumetric asymmetries in the pars triangularis. They have challenged previous findings that pars triangularis asymmetry exists and have suggested that inconsistencies in previous findings may be due to great variability in inter-individual pars triangularis morphology. That is, these regions tend to vary in size and shape much more than other areas of the brain, such as deep cortical nuclei. Furthermore, while these researcher found statistically significant asymmetries in the pars opercularis and the planum temporale, they found no correlations between asymmetries of these brain regions with that of the pars triangularis.[7]

Connections in inferior frontal gyrus edit

At least one study demonstrated a high degree of connectivity between the three subregions of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). By stimulating one region of the IFG and measuring the response in distinct regions, these researchers were able to demonstrate the existence of numerous pathways between the pars triangularis and pars opercularis. Also, stimulation of one region of the pars triangularis elicited a response in distinct regions of the pars triangularis, illustrating the presence of networks within the subgyral region. [8]

Additionally, The pars triangularis was implicated in semantic processing of language. By measuring the response of the brain by electroencephalography as it responded to different sentence types (those with or without semantic errors), Maess et al. demonstrated a time-lag in the comprehension of erroneous sentences. To understand this one would only need to imagine a person being told something they did not understand. They would pause and take a moment to process the information. Furthermore, these researchers demonstrated a characteristic processing pattern called an "N400", which refers to a negativity that appears in the pars triangularis about 400 ms after the syntactic mismatch is presented.[9] However, the pars triangularis is likely to be only part of the network generating the N400 response in EEG since the magnetic counterpart N400m measured using MEG has been consistently localized to the superior temporal cortex.[10]

Prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory edit

Pars triangularis has been shown to have a role in cognitive control of memory. There are more ways than one to remember something. When a person remembers, (s)he retrieves information from storage in a memory center of the brain. This information may be the muscle contraction sequence for shoe-tying, the face of a loved one, or anything in between. When someone remembers something automatically, without concentrating on it and without trying, it is called "bottom-up" processing. But sometimes, people really have to struggle to remember something. A student taking a test and trying to remember the answer to a question is concentrating their attention on retrieving the memory. The student is exhibiting cognitive control over their memory. This type of processing is directed, in part, by the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC). Pars triangularis is found in this region.[11]

When reading aloud, people must decode written language to decipher its pronunciation. This processing takes place in Broca's area. The reader might use previous knowledge of a word in order to correctly vocalize it, or the reader might use knowledge of systematic letter combinations, which represent corresponding phonemes. Scientists can learn about what the brain is doing while people process language by looking at what it does with errors in language. As above, scientists can investigate the extra processing that occurs when people are challenged with a problem. In this case, scientists took advantage of the way pseudo-words and exception words by examining the brain as it interprets these problematic words. When people process language, they use different parts of Broca's area for different things. Pars triangularis is involved in a specific type of language processing. Specifically, pars triangularis becomes activated when people read exception words, which are words with atypical spelling-to-sound relationships. For example, "have" is an exception word because it is pronounced with a short "a", which is contrary to grammatical rules of pronunciation. The "e" at the end of the word should lead to the pronunciation of the long "a" sound, as in "cave" or "rave". Because we are so familiar with the word "have", we are able to remember its pronunciation, and we don't have to think through the rules each time we read it. Pars triangularis helps us do that.[12]

When trying to retrieve information in a top-down fashion, some kind of control mechanism is necessary. Recalling that top-down retrieval depends on conscious control, it is easy to see that there must be some way to exclude irrelevant data from the retrieval. In order to home in on the desired information, some selection must occur. This selection is thought to occur post-retrieval in the mid-VLPFC, which corresponds generally to the location of pars triangularis. The theory here is that information is retrieved by certain regions of the left VLPFC, and then it is selected for relevance in another region. This is called the "two part" model of memory retrieval.[13]

Almost every person in the world has learned at least one language. Also, almost everyone that has learned a language has learned it at a young age. Some people are multilingual. Some of these multilingual have learned second or third languages in concert with their first, at a young age, and some have learned other languages in their adulthood. Studies on different subsets of monolinguals and multilinguals have revealed some interesting findings.[citation needed]

By looking at the similarities between the first and second language and what they do to the brain, these researchers found that brain activation looked very different depending on which language the test subjects were processing. They found that pars triangularis activation changes during processing of these different languages, which is understandable considering the known role of pars triangularis in language.[14]

