fbpx
Wikipedia

Self-enhancement

Self-enhancement is a type of motivation that works to make people feel good about themselves and to maintain self-esteem.[1] This motive becomes especially prominent in situations of threat, failure or blows to one's self-esteem.[2][3][4] Self-enhancement involves a preference for positive over negative self-views.[5] It is one of the three self-evaluation motives along with self-assessment (the drive for an accurate self-concept) and self-verification (the drive for a self-concept congruent with one's identity). Self-evaluation motives drive the process of self-regulation, that is, how people control and direct their own actions.

There are a variety of strategies that people can use to enhance their sense of personal worth. For example, they can downplay skills that they lack or they can criticise others to seem better by comparison. These strategies are successful, in that people tend to think of themselves as having more positive qualities and fewer negative qualities than others.[6] Although self-enhancement is seen in people with low self-esteem as well as with high self-esteem, these two groups tend to use different strategies. People who already have high esteem enhance their self-concept directly, by processing new information in a biased way. People with low self-esteem use more indirect strategies, for example by avoiding situations in which their negative qualities will be noticeable.[7]

There are controversies over whether or not self-enhancement is beneficial to the individual, and over whether self-enhancement is culturally universal or specific to Western individualism.

Levels

Self-enhancement can occur in many different situations and under many different guises. The general motive of self-enhancement can have many differing underlying explanations, each of which becomes more or less dominant depending on the situation.

The explanations of the self-enhancement motive can occur in different combinations. Self-enhancement can occur as an underlying motive or personality trait without occurring as an observed effect.

Levels of self-enhancement
Observed effectSelf-enhancement at the level of an observed effect describes the product of the motive. For example, self-enhancement can produce inflated self-ratings (positive illusions). Such ratings would be self-enhancement manifested as an observed effect. It is an observable instance of the motive.
Ongoing processSelf-enhancement at the level of an ongoing process describes the actual operation of the motive. For example, self-enhancement can result in attributing favourable outcomes to the self and unfavourable outcomes to others (self-serving attribution bias). The actual act of attributing such ratings would be self-enhancement manifested as an ongoing process. It is the motive in operation.
Personality traitSelf-enhancement at the level of a personality trait describes habitual or inadvertent self-enhancement. For example, self-enhancement can cause situations to be created to ease the pain of failure (self-handicapping). The fabrication of such situations or excuses frequently and without awareness would be self-enhancement manifested as a personality trait. It is the repetitive inclination to demonstrate the motive.
Underlying motiveSelf-enhancement at the level of an underlying motive describes the conscious desire to self-enhance. For example, self-enhancement can cause the comparison of the self to a worse other, making the self seem greater in comparison (strategic social comparisons). The act of comparing intentionally to achieve superiority would be self-enhancement manifested as an underlying motive. It is the genuine desire to see the self as superior.
The four levels of self-enhancement manifestation as defined by Sedikides & Gregg (2008)[5]

Dimensions

Both the extent and the type of self-enhancement vary across a number of dimensions.[5]

Self-advancement vs. self-protection

Self-enhancement can occur by either self-advancing or self-protecting, that is either by enhancing the positivity of one's self-concept, or by reducing the negativity of one's self-concept.[8] Self-protection appears to be the stronger of the two motives, given that avoiding negativity is of greater importance than encouraging positivity.[9] However, as with all motivations, there are differences between individuals. For example, people with higher self-esteem appear to favour self-advancement, whereas people with lower self-esteem tend to self-protect.[10] This highlights the role of risk: to not defend oneself against negativity in favour of self-promotion offers the potential for losses, whereas whilst one may not gain outright from self-protection, one does not incur the negativity either. People high in self-esteem tend to be greater risk takers and therefore opt for the more risky strategy of self-advancement, whereas those low in self-esteem and risk-taking hedge their bets with self-protection.[11]

Public vs. private

Self-enhancement can occur in private or in public.[12] Public self-enhancement is obvious positive self-presentation,[13] whereas private self enhancement is unnoticeable except to the individual.[14] The presence of other people i.e. in public self-enhancement, can either augment or inhibit self-enhancement.[15][16] Whilst self-enhancement may not always take place in public it is nevertheless still influenced by the social world, for example via social comparisons.[17]

Central vs. peripheral

Potential areas of self-enhancement differ in terms how important, or central, they are to a person.[18] Self-enhancement tends to occur more in the domains that are the most important to a person, and less in more peripheral, less important domains.[19][20]

Candid vs. tactical

Self-enhancement can occur either candidly or tactically.[21] Candid self-enhancement serves the purpose of immediate gratification whereas tactical self-enhancement can result in potentially larger benefits from delayed gratification.

Tactical self-enhancement is often preferred over candid self-enhancement as overt self-enhancement is socially displeasing for those around it.[22] Narcissism is an exemplification of extreme candid self-enhancement.[23]

Types

Self-enhancement does not just occur at random. Its incidence is often highly systematic and can occur in any number of ways in order to achieve its goal of inflating perceptions of the self. Importantly, we are typically unaware that we are self-enhancing. Awareness of self-enhancing processes would highlight the facade we are trying to create, revealing that the self we perceive is in fact an enhanced version of our actual self.

Self-serving attribution bias

Self-enhancement can also affect the causal explanations people generate for social outcomes. People have a tendency to exhibit a self-serving attribution bias, that is to attribute positive outcomes to one's internal disposition but negative outcomes to factors beyond one's control e.g. others, chance or circumstance.[24] In short, people claim credit for their successes but deny responsibilities for their failures. The self-serving attribution bias is very robust, occurring in public as well as in private,[25][26] even when a premium is placed on honesty.[27] People most commonly manifest a self-serving bias when they explain the origin or events in which they personally had a hand or a stake.[28][29]

Explanations for moral transgressions follow similar self-serving patterns,[30][31] as do explanations for group behaviour.[32] The ultimate attribution error[32] is the tendency to regard negative acts by one's out-group and positive acts by one's in-group as essential to their nature i.e. attributable to their internal disposition and not a product of external factors. This may reflect the operation of the self-serving bias refracted through social identification.[33][34]

Selectivity

Selective memory

Selectivity within information processing
Selective attentionPeople typically avoid attending to negative, unflattering information at encoding,[35][36] therefore its initial recognition is impaired. Selective attention manifests itself in the form of an overt behaviour via selective exposure.
Selective exposurePeople selectively expose themselves to information that justifies important prior decisions they have made.[37] This holds true so long as the information appears to be valid and the decision that was made was done so freely and is irreversible.[38]
Selective recallAt retrieval people bring to mind a highly biased collection of memories. Selective recall occurs for behaviours that exemplify desirable personality traits,[39] harmonious interpersonal relationships[40] or even health enhancing habits. Affect associated with unpleasant memories also fades faster than affect associated with pleasant memories.[41]

People sometimes self-enhance by selectively remembering their strengths rather than weaknesses. This pattern of selective forgetting has been described as mnemic neglect. Mnemic neglect may reflect biases in the processing of information at either encoding, retrieval or retention.

  • Biases at encoding occur via selective attention and selective exposure.
  • Biases at retrieval and retention occur via selective recall.

The role of mnemic neglect can be emphasised or reduced by the characteristics of a certain behaviour or trait. For example, after receiving false feedback pertaining to a variety of behaviours, participants recalled more positive behaviours than negative ones, but only when the behaviours exemplified central not peripheral traits and only when feedback pertained to the self and not to others.[36] Similar findings emerge when the to-be-recalled information is personality traits,[42] relationship promoting or undermining behaviours,[43] frequencies of social acts,[44] and autobiographical memories.[45]

Selective acceptance and refutation

Selective acceptance involves taking as fact self-flattering or ego-enhancing information with little regard for its validity. Selective refutation involves searching for plausible theories that enable criticism to be discredited. A good example of selective acceptance and refutation in action would be: Selective acceptance is the act of accepting as valid an examination on which one has performed well without consideration of alternatives, whereas selective refutation would be mindfully searching for reasons to reject as invalid an examination on which one has performed poorly.[46][47]

Concordant with selective acceptance and refutation is the observation that people hold a more critical attitude towards blame placed upon them, but a more lenient attitude to praise that they receive.[48][49] People will strongly contest uncongenial information but readily accept at without question congenial information[50][51]

Strategies

Strategic social comparisons

 
The potential directions for strategic social comparisons.

The social nature of the world we live in means that self-evaluation cannot take place in an absolute nature – comparison to other social beings is inevitable. Many social comparisons occur automatically as a consequence of circumstance, for example within an exam sitting social comparisons of intellect may occur to those sitting the same exam. However, the strength of the self-enhancement motive can cause the subjective exploitation of scenarios in order to give a more favourable outcome to the self in comparisons between the self and others. Such involuntary social comparisons prompt self-regulatory strategies.