There is a difference between the processing patterns of primary and secondary languages in processing of passive sentences. These are sentences using some form of the verb "be" with a verb in the past participle form. For example, "He is ruined" is a passive sentence because the verb "ruin" is in the past participle form and used with "is", which is a form of the verb "be". This study shows that processing this sentence, late bilinguals used their pars triangularis much more than their counterparts. This result implies certain things about the way language is learned. For example, It has been suggested that the reason people often have such difficulty learning foreign languages during adulthood is that their brains are trying to code language information in a region of the brain that is not dedicated to understanding language. According to this view, this is the reason native speakers are able to speak so quickly while their late-bilingual counterparts are forced to stutter as they struggle to process grammatical rules.[15]

Cortical dynamics of word recognition edit

There is a theory that pars triangularis is especially involved in semantic processing of language, as opposed to phonological processing. That is, pars triangularis is thought to be more involved in deciphering the meaning of words rather than trying to decide what the word is based on the sound that goes into the ear. This study got data that supported this theory. Furthermore, these researchers saw evidence for parallel semantic processing, which occurs when the brain multitasks. When their subjects were undergoing experimentation, they were presented with consonant strings, pseudo-words, and words, and the delay between stimulus and brain activity was about the same for phonological and semantic processing, even though the two seemed to occur in slightly different regions.[16]

In the study "Semantic Encoding and Retrieval in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex: A Functional magnetic resonance imaging Study of Task Difficulty and Process Specificity", researchers found that pars triangularis (as well as some of its neighbors) increased its activity during semantic encoding, regardless of difficulty of the word being processed. This is consistent with the theory that pars triangularis is involved in semantic processing more than phonological processing. Furthermore, they found that these semantic encoding decisions resulted in less involvement of pars triangularis with repetition of the used words. It may seem intuitive that practice would make the brain better at recognizing the words as they reappeared, but there is something else to be learned from this result, as well. That pars triangularis activity went down with repetition also signifies the movement of the task of recognizing the word from the conscious to the passive. This is called repetition priming, and it occurs independent of intention. This idea, when paired with theories about pt's involvement in conscious retrieval of memory, serves to illustrate the complexity of the brain and its functions. These results together imply the possibility that similar mechanics are required for encoding and retrieving information. Another point of interest was that decreased pars triangularis activation with repetition did not occur with redundant presentation of nonsemantically processed words.[17]

On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework edit

A person is highly interconnected with other regions of the brain, especially those in the left frontal language network. Though its function seems to be distinct from its neighbors, this high degree of connectivity supports the idea that language can be integrated into many of the seemingly unrelated thought processes we have. This is not a difficult idea to imagine. For instance, attempting to remember the name of a brand new acquaintance can be challenging, and it often demands the attention of the person doing the remembering. In this example, a person is trying to comprehend sound as a part of language, place the word they just heard in the category "names", while associating it also as a tag for the face they just saw, simultaneously committing all of these pieces of data to memory. In this view, it hardly seems far-fetched that the roles of pars triangularis in language processing, semantic comprehension, and conscious control of memory are unrelated. In fact, it would be unlikely for pars triangularis not to have multiple roles in the brain, especially considering its high degree of connectivity, both within the left frontal language center, and to other regions.[18]

Schizophrenia and Broca area edit

Schizophrenia is a poorly understood disease with complicated symptoms. In an effort to find a cause for this problem, these researchers looked at the brains of schizophrenic patients. It had been shown previously that abnormal gyrification, asymmetry, complexity, and variability occur in patients with schizophrenia. These investigators presented data showing that pt, specifically was highly distorted in schizophrenic patients compared with demographically matched normal subjects. They asserted that Broca's area is an especially plastic region of the brain in that its morphology can change dramatically from childhood to adulthood. This makes sense when considering the special ability of children to easily learn language, but it also means that the involvement of Broca's area is limited with respect to memory and recall; children do not seem to be unable to consciously search their memories. Furthermore, investigators took volumetric measurements of the grey and white matter of the brains of their test subjects and compared those measurements to their normal control subjects. They found that schizophrenic patients had dramatically reduced white matter.[citation needed]

As the brain develops, connectivity of different regions changes dramatically. Researchers found that there is a discrepancy in the way white matter and grey matter develop in schizophrenic patients. People with schizophrenia tend to have an absence of white matter expansion.[19]

Heuristic and analytic reasoning edit

Transcranial magnetic stimulation applied to the left BA45 facilitated incongruent reasoning performance and impaired congruent reasoning performance, suggesting that the left BA45 is a component of a belief-based heuristic system. The right BA45 involvement in blocking the heuristic system is inferred from the blocking of the left homologue and resulting facilitation of logical-analytic reasoning performance.[20]

Images edit

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Yamada, A; Sakai, KL (April 2017). "ブローカ野における文法処理" [Syntactic Processing in Broca's Area: Brodmann Areas 44 and 45]. Brain and Nerve = Shinkei Kenkyu No Shinpo (in Japanese). 69 (4): 479–487. doi:10.11477/mf.1416200767. PMID 28424402.
  2. ^ Gabrieli; et al. (1998). "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". PNAS. 95 (3): 906–913. Bibcode:1998PNAS...95..906G. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.906. PMC 33815. PMID 9448258.
  3. ^ Buckner, R. (1996). "Contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval". Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 3 (2): 149–158. doi:10.3758/BF03212413. PMID 24213862. S2CID 31024400.
  4. ^ Wagner, A. D. (2002). "Cognitive control and episodic memory: Contributions from prefrontal cortex". In Squire, Larry R.; Schacter, Daniel L. (eds.). Neuropsychology of Memory (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. pp. 174–192. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.11.7909. ISBN 978-1-57230-898-5.
  5. ^ Thompson-Schill; et al. (1999). "Effects of repetition and competition on activity of left prefrontal cortex during word generation". Neuron. 23 (3): 513–522. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80804-1. PMID 10433263. S2CID 15194595.
  6. ^ Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Gilmore RL, Fennell EB, Heilman KM (January 1996). "Pars triangularis asymmetry and language dominance". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 93 (2): 719–722. Bibcode:1996PNAS...93..719F. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.2.719. PMC 40120. PMID 8570622.
  7. ^ Keller, SS; Highley, JR; Garcia-Finana, M; Sluming, V; Rezaie, R; Roberts, N (2007). "Sulcal variability, stereological measurement and asymmetry of Broca's area on MRI images". J. Anat. 211 (4): 534–55. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2007.00793.x. PMC 2375829. PMID 17727624.
  8. ^ Jeremy D.W. Greenlee; Hiroyuki Oya; Hiroto Kawasaki; Igor O. Volkov; Meryl A. Severson III; Matthew A. Howard III; John F. Brugge (2007). "Functional connections within the human inferior frontal gyrus". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 503 (4): 550–559. doi:10.1002/cne.21405. PMID 17534935. S2CID 5685566.
  9. ^ Maess, Burkhard; Herrmann, Christoph S.; Hahne, Anja; Nakamura, Akinori; Friederici, Angela D. (2006). "Localizing the distributed language network responsible for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence processing". Brain Research. 1096 (1): 163–172. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.037. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0010-C478-0. PMID 16769041. S2CID 14551069.