Self-esteem moderates the beneficial, evaluative consequences of comparisons to both inferior and superior others. People with higher self-esteem are more optimistic about both evading the failures and misfortunes of their inferiors and about securing the successes and good fortunes of their superiors.[52]

Upward social comparisons

An upwards social comparison involves comparing oneself to an individual perceived to be superior to or better than oneself. Upwards social comparison towards someone felt to be similar to oneself can induce self-enhancement through assimilation of the self and other's characteristics,[53] however this only occurs when:

  • The gap between the self and the comparison target is not too large;[54]
  • The skill or success being compared is attainable;[55]
  • The comparison target is perceived as a competitor.[56]

Where assimilation does not occur as a result of a social comparison, contrast can instead occur which can lead to upwards social comparisons providing inspiration.[57]

Downward social comparisons

Even though upwards social comparisons are the most common social comparisons,[58][59] people do sometimes make downwards social comparisons. Downwards social comparisons involve comparing oneself to an individual perceived to be inferior to or less skilled than the self. Downwards social comparisons serve as a form of ego-defence whereby the ego is inflated due to the sense of superiority gained from such downwards social comparisons.[60][61]

Lateral social comparisons

Lateral social comparisons, comparisons against those perceived as equal to the self, can also be self-enhancing. Comparisons with members of one's in-group can lead be protective against low self-esteem, especially when the in-group are disadvantaged.[62]

Self-evaluation maintenance theory

Self-enhancement waxes and wanes as a function of one's ability level in the context of interpersonal relationships, and this, in turn influences interpersonal attitudes and behaviours. Three factors influence the self-evaluations people make:[63]

  • Closeness of a relationship: comparison of one's own performance with that of another is more likely to occur, and when it does is more consequential when others are close rather than distant.
  • Personal relevance of a particular ability: when the domain is not relevant to oneself reflection will occur and when the domain is relevant comparison will occur.
    • Reflection: one will undergo self-enhancement (pride) when the other does well, but self-derogation (shame) when the other does poorly.
    • Comparison: one will undergo self-derogation (humiliation) if others perform well, but self-enhancement (triumph) if the other performs poorly.
  • Level of performance in that ability domain.

People adopt a variety of coping strategies to deal with the pressures of self-evaluation:

  • Choose friends and partners who excel, but not in the same domains as they do;[64]
  • Withhold information that is likely to improve the performance of others of personally relevant domains;[65]
  • Alter the relevance of performance domains by changing their self-concept, thus moderating the impact of the reflection and comparison processes;[66]
  • Broaden or narrow the gap between the oneself and others, even by deliberately altering the difficulty of domain-relevant tasks.[67]

Strategic construal

The concepts that people use to understand themselves and their social world are relatively vague.[68] Consequently, when making social comparisons or estimations people can easily and subtly shift their construal of the meaning of those concepts in order to self-enhance. Strategic construals typically increase following negative feedback.[69] Numerous examples of strategic construals exist, a small selection include:

  • People's interpretation of what counts as a virtue or talent is biased in favour of the attributes they possess, and of what counts as a vice or deficiency in favour of attributes they lack.[70]
  • People rate personality feedback and scientific research as less credible if it implies they are susceptible to disease.[48][71]
  • Lazy people perceive the rest of the world as reasonably fit and healthy, whereas frequent exercisers see their athleticism as a single, unique attribute.[72]
  • Low achievers in a particular area are likely to perceive the successes of high achievers as exceptional, thereby lessening the shame of their own inability.[73]
  • People think harder about any discouraging test results they receive, will spend longer thinking about them, are more inclined to have them confirmed and are significantly more skeptical of them.[50] People do not react the same way to test results received by others however.[74]
  • When research tarnishes the reputation of groups with which people identify, they search for a statistical weakness of that research.[75]
  • Strategic construals can also be more subtle. People make self-aggrandizing interpretations not only of their own attributes, but also of others in order to appear greater by comparison.

Strategic construals appear to operate around one's self-esteem. After either positive or negative feedback people with high self-esteem alter their perceptions of others, typically varying their perceptions of others ability and performance in a self-enhancing direction.[76] Those with low self-esteem however do not. Self-esteem level appears to moderate the use of strategic construals. As well as operating as a function of self-esteem level, strategic construals also appear to protect self-esteem levels. For example, members of minority groups who perform poorly in academic settings due to negative cultural attitudes towards them, subsequently disengage psychologically from, and dissidentify with academic pursuits in general. Whilst buffering their self-esteem level they jeopardise their future socioeconomic prospects.[77]

Strategic construals also influence the degree to which categories are believed to characterise other people. There is a general tendency to assume that others share one's own characteristics.[78] Nevertheless, people reliably overestimate the prevalence of their shortcomings e.g. show enhanced false consensus effect, and underestimate the prevalence of their strengths e.g. show a contrary false uniqueness effect.[79] People perceive their flaws as relatively commonplace but their skills as unique.

Behavioural self-handicapping

Behavioural self-handicapping is the act of erecting obstacles in the path of task success in order to reduce the evaluative implications that can be drawn from task performance.[80] This permits self-enhancement to occur in two ways:[81]

  • In the case of failure, self-handicapping can protect self-esteem by attributing failure to obstacles that one has erected – discounting.
  • In the case of success, self-handicapping can promote self-esteem by attributing success to oneself despite the obstacles one has erected – augmenting.

People low in self-esteem opt for discounting as a self-protective route to avoid being perceived as incompetent, whereas people high in self-esteem preferentially select augmenting as a method of self-promotion to enhance their perceived competence.[10][82] Self-handicapping, whilst predominantly a behaviour that occurs in private performance[83] is magnified in public situations.[84] However, self-handicapping is highly risky in social situations. If found out, those who use it face the negative evaluation and criticism of others.[85]

Factors promoting behavioural self-handicapping
Task familiarity Uncertainty over ability to obtain a positive outcome due to experience of limited control over a similar task.[86]
Task complexity Holding a very fixed, concrete theory of the complexity of a task.[87]
Insecurity Uncertainty over ability to obtain a positive outcome due to generally insecure sense of self.[86]
Belief Holding the belief that improvement is physically possible.
Importance A task or evaluation has to be important to the self in order for self-handicapping to occur.
Feedback Negative feedback makes self-handicapping more probable as it allows any damage to the ego to be rectified.[88]
Neuroticism High neuroticism promotes discounting.[89]
Conscientiousness Low conscientiousness can increase the tendency to self-handicap.[89]

Regardless of the causes of self-handicapping the self-defeating end result remains the same – the integrity and quality of a task outcome or evaluation is compromised in order that the meaning of that outcome appears more agreeable. Behavioural self-handicapping is a good demonstration of active self-deception.[90]

Whilst task performance is important to people, they do sometimes act in ways so as to paradoxically impair task performance,[80] either to protect against the shame of performing poorly by creating a convenient excuse (discounting), or to enhance themselves by succeeding despite adversity by creating grounds for conceit (augmenting).[82] Furthermore, self-handicapping can have unintentional adverse consequences. Whilst allowing the maintenance of positive self-views[91] self-handicapping has the cost of impairing objective performance.[92] Students who report frequent use self-handicapping strategies underperform relative to their aptitude, with poor examination preparation mediating the effect.[93]

Ultimately, those who readily prepare themselves for the possibility of poor task performance beforehand use the strategy of discounting less.[94]

Outcomes

The effect of self-enhancement strategies is shown in the tendency of people to see themselves as having more positive qualities and a more positive future outlook than others.[6]

Self-enhancing triad

People generally hold unrealistically positive views about themselves. Such flattering views can often be neatly categorised within what has become known as the Triad of Positive Illusions.[95] The three illusions in question are above-average effect,[96] illusions of control,[97] and unrealistic optimism.[98] These illusions can be replicated across many situations and are highly resistant to revision. Rather ironically, when informed of the existence of such illusions, people generally consider themselves to be less prone to them than others.[99]

Above-average effect

The better-than-average-effect is the most common demonstration of an above-average effect. It is a highly robust effect, as evidenced by the fact that even when the criteria on which the self and others are judged are identical the self is still perceived more favourably.[96] Things close to the self also take on the perceived superiority of the above-average effect. People value both their close relationships[100][101] and their personal possessions[102] above those of others. However, where an outcome is perceived as highly skilled, people often err on the side of caution and display a worse-than-average effect. The majority of people would rate themselves as below average in unicycling ability, for example.