  10. ^ Vartiainen, J; Parviainen, T; Salmelin, R (2009). "Spatiotemporal convergence of semantic processing in reading and speech perception". Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (29): 9271–9280. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5860-08.2009. PMC 6665572. PMID 19625517.
  11. ^ David Badre & Anthony D. Wagner (2007). "Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory". Neuropsychologia. 45 (13): 2883–2901. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.015. PMID 17675110. S2CID 16062085.
  12. ^ Mechelli, Andrea; Crinion, Jennifer T.; Long, Steven; Friston, Karl J.; Lambon Ralph, Matthew A.; Patterson, Karalyn; McClelland, James L.; Price, Cathy J. (2005). "Dissociating Reading Processes on the Basis of Neuronal Interactions" (PDF). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 17 (11): 1753–1765. doi:10.1162/089892905774589190. PMID 16269111. S2CID 9596621.
  13. ^ Badre, D.; Poldrack, R.; Pare-Blagoev, E.; Insler, R.; Wagner, A. (2005). "Dissociable Controlled Retrieval and Generalized Selection Mechanisms in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex". Neuron. 47 (6): 907–918. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.023. PMID 16157284. S2CID 5846073.
  14. ^ Jeong, Hyeonjeong; Sugiura, Motoaki; Sassa, Yuko; Haji, Tomoki; Usui, Nobuo; Taira, Masato; Horie, Kaoru; Sato, Shigeru; Kawashima, Ryuta (March 2007). "Effect of syntactic similarity on cortical activation during second language processing: A comparison of English and Japanese among native Korean trilinguals". Human Brain Mapping. 28 (3): 194–204. doi:10.1002/hbm.20269. PMC 6871317. PMID 16767768.
  15. ^ Yokoyama, Satoru; Okamoto, Hideyuki; Miyamoto, Tadao; Yoshimoto, Kei; Kim, Jungho; Iwata, Kazuki; Jeong, Hyeonjeong; Uchida, Shinya; Ikuta, Naho; Sassa, Yuko; Nakamura, Wataru; Horie, Kaoru; Sato, Shigeru; Kawashima, Ryuta (April 2006). "Cortical activation in the processing of passive sentences in L1 and L2: An fMRI study". NeuroImage. 30 (2): 570–579. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.066. PMID 16300965. S2CID 9226986.
  16. ^ Mainy, Nelly; Jung, Julien; Baciu, Monica; Kahane, Philippe; Schoendorff, Benjamin; Minotti, Lorella; Hoffmann, Dominique; Bertrand, Olivier; Lachaux, Jean-Philippe (November 2008). "Cortical dynamics of word recognition". Human Brain Mapping. 29 (11): 1215–1230. doi:10.1002/hbm.20457. PMC 6871193. PMID 17712785.
  17. ^ Demb, J.; Desmond, J.; Wagner, A.; Vaidya, C.; Glover, G.; Gabrieli, J. (1995). "Cortex: A Functional magnetic resonance imaging Study of Task Difficulty and Process Specificity". The Journal of Neuroscience. 15 (9): 5870–5878. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-09-05870.1995. PMC 6577672. PMID 7666172.
  18. ^ Hagoort, Peter (September 2005). "On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (9): 416–423. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-1E16-A. PMID 16054419. S2CID 2826729.
  19. ^ Wisco, Jonathan J.; Kuperberg, Gina; Manoach, Dara; Quinn, Brian T.; Busa, Evelina; Fischl, Bruce; Heckers, Stephan; Sorensen, A. Gregory (August 2007). "Abnormal cortical folding patterns within Broca's area in schizophrenia: Evidence from structural MRI". Schizophrenia Research. 94 (1–3): 317–327. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.031. PMC 2034662. PMID 17490861.
  20. ^ Tsujii, Takeo; Masuda, Sayako; Akiyama, Takekazu; Watanabe, Shigeru (2010). "The role of inferior frontal cortex in belief-bias reasoning: an rTMS study". Neuropsychologia. 48 (7): 2005–2008. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.021. PMID 20362600. S2CID 39839238.