 
The three related divisions of the self-enhancing triad.

The illusory nature of the above-average effect comes from the fact that not everyone can be above-average – otherwise the average would not be the average! The majority of people rating themselves as being better than the majority of people does not quite seem plausible, and in some situations is 100% impossible. Where a distribution is symmetrical i.e. mean = median = mode, it is statistically impossible for the majority of people to be above average, as whichever of the three averages is taken, all are equal to the 50th percentile.[103] In a non-symmetrical distribution i.e. mean < median < mode or mode < median < mean, it is statistically impossible for the majority of people to be above average when the average is taken to be the median, as the median represents the 50th percentile, or the midpoint of the data.[103] However, in a non-symmetrical distribution where the average is taken to be either the mean or the mode, the above-average effect can be statistically plausible. In some situations the majority of people can be above-average.

People show self-enhancement in the form of the above-average effect in many different ways. It is typical for people to profess to be above-average at a task yielding positive or desirable outcomes, and below average at a task yielding negative or undesirable outcomes.

Some of the wide variety of documented examples of the above-average effect include observations that:

  • Most university students regard themselves as well above the 50th percentile in exhibiting social grace, athletic prowess and leadership abilities.[104][105][106]
  • Even 12th percentile achievers in domains such as grammar and logic consider themselves to be of 62nd percentile achievement.[107]
  • 94% of university professors believe their teaching ability to be above average.[108]
  • University students in the UK and the US regard themselves as above average drivers.[109] Even drivers hospitalised after causing accidents persist in believing they are no worse than regular drivers.[110]
  • Even when informed about the above average effect people rate themselves as less susceptible to such biases than others.[111]

Illusions of control

People overestimate the level of control they have over outcomes and contingencies,[112] seeing their actions as influential even when they are in fact inconsequential.[113] Also, people stand by their apparent conviction that they can influence the outcomes of inherently random systems for example lotteries, especially when such systems possess features typically associated with skill-based tasks. Even when a degree of contingency does exist between actions and outcomes, people still reliably overestimate the strength of that contingency.[113]

Unrealistic optimism

People typically believe that their life will hold a greater number of positive experiences and fewer negative experiences than the lives of similar others.[98][114][115] They have the same unrealistic optimism, but to a lesser degree, for others who are closely linked, such as romantic partners and close personal friends.[116]

Unrealistic optimism is apparent in people's behaviours and beliefs across many different situations. People can both overestimate their ability to predict the future,[117] and underestimate how long it will take them to complete a variety of tasks.[118] People also overestimate the accuracy of their social predictions,[119] and interpret probability adverbs to award higher values for personal positive outcomes and lower values for personal negative outcomes.[120] Smokers, rather alarmingly, underestimate their risk of cancer relative to both non-smokers and even in comparison with fellow smokers.[121]

Benefits and costs to the individual

There is controversy over whether self-enhancement is adaptive or maladaptive.[122][123] A single operationalisation of self-enhancement can be influenced by a variety of motives and thus can be coordinated with both positive and negative outcomes.[124] Those who misperceive their performance (self-enhancers and self-effacers) tend to have a lower academic achievement, lower subsequent performance. These results appear to be culturally universal.[125] Surely, it's a false assumption to relate self enhancement to depression.

  • If self-enhancement is taken to mean rendering more positive judgments of oneself than of others then outcomes are frequently favourable.[126][127]
  • If self-enhancement is taken to mean the rendering of more positive judgements of oneself than others render then outcomes are often untoward.[128][129]

Which definition is better at measuring self-enhancement has been disputed, as rating oneself more positively than one rates others is not seen as self-enhancement by some researchers.[130]

In some studies, self-enhancement has been shown to have strong positive links with good mental health[131] and in others with bad mental health.[128] Self-enhancing can also have social costs. Whilst promoting resilience amongst survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks, those who self-enhanced were rated as having decreased social adaptation and honesty by friends and family.[132]

Constraints

Plausibility

Self-enhancement thrives upon the vagueness or ambiguity of evidence. Where criteria are rigidly defined, self-enhancement typically reduces. For example, the above-average effect decreases as clarity and definition of the defined trait increases.[133] The easier it is to verify a behaviour or trait, the less that trait will be subject to self-enhancement. The plausibility of a trait or characteristic given real world evidence moderates the degree to which the self-enhancement of that trait occurs. Selectively recalling instances of desirable traits is moderated by one's actual standing on those traits in reality.[39]

When plausibility reduces the impact of self-enhancement, undesirable evidence often has to be accepted, albeit reluctantly. This typically occurs when all possible interpretations of the evidence in question have been made.[75] The reason for this unwilling acceptance is to maintain effective social functioning, where unqualified self-aggrandizement would otherwise prevent it.[134] People will continue to self-enhance so long as they think they can get away with it.[135][136]

The constraint of plausibility on self enhancement exists because self-enhancing biases cannot be exploited. Self-enhancement works only under the assumption of rationality – to admit to self-enhancing totally undermines any conclusions one can draw and any possibility of believing its facade, since according to legit rational processes it functions as a genuinely verifiable and accredited improvement.[137]

Mood

Both positive and negative moods can reduce the presence of the self-enhancement motive. The effects of mood on self-enhancement can be explained by a negative mood making the use self-enhancing tactics harder, and a positive mood making their use less necessary in the first place.

The onset of a positive mood can make people more receptive to negative diagnostic feedback. Past successes are reviewed with expectation of receiving such positive feedback, presumably to buffer their mood.[138]

Depression has quite a well-evidenced link with a decrease in the motive to self-enhance. Depressives are less able to self-enhance in response to negative feedback than non-depressive controls.[139][140] Having a depressive disposition decreases the discrepancy between one's own estimates of one's virtues and the estimates of a neutral observer, namely by increasing modesty.[141][142] Illusions of control are moderated by melancholy.[143] However, whilst the self-ratings of depressives are more in line with those of neutral observers than the self ratings of normals, the self ratings of normals are more in line with those of friends and family than the self ratings of depressives.[141]

Social context and relationships

The presence of the motive to self-enhance is dependent on many social situations, and the relationships shared with the people in them. Many different materialisations of self-enhancement can occur depending on such social contexts:

  • The self-enhancement motive is weaker during interactions with close and significant others.
  • When friends (or previous strangers whose intimacy levels have been enhanced) cooperate on a task, they do not exhibit a self-serving attribution bias.
    • Casual acquaintances and true strangers however do exhibit a self-serving attribution bias.[135]
    • Where no self-serving bias is exhibited in a relationship, a betrayal of trust in the relationship will reinstate the self-serving bias. This corresponds to findings that relationship satisfaction is inversely correlated with the betrayal of trust.[144]
  • Both mutual liking and expectation of reciprocity appear to mediate graciousness in the presence of others.[145]
  • Whilst people have a tendency to self-present boastfully in front of strangers, this inclination disappears in the presence of friends.[136]
  • Others close to the self are generally more highly evaluated than more distant others.[146]

Culture

Psychological functioning is moderated by the influence of culture.[147][148][149] There is much evidence to support a culture-specific view of self-enhancement.

Westerners typically... Easterners typically...
Prioritise intradependence Prioritise interdependence
Place greater importance on individualistic values Place greater importance on collectivistic values
Have more inflated ratings of their own merits Have less inflated ratings of their own merits[150]
Emphasise internal attributes Emphasise relational attributes[151]
Show self-enhancement that overshadows self-criticism Show self-criticism that overshadows self-enhancement[151]
Give spontaneously more positive self-descriptions Give spontaneously more negative self-descriptions[152]
Make fewer self-deprecatory social comparisons Make more self-deprecatory social comparisons[153]
Hold more unrealistically optimistic views of the future Hold fewer unrealistically optimistic views of the future[154]
Display a self-serving attributional bias Do not display a self-serving attributional bias[155]
Show a weak desire to self-improve via self-criticism Show a strong desire to self-improve via self-criticism[156]
Are eager to conclude better performance than a classmate Are reluctant to conclude better performance than a classmate[157]
Reflexively discount negative feedback Readily acknowledge negative feedback[158]
Persist more after initial success Persist more after initial failure[159]
Consider tasks in which they succeed to be most diagnostic Consider tasks in which they fail to be most diagnostic[159]
Self-enhance on the majority of personality dimensions Self-enhance only on some personality dimensions[160]
Self-enhance on individualistic attributes Self-enhance on collectivist attributes[20][161]

Self-enhancement appears to be a phenomenon largely limited to Western cultures, where social ties are looser than in the East. This is concordant with empirical evidence highlighting relationship closeness as a constraint on self-enhancement.[162] The self-improvement motive, as an aspiration towards a possible self[163] may also moderate a variety of psychological processes in both independent and interdependent cultures.[164]

There are nevertheless signs that self-enhancement is not completely absent in interdependent cultures. Chinese schoolchildren rate themselves highly on the dimension of competence,[165] and Taiwanese employees rate themselves more favourably than their employers do,[166] both of which show self-enhancing tendencies in Eastern cultures.