External links edit

brodmann, area, ba45, part, frontal, cortex, human, brain, situated, lateral, surface, inferior, adjacent, ba46, detailspart, ofcerebral, cortex, frontal, lobe, human, brainarterymiddle, cerebral, arteryidentifierslatinarea, triangularisneurolex, idbirnlex, 17. Brodmann area 45 BA45 is part of the frontal cortex in the human brain It is situated on the lateral surface inferior to BA9 and adjacent to BA46 Brodmann area 45DetailsPart ofCerebral cortex of frontal lobe in the human brainArterymiddle cerebral arteryIdentifiersLatinArea triangularisNeuroLex IDbirnlex 1777FMA68642Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy edit on Wikidata This area in humans occupies the triangular part of inferior frontal gyrus H and surrounding the anterior horizontal limb of the lateral sulcus H a portion of the orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus H Bounded caudally by the anterior ascending limb of the lateral sulcus H it borders on the insula in the depth of the lateral sulcus In terms of cytoarchitecture it is bounded caudally by the opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus Brodmann area 44 BA44 rostrodorsally by the middle frontal area 46 BA46 and ventrally by the orbital part of inferior frontal gyrus Brodmann area 47 BA47 Contents 1 Functions 2 Research findings 2 1 Asymmetry and language dominance 2 2 Connections in inferior frontal gyrus 2 3 Prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory 2 4 Cortical dynamics of word recognition 2 5 On Broca brain and binding a new framework 2 6 Schizophrenia and Broca area 2 7 Heuristic and analytic reasoning 3 Images 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksFunctions editThe left hemisphere Brodmann area 44 and Brodmann area 45 make up Broca s area a region that is active in semantic tasks 1 such as semantic decision tasks determining whether a word represents an abstract or a concrete entity and generation tasks generating a verb associated with a noun The precise role of BA45 in semantic tasks remains controversial For some researchers its role would be to subserve semantic retrieval or semantic working memory processes Under this view BA44 and BA45 would together guide recovery of semantic information and evaluate the recovered information with regard to the criterion appropriate to a given context 2 3 A slightly modified account of this view is that activation of BA45 is needed only under controlled semantic retrieval when strong stimulus stimulus associations are absent 4 For other researchers BA45 s role is not restricted to semantics per se but to all activities that require task relevant representations from among competing representations 5 Lesions of the BA45 lead to the characteristic findings of expressive aphasia in individuals who are left hemispheric dominant Research findings editThis section has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article contains an enumeration of examples but lacks a general overview of its topic You can help by adding an appropriate introductory section Editing help is available January 2015 This section contains information of unclear or questionable importance or relevance to the article s subject Please help improve this section by clarifying or removing indiscriminate details Non important content should likely be moved to another article or removed January 2015 Learn how and when to remove this template message This article is written like a personal reflection personal essay or argumentative essay that states a Wikipedia editor s personal feelings or presents an original argument about a topic Please help improve it by rewriting it in an encyclopedic style January 2015 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message Asymmetry and language dominance edit nbsp Triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus Shown in red A strong correlation has been found between speech language and the anatomically asymmetric pars triangularis Foundas et al showed that language function can be localized to one region of the brain as Paul Broca had done before them but they also supported the idea that one side of the brain is more involved with language than the other The human brain has two hemispheres and each one looks similar to the other that is it looks like one hemisphere is a mirror image of the other However Foundas et al found that the pars triangularis in Broca s area is actually larger than the same region in the right side of the brain This leftward asymmetry corresponded both in form and function which means that the part of the brain that is active during language processing is larger In almost all the test subjects this was the left side In fact the only subject tested that had right hemispheric language dominance was found to have a rightward asymmetry of the pars triangularis 6 Certain other researchers however have found no volumetric asymmetries in the pars triangularis They have challenged previous findings that pars triangularis asymmetry exists and have suggested that inconsistencies in previous findings may be due to great variability in inter individual pars triangularis morphology That is these regions tend to vary in size and shape much more than other areas of the brain such as deep cortical nuclei Furthermore while these researcher found statistically significant asymmetries in the pars opercularis and the planum temporale they found no correlations between asymmetries of these brain regions with that of the pars triangularis 7 Connections in inferior frontal gyrus edit At least one study demonstrated a high degree of connectivity between the three subregions of the inferior frontal gyrus IFG By stimulating one region of the IFG and measuring the response in distinct regions these researchers were able to demonstrate the existence of numerous pathways between the pars