One possible explanation for the observed differences in self-enhancement between cultures is that they may occur through differences in how candidly of tactically the motive to self-enhance is acted upon, and not due to variations in the strength of motive.[21] Alternatively, self-enhancement may be represented only in terms of the characteristics that are deemed important by individuals as they strive to fulfil their culturally prescribed roles.

The issue over whether self-enhancement is universal or specific to Western cultures has been contested within modern literature by two researchers — Constantine Sedikides and Steven Heine. Sedikides argues that self-enhancement is universal, and that different cultures self-enhance in domains important in their culture. Heine on the other hand describes self-enhancement as a predominantly Western motive.[20][167][168][169][170][171][172]

Other motives

It is an exaggeration to say that self-enhancement is the dominant self-evaluation motive. Many controversies exist regarding the distinction between the self-evaluation motives, and there are situations in which motives asides from self-enhancement assume priority.

  1. The self-assessment motive is often contrasted with the self-enhancement motive due to the relative adaptiveness of each approach within social interactions.
  2. The self-verification motive is often challenged by supporters of the self-enhancement as being unfeasible as it often appears implausible.
  3. The self-improvement motive is often taken to be the physical manifestation of the self-enhancement motive i.e. the act of attaining desired positive self views.

Where the truth about oneself worsens or varies it gradually becomes less feasible to satisfy all motives simultaneously.

In an attempt to compare the self-evaluation motives (excluding self-improvement) a self-reflection task was employed. Participants were asked to choose the question they would most likely ask themselves in order to determine whether they possessed a certain personality trait. On the whole, people self-enhanced more than they self-assessed or self-verified. People chose higher diagnosticity questions concerning central, positive traits than central, negative ones, and answered yes more often to central, positive than negative questions. Also, people self-verified more than the self-assessed, and chose more questions overall concerning relatively certain central traits than relatively uncertain peripheral traits.[173]

Other factors

  • Cognitive load: Where people are in situations of great cognitive load, the tendency to self-enhance increases, almost as if instinctive. People are quicker to agree with possessing positive traits and slower to reject having negative traits.[174][175]
  • Modifiability: Where a trait or characteristic is seen as unchangeable people are more self-enhancing versus perceiving the trait to be modifiable.[176]
  • Diagnosticity: Where a trait or characteristic is seen as highly diagnostic people are less likely to self-enhance, for fear of being caught out in the process of an erroneous attempt at self enhancement as neurosis.[135][136]

See also

References

Notes

  1. ^ Sedikides, C.; Strube, M. J. (1995), "The Multiply Motivated Self", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (12): 1330–1335, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.561.6126, doi:10.1177/01461672952112010, ISSN 0146-1672, S2CID 34670867, The self-enhancement motive refers to people's desire to enhance the positivity or decrease the negativity of the self-concept.
  2. ^ Beauregard, Keith S.; Dunning, David (1998), "Turning up the contrast: Self-enhancement motives prompt egocentric contrast effects in social judgments", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (3): 606–621, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.606, ISSN 0022-3514, PMID 9523408.
  3. ^ Krueger, J. (1998), "Enhancement Bias in Descriptions of Self and Others", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 (5): 505–516, doi:10.1177/0146167298245006, ISSN 0146-1672, S2CID 144483633.
  4. ^ Wills, Thomas A. (1981), "Downward comparison principles in social psychology", Psychological Bulletin, 90 (2): 245–271, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245, ISSN 0033-2909.
  5. ^ a b c Sedikides, Constantine; Gregg, Aiden P. (2008), "Self-Enhancement: Food for Thought" (PDF), Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3 (2): 102–116, doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00068.x, ISSN 1745-6916, PMID 26158877, S2CID 5171543.
  6. ^ a b Kunda 1999, pp. 485–486
  7. ^ Kunda 1999, pp. 465–466
  8. ^ Arkin, R. M. (1981), Self-presentation styles. In J. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression management theory and research (pp. 311-333). New York: Academic Press
  9. ^ Baumeister, R. F.; Bratslavsky, Ellen; Finkenauer, Catrin & Vohs, Kathleen D. (2001), "Bad is stronger than good" (PDF), Review of General Psychology, 5 (4): 323–370, doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323, S2CID 13154992
  10. ^ a b Tice, Dianne M. (1991), "Esteem protection or enhancement? Self-handicapping motives and attributions differ by trait self-esteem", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (5): 711–725, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.711, ISSN 0022-3514.
  11. ^ Josephs, R. A.; Larrick, RP; Steele, CM; Nisbett, RE (1992), "Protecting the self from the negative consequences of risky decisions", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 (1): 26–37, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.1.26, PMID 1538314
  12. ^ Brown, J. D. & Gallagher, Frances M (1992), "Coming to terms with failure: Private self-enhancement and public self-effacement", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28: 3–22, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(92)90029-J
  13. ^ Leary, M. R. & Kowalski, Robin M. (1990), "Impression management: A literature review and two component model", Psychological Bulletin, 107: 34–47, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.776, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.34, S2CID 15886705
  14. ^ Greenwald, A. G. & Breckler, S. J. (1985), "To whom is the self presented?", in B. E. Schlenker (ed.), The self and social life, New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 126–145
  15. ^ Leary, M. R.; Tchividjian, LR & Kraxberger, BE (1994), "Self-presentation can be hazardous to your health: Impression management and health risk", Health Psychology, 13 (6): 461–470, doi:10.1037/0278-6133.13.6.461, PMID 7889900
  16. ^ Sedikides, C.; Campbell, W. K.; Reeder, G. & Elliot, A. J. (1998), "The self-serving bias in relational context", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (2): 378–79, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.378, S2CID 67763602
  17. ^ Klein, W. M. (1997), "Objective standards are not enough: Affective, self-evaluative and behavioural responses to social comparison information", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 (4): 763–774, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.763, PMID 9108694
  18. ^ Crocker, J. & Wolfe, C. T. (2001), "Contingencies of self-worth", Psychological Review, 108 (3): 592–623, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.593, PMID 11488379
  19. ^ Crocker, J. (2002), "Contingencies of self-worth: Implications for self-regulation and psychological vulnerability", Self and Identity, 1 (2): 143–149, doi:10.1080/152988602317319320, S2CID 142982721
  20. ^ a b c Sedikides, Constantine; Gaertner, Lowell; Toguchi, Yoshiyasu (2003), "Pancultural self-enhancement", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (1): 60–79, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60, ISSN 1939-1315, PMID 12518971.
  21. ^ a b Sedikides, C; Strube, M (1997), "Self-Evaluation: To Thine Own Self Be Good, To Thine Own Self Be Sure, To Thine Own Self Be True, and To Thine Own Self be Better", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Volume 29, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 29, pp. 209–269, doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60018-0, ISBN 9780120152292, ISSN 0065-2601. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 209-269). New York: Academic Press.
  22. ^ Sedikides, C.; Gregg., A. P. & Hart, C. M. (2007), "The importance of being modest", in C. Sedikides & S. Spencer (eds.), The self: Frontiers in social psychology, New York: Psychology Press, pp. 163–184
  23. ^ Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C.; Funder, DC (2006), "Impulsivity and the self-defeating behaviour of narcissists", Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10 (2): 154–165, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.419.6963, doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_4, PMID 16768652, S2CID 1924100
  24. ^ Mezulis, Amy H.; Abramson, Lyn Y.; Hyde, Janet S.; Hankin, Benjamin L. (2004), "Is There a Universal Positivity Bias in Attributions? A Meta-Analytic Review of Individual, Developmental, and Cultural Differences in the Self-Serving Attributional Bias", Psychological Bulletin, 130 (5): 711–747, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711, ISSN 0033-2909, PMID 15367078.
  25. ^ Schlenker, Barry R.; Miller, Rowland S. (1977), "Egocentrism in groups: Self-serving biases or logical information processing?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 (10): 755–764, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.10.755, ISSN 0022-3514.
  26. ^ Greenberg, J; Pyszczynski, Tom; Solomon, Sheldon (1982), "The self-serving attributional bias: Beyond self-presentation", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18 (1): 56–67, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(82)90081-6, ISSN 0022-1031.
  27. ^ Riess, Marc; Rosenfeld, Paul; Melburg, Valerie; Tedeschi, James T. (1981), "Self-serving attributions: Biased private perceptions and distorted public descriptions", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 (2): 224–231, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.224, ISSN 0022-3514.
  28. ^ Campbell, W. K. & Sedikides, Constantine (1999), "Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration", Review of General Psychology, 3: 23–43, doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23, S2CID 144756539
  29. ^ Zuckerman, M. (1979), "Attribution of success and failure revisited, of: The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory", Journal of Personality, 47 (2): 245–287, doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1979.tb00202.x
  30. ^ Baumeister, R. F.; Stillwell, A & Wotman, SR (1990), "Victim and perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives about anger", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (5): 994–1005, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.994, PMID 2266485, S2CID 28145593
  31. ^ Gonzales, M. H., Pederson, J. H., Manning, D. J., & Wetter, D. W.; Pederson, Julie Haugen; Manning, Debra J.; Wetter, David W. (1990), "Pardon my gaffe: Effects of sex, status and consequence severity on accounts", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (4): 610–621, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.610
  32. ^ a b Pettigrew, T. F. (2001), "The ultimate attribution error: Extending Allport's cognitive analysis of prejudice", in M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (eds.), Intergroup relations: Essential readings, Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis, pp. 162–173
  33. ^ Ciladini, R. B.; Richard j., Borden; Avril, Thorne; Marcus Randall, Walker; Stephen, Freeman & Lloyd Reynolds, Sloan (1976), "Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (3): 366–375, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.3.366
  34. ^ Gramzow, R. H.; Gaertner, L & Sedikides, C (2001), "Memory for ingroup and outgroup information in a minimal group context: The self as an informational base", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (2): 188–205, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.188, PMID 11220440
  35. ^ Baumeister, R. F. & Cairns, KJ (1982), "Repression and self-presentation: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 (5): 851–862, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.851, PMID 1593424
  36. ^ a b Sedikides, C. & Green, JD (2000), "On the self-protective nature of inconsistency/negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6): 906–922, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.906, PMID 11138760
  37. ^ Festinger, L. (1957), A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
  38. ^ Frey, D. (1986), Recent research on selective exposure to information. In L. Bercowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 41-80). New York: Academic Press
  39. ^ a b Sanitioso, R.; Kunda, Z & Fong, GT (1990), "Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (2): 229–241, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.229, PMID 2213492
  40. ^ Murray, S. L. & Holmes, John G. (1993), "Seeing virtues in faults: Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives in close relationships", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (4): 707–722, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.707
  41. ^ Walker, W. R.; Skowronski, John J. & Thompson, Charles P. (2003), "Life is pleasant - and memory helps to keep it that way!", Review of General Psychology, 7 (2): 203–210, doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203, S2CID 43179740
  42. ^ Mischel, W.; Ebbesen, EB & Zeis, AM (1976), "Determinants of selective memory about the self", Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44 (1): 92–103, doi:10.1037/0022-006X.44.1.92, PMID 1245637
  43. ^ van Lange, P.; Rusbult, Caryl E.; Semin-Goossens, Astrid; Gorts, Carien A. & Stalpers, Mirjam (1999), "Being better than others but otherwise perfectly normal: Perceptions of uniqueness and similarity in close relationships", Personal Relationships, 6 (3): 269–289, doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1999.tb00192.x
  44. ^ Gosling, Samuel D.; John, Oliver P.; Craik, Kenneth H.; Robins, Richard W. (1998), "Do people know how they behave? Self-reported act frequencies compared with on-line codings by observers", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (5): 1337–1349, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1337, ISSN 0022-3514, PMID 9599447.
  45. ^ Skowronski, J. J; Betz, Andrew L.; Thompson, Charles P. & Shannon, Laura (1991), "Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (6): 831–843, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.831
  46. ^ Arkin, R. M. & Maruyama, Geoffrey M. (1979), "Attribution, affect and college exam performance", Journal of Educational Psychology, 71: 85–93, doi:10.1037/0022-0663.71.1.85
  47. ^ Greenwald, A. G. (2002), "Constructs in student ratings of instructors", in H. I. Braun & D. N. Douglas (eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 277–297
  48. ^ a b Ditto, P. H. & Boardman, A. F. (1995), "Perceived accuracy of favourable and unfavourable psychological feedback", Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16 (1–2): 137–157, doi:10.1080/01973533.1995.9646106
  49. ^ Pyszczynski, T. & Greenberg, Jeff (1987), toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 20, pp. 297–341, doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60417-7, ISBN 9780120152209
  50. ^ a b Ditto, P. H. & Lopez, David F. (1992), "Motivated skepticism: Use of differential decision criteria for preferred and non-preferred conclusions", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63 (4): 568–584, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.568
  51. ^ Ditto, P. H.; Scepansky, James A.; Munro, Geoffrey D.; Apanovitch, Anne Marie & Lockhart, Lisa K. (1998), "Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75: 53–69, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.53
  52. ^ Buunk, B. P.; Collins, RL; Taylor, SE; Vanyperen, NW & Dakof, GA (1990), "The affective consequences of social comparisons: Either directions has its ups and downs", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (6): 1238–1249, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1238, PMID 2283590
  53. ^ Collins, R. L. (1996), "For better for worse: The impact of upwards social comparison on self-evaluation", Psychological Bulletin, 119: 51–69, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.51
  54. ^ Wheeler, L. (1966), "Motivation as a determinant of upwards social comparison", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2: 27–31, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(66)90062-X
  55. ^ Lockwood, P. & Kunda, Ziva (1997), "Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on self", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73: 91–103, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.578.7014, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91
  56. ^ Wood, J. V. (1989), "Theory and research concerning social comparison of personality attributes", Psychological Bulletin, 106 (2): 231–248, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.456.7776, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231
  57. ^ Collins, R. L. (2000), "Among the better ones: Upward assimilation in social comparison", in J. Suls & L. Wheeler (eds.), Handbook of social comparison, New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum, pp. 159–172