triangularis and pars opercularis Also stimulation of one region of the pars triangularis elicited a response in distinct regions of the pars triangularis illustrating the presence of networks within the subgyral region 8 Additionally The pars triangularis was implicated in semantic processing of language By measuring the response of the brain by electroencephalography as it responded to different sentence types those with or without semantic errors Maess et al demonstrated a time lag in the comprehension of erroneous sentences To understand this one would only need to imagine a person being told something they did not understand They would pause and take a moment to process the information Furthermore these researchers demonstrated a characteristic processing pattern called an N400 which refers to a negativity that appears in the pars triangularis about 400 ms after the syntactic mismatch is presented 9 However the pars triangularis is likely to be only part of the network generating the N400 response in EEG since the magnetic counterpart N400m measured using MEG has been consistently localized to the superior temporal cortex 10 Prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory edit Pars triangularis has been shown to have a role in cognitive control of memory There are more ways than one to remember something When a person remembers s he retrieves information from storage in a memory center of the brain This information may be the muscle contraction sequence for shoe tying the face of a loved one or anything in between When someone remembers something automatically without concentrating on it and without trying it is called bottom up processing But sometimes people really have to struggle to remember something A student taking a test and trying to remember the answer to a question is concentrating their attention on retrieving the memory The student is exhibiting cognitive control over their memory This type of processing is directed in part by the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex VLPFC Pars triangularis is found in this region 11 When reading aloud people must decode written language to decipher its pronunciation This processing takes place in Broca s area The reader might use previous knowledge of a word in order to correctly vocalize it or the reader might use knowledge of systematic letter combinations which represent corresponding phonemes Scientists can learn about what the brain is doing while people process language by looking at what it does with errors in language As above scientists can investigate the extra processing that occurs when people are challenged with a problem In this case scientists took advantage of the way pseudo words and exception words by examining the brain as it interprets these problematic words When people process language they use different parts of Broca s area for different things Pars triangularis is involved in a specific type of language processing Specifically pars triangularis becomes activated when people read exception words which are words with atypical spelling to sound relationships For example have is an exception word because it is pronounced with a short a which is contrary to grammatical rules of pronunciation The e at the end of the word should lead to the pronunciation of the long a sound as in cave or rave Because we are so familiar with the word have we are able to remember its pronunciation and we don t have to think through the rules each time we read it Pars triangularis helps us do that 12 When trying to retrieve information in a top down fashion some kind of control mechanism is necessary Recalling that top down retrieval depends on conscious control it is easy to see that there must be some way to exclude irrelevant data from the retrieval In order to home in on the desired information some selection must occur This selection is thought to occur post retrieval in the mid VLPFC which corresponds generally to the location of pars triangularis The theory here is that information is retrieved by certain regions of the left VLPFC and then it is selected for relevance in another region This is called the two part model of memory retrieval 13 Almost every person in the world has learned at least one language Also almost everyone that has learned a language has learned it at a young age Some people are multilingual Some of these multilingual have learned second or third languages in concert with their first at a young age and some have learned other languages in their adulthood Studies on different subsets of monolinguals and multilinguals have revealed some interesting findings citation needed By looking at the similarities between the first and second language and what they do to the brain these researchers found that brain activation looked very different depending on which language the test subjects were processing They found that pars triangularis activation changes during processing of these different languages which is understandable considering the known role of pars triangularis in language 14 There is a difference between the processing patterns of primary and secondary languages in processing of passive sentences These are sentences using some form of the verb be with a verb in the past participle form For example He is ruined is a passive sentence because the verb ruin is in the past participle form and used with is which is a form of the verb be This study shows that processing this sentence late bilinguals used their pars triangularis much more than their counterparts This result implies certain things about the way language is learned For example It has been suggested that the reason people often have such difficulty learning foreign languages during adulthood is that their brains are trying to code language information in a region of the brain that is not dedicated to understanding language According to this view this is the reason native speakers are able to speak so quickly while their late bilingual counterparts are forced to stutter as they struggle to process grammatical rules 15 Cortical dynamics of word recognition edit There is a theory that pars triangularis is especially involved in semantic processing of language as opposed to phonological processing That is pars triangularis is thought to be more involved in deciphering the meaning of words rather than trying to decide what the word is based on the sound that goes into the ear This study got data that supported this theory Furthermore these researchers saw evidence for parallel semantic processing which occurs when the brain multitasks When their subjects were undergoing experimentation they were presented with consonant strings pseudo words and words and the delay between stimulus and brain activity was about the same for phonological and semantic processing even though the two seemed to occur in slightly different regions 16 In the study Semantic Encoding and Retrieval in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex A Functional magnetic resonance imaging Study of Task Difficulty and Process Specificity researchers found that pars triangularis as well as some of its neighbors increased its activity during semantic encoding regardless of difficulty of the word being processed This is consistent with the theory that pars triangularis is involved in semantic processing more than phonological processing Furthermore they found that these semantic encoding decisions resulted in less involvement of pars triangularis with repetition of the used words It may seem intuitive that practice would make the brain better at recognizing the words as they reappeared but there is something else to be learned from this result as well That pars triangularis activity went down with repetition also signifies the movement of the task of recognizing the word from the conscious to the passive This is called repetition priming and it occurs independent of intention This idea when paired with theories about pt s involvement in conscious retrieval of memory serves to illustrate the complexity of the brain and its functions These results together imply the possibility that similar mechanics are required for encoding and retrieving information Another point of interest was that decreased pars triangularis activation with repetition did not occur with redundant presentation of nonsemantically processed words 17 On Broca brain and binding a new framework edit A person is highly interconnected with other regions of the brain especially those in the left frontal language network Though its function seems to be distinct from its neighbors this high degree of connectivity supports the idea that language can be integrated into many of the seemingly unrelated thought processes we have This is not a difficult idea to imagine For instance attempting to remember the name of a brand new acquaintance can be challenging and it often demands the attention of the person doing the remembering In this example a person is trying to comprehend sound as a part of language place the word they just heard in the category names while associating it also as a tag for the face they just saw simultaneously committing all of these pieces of data to memory In this view it hardly seems far fetched that the roles of pars triangularis in language processing semantic comprehension and conscious control of memory are unrelated In fact it would be unlikely for pars triangularis not to have multiple roles in the brain especially considering its high degree of connectivity both within the left frontal language center and to other regions 18 Schizophrenia and Broca area edit Schizophrenia is a poorly understood disease with complicated symptoms In an effort to find a cause for this problem these researchers looked at the brains of schizophrenic patients It had been shown previously that abnormal gyrification asymmetry complexity and variability occur in patients with schizophrenia These investigators presented data showing that pt specifically was highly distorted in schizophrenic patients compared with demographically matched normal subjects They asserted that Broca s area is an especially plastic region of the brain in that its morphology can change dramatically from childhood to adulthood This makes sense when considering the special ability of children to easily learn language but it also means that the involvement of Broca s area is limited with respect to memory and recall children do not seem to be unable to consciously search their memories Furthermore investigators took volumetric measurements of the grey and white matter of the brains of their test subjects and compared those measurements to their normal control subjects They found that schizophrenic patients had dramatically reduced white matter citation needed As the brain develops connectivity of different regions changes dramatically Researchers found that there is a discrepancy in the way white matter and grey matter develop in schizophrenic patients People with schizophrenia tend to have an absence of white matter expansion 19 Heuristic and analytic reasoning edit Transcranial magnetic stimulation applied to the left BA45 facilitated incongruent reasoning performance and impaired congruent reasoning performance suggesting that the left BA45 is a component of a belief based heuristic system The right BA45 involvement in blocking the heuristic system is inferred from the blocking of the left homologue and resulting facilitation of logical analytic reasoning performance 20 Images edit nbsp Frontal view nbsp Lateral view nbsp Lateral surface of left cerebral hemisphere viewed from the side shown in orange See also editBrodmann area List of regions in the human brainReferences edit Yamada