  58. ^ Gruder, C. L. (1971), "Determinants of social comparison choices", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1 (5): 473–489, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(71)90010-2
  59. ^ Miller, D. T.; Turnbull, William & McFarland, Cathy (1988), "Particularistic and universalistic evaluation in the social comparison process", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55 (6): 908–917, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.908
  60. ^ Biernat, M. & Billings, L. S. (2001), "Standards, expectancies and social comparisons", in A. Tesser & N. Schwartz (eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intraindividual processes, Oxford: Blackwell Publishing, pp. 257–283
  61. ^ Suls, J.; Wills, T. A., eds. (1991). Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  62. ^ Crocker, J., Voelkl, K.; Testa, M. & Major, B. (1991), "Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma", Psychological Review, 96 (4): 803–808, doi:10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.608
  63. ^ Tesser, A. (1988), Towards a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behaviour. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 21, pp. 181-227). New York: Academic Press
  64. ^ Beach, S. R. H. & Tesser, Abraham (1993), "Decision making power and marital satisfaction: A self-evaluation maintenance perspective", Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12 (4): 471–494, doi:10.1521/jscp.1993.12.4.471
  65. ^ Pemberton, M. & Sedikides, C (2001), "When do individuals help close others improve? Extending the self-evaluation maintenance model to future comparisons", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (2): 234–246, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.234, PMID 11519929
  66. ^ Tesser, A. & Paulhus, Del (1983), "The definition of self: Private and public self-evaluation maintenance strategies", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 (4): 672–682, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.672
  67. ^ Tesser, A. & Smith, Jonathan (1980), "Some effects of task relevance and friendship on helping: You don't always help the one you like", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16 (6): 582–590, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(80)90060-8
  68. ^ Cantor, N. & Mischel, Walter (1979), "Prototypicality and personality: Effects on free recall and personality impressions", Journal of Research in Personality, 13 (2): 187–205, doi:10.1016/0092-6566(79)90030-8
  69. ^ Dunning, D.; Leuenberger, Ann & Sherman, David A. (1995), "A new look at motivated inference: Are self-serving theories of success a product of motivational forces?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69: 58–68, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.58, S2CID 53358618
  70. ^ Dunning, D.; Perie, M & Story, AL (1991), "Self-serving prototypes of social categories", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (6): 957–968, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.957, PMID 1774633
  71. ^ Kunda, Z. (1987), "Motivated interference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 (4): 636–647, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.636
  72. ^ Dunning, D. & Cohen, Geoffrey L. (1992), "Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities in social judgment", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63 (3): 341–355, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.341
  73. ^ Alicke, M. D.; Loschiavo, FM; Zerbst, J & Zhang, S (1997), "The person who outperforms me is a genius: Maintaining perceived competence in upward social comparison", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (4): 781–789, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.781, PMID 9325593, S2CID 1291407
  74. ^ Ditto, P. H.; Munro, GD; Apanovitch, AM; Scepansky, JA & Lockhart, LK (2003), "Spontaneous skepticism: The interplay or motivation and expectation in responses to favourable and unfavourable medical diagnoses", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29 (9): 1120–1132, doi:10.1177/0146167203254536, PMID 15189608, S2CID 6555808
  75. ^ a b Doosje, B.; Spears, Russell & Koomen, Willem (1995), "When bad isn't all bad: The strategic use of sample information in generalization and stereotyping", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (4): 642–655, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.642
  76. ^ Dunning, D. & Beauregard, Keith S. (2000), "Regulating impressions of others to affirm images of the self", Social Cognition, 18 (2): 198–222, doi:10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.198
  77. ^ Crocker, J.; Major, B. & Steele, C. (1998), "Social stigma", in D. T. Gilbert; S. T. Fiske & G. Lindzey (eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.), New York: McGraw Hill, pp. 504–553
  78. ^ Ross, L.; Greene, David & House, Pamela (1977), "The false consensus effect: An attributional bias in self-perception and social perception processes", Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13 (3): 279–301, doi:10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-X
  79. ^ Mullen, B. & Goethals, George R. (1990), "Social projection, actual consensus and valence", British Journal of Social Psychology, 29 (3): 279–282, doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00907.x
  80. ^ a b Jones, E. E. & Berglas, S. (1978), "Control of attributions about the self through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and underachievement", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4 (2): 200–206, doi:10.1177/014616727800400205, S2CID 146420423
  81. ^ Feick, D. L. & Rhodewalt, Frederick (1997), "The double-edged sword of self-handicapping: Discounting, augmentation, and the protection and enhancement of self-esteem", Motivation and Emotion, 21 (2): 147–163, doi:10.1023/A:1024434600296, S2CID 141218684
  82. ^ a b Rhodewalt, F.; Morf, Carolyn; Hazlett, Susan & Fairfield, Marita (1991), "Self-handicapping: The role of discounting and augmentation in the preservation of self-esteem", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61: 122–131, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.122
  83. ^ Rhodewalt, F. & Fairfield, Marita (1991), "Claimed self-handicaps and the self-handicapper: The relation of reduction in intended effort to performance", Journal of Research in Personality, 25 (4): 402–417, doi:10.1016/0092-6566(91)90030-T
  84. ^ Tice, D. M. & Baumeister, Roy F. (1990), "Self-esteem, self-handicapping and self-presentation: The strategy of inadequate practice", Journal of Personality, 58 (2): 443–464, doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00237.x
  85. ^ Rhodewalt, F.; Sanbonmatsu, D. M.; Tschanz, B.; Feick, D. L. & Waller, A. (1995), "Self-handicapping and interpersonal trade-offs: The effects of claimed self-handicaps on observers' performance evaluations and feedback", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (10): 1042–1050, doi:10.1177/01461672952110005, S2CID 143465519
  86. ^ a b Arkin, R. M. & Oleson, K. C. (1998), "Self-handicapping", in J. Darley & J. Cooper (eds.), Attributional and social interaction: The legacy of Edward E. Jones, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 313–348
  87. ^ Dweck, C. S. (1999), Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press
  88. ^ Rhodewalt, F. & Tragakis, M. (2002), "Self-handicapping and the social self: The costs and rewards of interpersonal self-construction", in J. Forgas & K. Williams (eds.), The social self: Cognitive, interpersonal and intergroup perspectives, Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press, pp. 121–143
  89. ^ a b Ross, S. R.; Canada, Kelli E & Rausch, Marcus K (2002), "Self-handicapping and the Five Factor model of personality: Mediation between neuroticism and conscientiousness", Personality and Individual Differences, 32 (7): 1173–1184, doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00079-4
  90. ^ Giannetti, E. (2001), Lies we live by: The art of self-deception. London: Bloomsbury
  91. ^ McCria, S. M. & Hirt, E. R. (2001), "The role of ability judgments in self-handicapping", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (10): 1378–1389, doi:10.1177/01461672012710013, S2CID 2174958
  92. ^ Zuckerman, M. & Tsai, FF (2005), "Costs of self-handicapping", Journal of Personality, 73 (2): 411–442, doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00314.x, PMID 15745436
  93. ^ Zuckerman, M.; Kieffer, SC & Knee, CR (1998), "Consequences of self-handicapping: Effects on coping, academic performance and adjustment", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (6): 1619–1628, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1619, PMID 9654762
  94. ^ Arndt, J.; Schimel, J.; Greenberg, J. & Pyszczynski, T. (2002), "The intrinsic self and defensiveness: Evidence that activating the intrinsic self reduces self-handicapping and conformity", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28 (5): 671–683, doi:10.1177/0146167202288011, S2CID 145296762
  95. ^ Taylor, S. E. & Brown, JD (1988), "Illusions and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health", Psychological Bulletin, 103 (2): 193–210, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193, PMID 3283814, S2CID 762759
  96. ^ a b Alicke, M. D.; Vredenburg, Debbie S.; Hiatt, Matthew & Govorun, Olesya (2001), "The "better than myself effect"", Motivation and Emotion, 25: 7–22, doi:10.1023/A:1010655705069, S2CID 140785247
  97. ^ Fenton-O'Creevy, M.; Nicholson, Nigel; Soane, Emma & Willman, Paul (2003), "Trading on illusion: Unrealistic perceptions of control and trading performance", Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 53–68, doi:10.1348/096317903321208880
  98. ^ a b Helweg-Larsen, M. & Shepperd, J. A. (2001), "Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature", Personality and Social Psychology Review, 51: 74–95, doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_5, S2CID 30461688
  99. ^ Pronin, E.; Gilovich, T & Ross, L (2004), "Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others", Psychological Review, 111 (3): 781–799, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.781, PMID 15250784
  100. ^ Murray, S. L. (1999), "The quest for conviction: Motivated cognition in romantic relationships", Psychological Inquiry, 10: 23–34, doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1001_3
  101. ^ Rusbult, C. E.; Van Lange, PA; Wildschut, T; Yovetich, NA & Verette, J (2000), "Perceived superiority in close relationships: Why it exists and persists", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (4): 521–545, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.521, hdl:1871/17595, PMID 11045737
  102. ^ Nesselroade, K. P., Beggan, J. K.; Beggan, James K. & Allison, Scott T. (1999), "Possession enhancement in an interpersonal context: An extension of the mere ownership effect", Psychology and Marketing, 16: 21–34, doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199901)16:1<21::AID-MAR2>3.0.CO;2-9
  103. ^ a b Brown, J. (1998), The self. New York: McGraw-Hill
  104. ^ Alicke, M. D. (1985), "Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49 (6): 1621–1630, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621
  105. ^ College, Board (1976-1977), Student descriptive questionnaire, Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service
  106. ^ Dunning, D.; Meyerowitz, Judith A. & Holzberg, Amy D. (1989), "Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (6): 1082–1090, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1082
  107. ^ Kruger, J. & Dunning, D (1999), "Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognising one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (6): 1121–1134, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1121, PMID 10626367
  108. ^ Cross, P. (1977), "Not can but will college teaching be improved?", New Directions for Higher Education, 1977 (17): 1–15, doi:10.1002/he.36919771703
  109. ^ Svenson, O. (1981), "Are we less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers?", Acta Psychologica, 47 (2): 143–151, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(81)90005-6
  110. ^ Preston, C. E. & Harris, S (1965), "Psychology of drivers in traffic accidents", Journal of Applied Psychology, 49 (4): 284–288, doi:10.1037/h0022453, PMID 5826671
  111. ^ Pronin, E.; Yin, D. Y. & Ross, L. (2002), "The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28 (3): 369–381, doi:10.1177/0146167202286008, S2CID 14259317
  112. ^ Langer, E. J. (1975), "The illusion of control", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (2): 311–328, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.311, S2CID 30043741
  113. ^ a b Jenkins, H. M. & Ward, WC (1965), "Judgments of contingency between response and outcome", Psychological Monographs, 79: 1–17, doi:10.1037/h0093874, PMID 14300511
  114. ^ Weinstein, N. D. (1980), "Unrealistic optimism about future events", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (5): 806–829, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806
  115. ^ Weinstein, N. D. & Klein, WM (1995), "Resistance of personal risk perceptions to debiasing manipulation", Health Psychology, 14 (2): 132–140, doi:10.1037/0278-6133.14.2.132, PMID 7789348, S2CID 25474023
  116. ^ Regan, P. C.; Snyder, M. & Kassin, S. M. (1995), "Unrealistic optimism: Self-enhancement or person positivity?", Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21 (10): 1073–1082, doi:10.1177/01461672952110008, S2CID 144119515
  117. ^ Vallone, R. P.; Griffin, DW; Lin, S & Ross, L (1990), "Overconfident predictions of future actions and outcomes by self and others", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (4): 582–592, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.582, PMID 2348360
  118. ^ Buehler, R.; Griffin, Dale & Ross, Michael (1994), "Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (3): 366–381, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.366, S2CID 4222578
  119. ^ Dunning, D.; Griffin, DW; Milojkovic, JD & Ross, L (1990), "The overconfidence effect in social predictions", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (4): 568–581, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.568, PMID 2348359
  120. ^ Smits, T., & Hoorens, V.; Hoorens, Vera (2005), "How probable is probably? It depends on whom you're talking about", Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18 (2): 83–96, doi:10.1002/bdm.485, S2CID 144967476