A Sakai KL April 2017 ブローカ野における文法処理 Syntactic Processing in Broca s Area Brodmann Areas 44 and 45 Brain and Nerve Shinkei Kenkyu No Shinpo in Japanese 69 4 479 487 doi 10 11477 mf 1416200767 PMID 28424402 Gabrieli et al 1998 The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory PNAS 95 3 906 913 Bibcode 1998PNAS 95 906G doi 10 1073 pnas 95 3 906 PMC 33815 PMID 9448258 Buckner R 1996 Contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long term memory retrieval Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3 2 149 158 doi 10 3758 BF03212413 PMID 24213862 S2CID 31024400 Wagner A D 2002 Cognitive control and episodic memory Contributions from prefrontal cortex In Squire Larry R Schacter Daniel L eds Neuropsychology of Memory 3rd ed New York Guilford Press pp 174 192 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 11 7909 ISBN 978 1 57230 898 5 Thompson Schill et al 1999 Effects of repetition and competition on activity of left prefrontal cortex during word generation Neuron 23 3 513 522 doi 10 1016 S0896 6273 00 80804 1 PMID 10433263 S2CID 15194595 Foundas AL Leonard CM Gilmore RL Fennell EB Heilman KM January 1996 Pars triangularis asymmetry and language dominance Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93 2 719 722 Bibcode 1996PNAS 93 719F doi 10 1073 pnas 93 2 719 PMC 40120 PMID 8570622 Keller SS Highley JR Garcia Finana M Sluming V Rezaie R Roberts N 2007 Sulcal variability stereological measurement and asymmetry of Broca s area on MRI images J Anat 211 4 534 55 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7580 2007 00793 x PMC 2375829 PMID 17727624 Jeremy D W Greenlee Hiroyuki Oya Hiroto Kawasaki Igor O Volkov Meryl A Severson III Matthew A Howard III John F Brugge 2007 Functional connections within the human inferior frontal gyrus The Journal of Comparative Neurology 503 4 550 559 doi 10 1002 cne 21405 PMID 17534935 S2CID 5685566 Maess Burkhard Herrmann Christoph S Hahne Anja Nakamura Akinori Friederici Angela D 2006 Localizing the distributed language network responsible for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence processing Brain Research 1096 1 163 172 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2006 04 037 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0010 C478 0 PMID 16769041 S2CID 14551069 Vartiainen J Parviainen T Salmelin R 2009 Spatiotemporal convergence of semantic processing in reading and speech perception Journal of Neuroscience 29 29 9271 9280 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 5860 08 2009 PMC 6665572 PMID 19625517 David Badre amp Anthony D Wagner 2007 Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory Neuropsychologia 45 13 2883 2901 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2007 06 015 PMID 17675110 S2CID 16062085 Mechelli Andrea Crinion Jennifer T Long Steven Friston Karl J Lambon Ralph Matthew A Patterson Karalyn McClelland James L Price Cathy J 2005 Dissociating Reading Processes on the Basis of Neuronal Interactions PDF Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17 11 1753 1765 doi 10 1162 089892905774589190 PMID 16269111 S2CID 9596621 Badre D Poldrack R Pare Blagoev E Insler R Wagner A 2005 Dissociable Controlled Retrieval and Generalized Selection Mechanisms in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Neuron 47 6 907 918 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2005 07 023 PMID 16157284 S2CID 5846073 Jeong Hyeonjeong Sugiura Motoaki Sassa Yuko Haji Tomoki Usui Nobuo Taira Masato Horie Kaoru Sato Shigeru Kawashima Ryuta March 2007 Effect of syntactic similarity on cortical activation during second language processing A comparison of English and Japanese among native Korean trilinguals Human Brain Mapping 28 3 194 204 doi 10 1002 hbm 20269 PMC 6871317 PMID 16767768 Yokoyama Satoru Okamoto Hideyuki Miyamoto Tadao Yoshimoto Kei Kim Jungho Iwata Kazuki Jeong Hyeonjeong Uchida Shinya Ikuta Naho Sassa Yuko Nakamura Wataru Horie Kaoru Sato Shigeru Kawashima Ryuta April 2006 Cortical activation in the processing of passive sentences in L1 and L2 An fMRI study NeuroImage 30 2 570 579 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2005 09 066 PMID 16300965 S2CID 9226986 Mainy Nelly Jung Julien Baciu Monica Kahane Philippe Schoendorff Benjamin Minotti Lorella Hoffmann Dominique Bertrand Olivier Lachaux Jean Philippe November 2008 Cortical dynamics of word recognition Human Brain Mapping 29 11 1215 1230 doi 10 1002 hbm 20457 PMC 6871193 PMID 17712785 Demb J Desmond J Wagner A Vaidya C Glover G Gabrieli J 1995 Cortex A Functional magnetic resonance imaging Study of Task Difficulty and Process Specificity The Journal of Neuroscience 15 9 5870 5878 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 15 09 05870 1995 PMC 6577672 PMID 7666172 Hagoort Peter September 2005 On Broca brain and binding a new framework Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 9 416 423 doi 10 1016 j tics 2005 07 004 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0013 1E16 A PMID 16054419 S2CID 2826729 Wisco Jonathan J Kuperberg Gina Manoach Dara Quinn Brian T Busa Evelina Fischl Bruce Heckers Stephan Sorensen A Gregory August 2007 Abnormal cortical folding patterns within Broca s area in schizophrenia Evidence from structural MRI Schizophrenia Research 94 1 3 317 327 doi 10 1016 j schres 2007 03 031 PMC 2034662 PMID 17490861 Tsujii Takeo Masuda Sayako Akiyama Takekazu Watanabe Shigeru 2010 The role of inferior frontal cortex in belief bias reasoning an rTMS study Neuropsychologia 48 7 2005 2008 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2010 03 021 PMID 20362600 S2CID 39839238 External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Brodmann area 45 ancil 53 at NeuroNames Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Brodmann area 45 amp oldid 1180355987, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.