  121. ^ Weinstein, N. D.; Marcus, SE & Moser, RP (2005), "Smokers' unrealistic optimism about their risk", Tobacco Control, 14 (1): 55–59, doi:10.1136/tc.2004.008375, PMC 1747991, PMID 15735301
  122. ^ Sedikides, C.; Gregg, A. P. & Hart, C. M. (2007), The self: Frontiers in social psychology, New York: Psychology Press, pp. 163–184
  123. ^ Chang, E. C. (Ed.). (2007), Self-criticism and self-enhancement: Theory, research, and clinical implications., Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  124. ^ Gramzow, R. H.; Elliot, Andrew J; Asher, Evan & McGregor, Holly A (2003), "Self-evaluation bias and academic performance: Some ways and some reasons why", Journal of Research in Personality, 37 (2): 41–61, doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00535-4
  125. ^ Young-Hoon, Kim; Chiu, Chiu; Zou, Zhimin (2010), "Know Thyself: Misperceptions of Actual Performance Undermine Achievement Motivation, Future Performance, and Subjective Well-Being", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99 (3): 395–409, doi:10.1037/a0020555, PMID 20804261.
  126. ^ Swann, W. B. Jr.; Chang-Schneider, C & Larsen Mcclarty, K (2007), "Do our self-views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in everyday life", American Psychologist, 62 (2): 84–94, doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.84, PMID 17324034, S2CID 13589535
  127. ^ Trzesniewski, K.; Donnellan, MB; Moffitt, TE; Robins, RW; Poulton, R & Caspi, A (2006), "Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health, criminal behaviour and limited economic prospects during adulthood", Developmental Psychology, 42 (2): 381–390, doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381, PMID 16569175
  128. ^ a b Colvin, C. R.; Block, Jack & Funder, David C. (1995), "Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 (6): 1152–1162, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.6.1152, PMID 7608859
  129. ^ Paulhus, D. L. (1998), "Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (5): 1197–1208, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1197, PMID 9599439
  130. ^ Paulhus, Delroy L.; Aliye Kurt (August 2008), "Moderators of the adaptiveness of self-enhancement: Operationalization, motivational domain, adjustment facet, and evaluator☆", Journal of Research in Personality, 42 (4): 839–853, doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.11.005.
  131. ^ Taylor, S. E.; Lerner, JS; Sherman, DK; Sage, RM & McDowell, NK (2003), "Portrait of the self-enhancer: Well adjusted and well liked or maladjusted and friendless?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (1): 165–176, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.165, PMID 12518977
  132. ^ Bonanno, G. A.; Rennicke, C & Dekel, S (2005), "Self-enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social maladjustment?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (6): 984–998, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.984, PMID 15982117
  133. ^ van Lange, P. A. M. & Sedikides, Constantine (1998), "Being more honest but not necessarily more intelligent than others: Generality and explanations for the Muhammad Ali effect" (PDF), European Journal of Social Psychology, 28 (4): 675–680, doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199807/08)28:4<675::AID-EJSP883>3.0.CO;2-5
  134. ^ Morf, C. C., Carolyn C. & Rhodewalt, Frederick (2001), "Unraveling the Paradoxes of Narcissism: A Dynamic Self-Regulatory Processing Model", Psychological Inquiry, 12 (4): 177–196, doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1, S2CID 2004430
  135. ^ a b c Sedikides, C.; Herbst, KC; Hardin, DP & Dardis, GJ (2002), "Accountability as a deterrent to self-enhancement: The search for mechanisms", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (3): 592–605, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.592, PMID 12219856
  136. ^ a b c Tice, D. M.; Butler, J. L.; Muraven, M. B. & Stillwell, A. M. (1995), "When modesty prevails: Differential favourability of self-presentation to friends and strangers", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (6): 443–464, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1120
  137. ^ Gilbert, D. T.; Pinel, E. C.; Wilson, T. D.; Blumberg, S. J. & Wheatley, T. P. (1998), "Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (3): 617–638, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.617, PMID 9781405
  138. ^ Trope, Y. & Neter, E (1994), "Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: The role of self-control in feedback seeking", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (4): 646–657, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.646, PMID 8189345
  139. ^ Blaine, B. & Crocker, J. (1993), "Self-esteem and self-serving biases in reactions to positive and negative events: An integrative review", in R. F. Baumeister (ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard, New York: Plenum Press, pp. 55–85
  140. ^ Kuiper, Nicholas A. (1978), "Depression and causal attributions for success and failure", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 (3): 236–246, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.236, ISSN 0022-3514, PMID 650382.
  141. ^ a b Campbell, Jennifer D.; Fehr, Beverley (1990), "Self-esteem and perceptions of conveyed impressions: Is negative affectivity associated with greater realism?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (1): 122–133, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.1.122, ISSN 0022-3514, PMID 2308069.
  142. ^ Lewinsohn, Peter M.; Mischel, Walter; Chaplin, William; Barton, Russell (1980), "Social competence and depression: The role of illusory self-perceptions", Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89 (2): 203–212, doi:10.1037/0021-843X.89.2.203, ISSN 0021-843X, PMID 7365132.
  143. ^ Alloy, L. B. (1988), Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression(pp. 223-265). New York: Guilford Press
  144. ^ Fincham, F. D. & Bradbury, T. N. (1989), "The impact of attributions in marriage: An individual difference analysis", Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6: 69–85, doi:10.1177/026540758900600105, S2CID 145745264
  145. ^ Clark, M. S. & Mills, Judson (1979), "Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37: 12–24, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.12
  146. ^ Murray, S. L.; Holmes, J. G.; Griffin, D. W. (1996a) (1996), "The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70: 79–98, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.79
  147. ^ Fiske, A. P.; Kitayama, S.; Markus, H. R. & Nisbett, R. E. (1998), "The cultural matrix of social psychology", in D. T. Gilbert; S. T. Fiske & G. Lindzey (eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.), Boston, MA: McGraw Hill, pp. 915–981
  148. ^ Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, Shinobu (1991), "Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation", Psychological Review, 98 (2): 224–253, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
  149. ^ Triandis, H. C. & Suh, EM (2002), "Cultural influences on personality", Annual Review of Psychology, 53: 133–160, doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135200, PMID 11752482
  150. ^ Kitayama, S.; Markus, HR; Matsumoto, H; Norasakkunkit, V (1997), "Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72 (6): 1245–1267, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245, PMID 9177018
  151. ^ a b Kitayama, S.; Markus, H. R. & Lieberman, C. (1995a), "The collective construction of self-esteem: Implications for culture, self and emotion", in R. Russell; J. Fernandez-Dols; T. Manstead & J. Wellenkamp (eds.), Everyday conception of emotion: An introduction to the psychology, anthropology and linguistics of emotion, Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic, pp. 523–550
  152. ^ Kanagawa, C.; Cross, S. E. & Markus, H. R. (2001), ""Who am I?": The cultural psychology of the conceptual self", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27: 90–103, doi:10.1177/0146167201271008, S2CID 145634514
  153. ^ Takata, T. (1987), "Self-depreciative tendencies in self-evaluation through social comparison", Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27: 27–36, doi:10.2130/jjesp.27.27
  154. ^ Heine, S. J. & Lehman, Darrin R. (1995), "Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the west feel more invulnerable than the east?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 (4): 595–607, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.595
  155. ^ Kitayama, S.; Takagi, H. & Matsumoto, H. (1995b), "Seiko to shippai no kiin: nihonteki jiko no bunkashinrigaku" [Causal attributions of success and failure: Cultural psychology of Japanese selves], Japanese Psychological Review, 38: 247–280
  156. ^ Heine, S. H.; Lehman, DR; Markus, HR & Kitayama, S (1999), "Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?", Psychological Review, 106 (4): 766–794, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766, PMID 10560328
  157. ^ Heine, S. J.; Takata, T. & Lehman, D. R. (2000), "Beyond self-presentation: Evidence for self-criticism among Japanese", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26: 71–78, doi:10.1177/0146167200261007, S2CID 40878324
  158. ^ Heine, S. J.; Kitayama, S. & Lehman, D. R. (2001a) (2001), "Cultural Differences in Self-Evaluation: Japanese Readily Accept Negative Self-Relevant Information", Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (4): 434–443, doi:10.1177/0022022101032004004, S2CID 40475406
  159. ^ a b Heine, S. J.; Kitayama, S.; Lehman, D. R.; Takata, T.; Ide, E.; Leung, C. & Matsumoto, H. (2001b) (2001), "Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (4): 599–615, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599, PMID 11642348
  160. ^ Yik, M. S. M.; Bond, M. H. & Paulhus, D. L. (1998), "Do Chinese self-enhance or self-efface? It's a matter of domain", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 (4): 399–406, doi:10.1177/0146167298244006, S2CID 145725445
  161. ^ Kurman, J. (2001), "Self-enhancement: Is it restricted to individualistic cultures?", Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (12): 1705–1716, doi:10.1177/01461672012712013, S2CID 145763977
  162. ^ Sedikides, C.; Campbell, W. Keith; Reeder, Glenn D. & Elliot, Andrew J. (2002), W. Strobe & M. Hewstone (eds.), "The self in relationships: Whether, how and when close others put the self "in its place"" (PDF), European Review of Social Psychology, 12: 237–265, doi:10.1080/14792772143000076, S2CID 146180762
  163. ^ Markus, H. R. & Nurius, Paula (1986), "Possible selves", American Psychologist, 41 (9): 954–969, doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954
  164. ^ Sedikides, C. (1999), "A multiplicity of motives: The case of self-improvement", Psychological Inquiry, 9: 64–65, doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1001_10
  165. ^ Falbo, T.; Poston, D. L.; Triscari, R. S. & Zhang, X. (1997), "Self-enhancing illusions among Chinese schoolchildren", Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28 (2): 172–191, doi:10.1177/0022022197282003, S2CID 145500149
  166. ^ Fahr, J.; Dobbins, Gregory H. & Cheng, BOR-Shiuan (1991), "Cultural relativity in action: A comparison of self-ratings made by Chinese and U.S. workers", Personnel Psychology, 44: 129–147, doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00693.x, S2CID 144606718
  167. ^ Gaertner, L.; Sedikides, C. & Chang, K. (2008), "On pancultural self-enhancement: Well-adjusted Taiwanese self-enhance on personally valued traits" (PDF), Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39 (4): 463–477, doi:10.1177/0022022108318431, S2CID 30238954
  168. ^ Heine, S. J.; Kitayama, Shinobu & Hamamura, Takeshi (2007), "Inclusion of additional studies yields different conclusions: Comment on Sedikides, Gaertner, & Vevea (2005), Journal of Personality and Social Psychology", Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10 (2): 49–58, doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00211.x, hdl:2027.42/75100
  169. ^ Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L.; Gaertner, Lowell; Vevea, Jack L. (2007), "Inclusion of theory relevant moderators yield the same conclusions as Sedikides, Gaertner and Vevea (2005): A meta-analytical reply to Heine, Kitayama and Hamamura (2007)" (PDF), Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10 (2): 59–67, doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00212.x
  170. ^ Heine, S. J.; Kitayama, S. & Hamamura, T. (2007b) (2007), "Which studies test whether self-enhancement is pancultural? Reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea, 2007", Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10 (3): 198–200, doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00226.x, hdl:2027.42/75225
  171. ^ Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L.; Gaertner, L; Vevea, JL (2005), "Pancultural self-enhancement reloaded: A meta-analytic reply to Heine(2005)", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (4): 539–551, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.550.7957, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.539, PMID 16287417
  172. ^ Heine, S. J. (2005), "Where is the evidence for pancultural self-enhancement? A reply to Sedikides, Gaertner & Toguchi (2003)", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (4): 531–538, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.531, PMID 16287416
  173. ^ Sedikides, Constantine (1993), "Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 (2): 317–338, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.317, ISSN 1939-1315.
  174. ^ Paulhus, D. L.; Graf, Peter & Van Selst, Mark (1989), "Attentional load increases the positivity of self-presentation", Social Cognition, 7 (4): 389–400, doi:10.1521/soco.1989.7.4.389
  175. ^ Paulhus, D. L. & Levitt, Karen (1987), "Desirable responding triggered by affect: Automatic egotism?", Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (2): 245–259, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.245
  176. ^ Roese, N. J. & Olson, JM (2007), "Better, stronger, faster: Self-serving judgment, affect regulation, and the optimal vigilance hypothesis", Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2 (2): 124–141, doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00033.x, PMC 2429993, PMID 18552989

Sources

Further reading

  • Edward Chin-Ho Chang: Self-Criticism and Self-Enhancement. American Psychological Association, 2008 ISBN 978-1-4338-0115-0
  • Hogg, Michael A.; Cooper, Joel (2003), The Sage Handbook of Social Psychology, Sage, ISBN 978-0-7619-6636-4
  • Mark R Leary & June Price Tangney: Handbook of Self and Identity. Guilford Press, 2005 ISBN 978-1-59385-237-5

External links

  • (videos)
  • Home Page;
  • Home Page of Steven Heine
  • University of Southampton's Home Page
  • .

self, enhancement, type, motivation, that, works, make, people, feel, good, about, themselves, maintain, self, esteem, this, motive, becomes, especially, prominent, situations, threat, failure, blows, self, esteem, involves, preference, positive, over, negativ. Self enhancement is a type of motivation that works to make people feel good about themselves and to maintain self esteem 1 This motive becomes especially prominent in situations of threat failure or blows to one s self esteem 2 3 4 Self enhancement involves a preference for positive over negative self views 5 It is one of the three self evaluation motives along with self assessment the drive for an accurate self concept and self verification the drive for a self concept congruent with one s identity Self evaluation motives drive the process of self regulation that is how people control and direct their own actions There are a variety of strategies that people can use to enhance their sense of personal worth For example they can downplay skills that they lack or they can criticise others to seem better by comparison These strategies are successful in that people tend to think of themselves as having more positive qualities and fewer negative qualities than others 6 Although self enhancement is seen in people with low self esteem as well as with high self esteem these two groups tend to use different strategies People who already have high esteem enhance their self concept directly by processing new information in a biased way People with low self esteem use more indirect strategies for example by avoiding situations in which their negative qualities will be noticeable 7 There are controversies over whether or not self enhancement is beneficial to the individual and over whether self enhancement is culturally universal or specific to Western individualism Contents 1 Levels 2 Dimensions 2 1 Self advancement vs self protection 2 2 Public vs private 2 3 Central vs peripheral 2 4 Candid vs tactical 3 Types 3 1 Self serving attribution bias 3 2 Selectivity 3 2 1 Selective memory 3 2 2 Selective acceptance and refutation 3 3 Strategies 3 3 1 Strategic social comparisons 3 3 1 1 Upward social comparisons 3 3 1 2 Downward social comparisons 3 3 1 3 Lateral social comparisons 3 3 1 4 Self evaluation maintenance theory 3 3 2 Strategic construal 3 4 Behavioural self handicapping 4 Outcomes 4 1 Self enhancing triad 4 1 1 Above average effect 4 1 2 Illusions of control 4 1 3 Unrealistic optimism 4 2 Benefits and costs to the individual 5 Constraints 5 1 Plausibility 5 2 Mood 5 3 Social context and relationships 5 4 Culture 5 5 Other motives 5 6 Other factors 6 See also 7 References 7 1 Notes 7 2 Sources 8 Further reading 9 External linksLevels EditSelf enhancement can occur in many different situations and under many different guises The general motive of self enhancement can have many differing underlying explanations each of which becomes more or less dominant depending on the situation The explanations of the self enhancement motive can occur in different combinations Self enhancement can occur as an underlying motive or personality trait without occurring as an observed effect Levels of self enhancementObserved effectSelf enhancement at the level of an observed effect describes the product of the motive For example self enhancement can produce inflated self ratings positive illusions Such ratings would be self enhancement manifested as an observed effect It is an observable instance of the motive Ongoing processSelf enhancement at the level of an ongoing process describes the actual operation of the motive For example self enhancement can result in attributing favourable outcomes to the self and unfavourable outcomes to others self serving attribution bias The actual act of attributing such ratings would be self enhancement manifested as an ongoing process It is the motive in operation Personality traitSelf enhancement at the level of a personality trait describes habitual or inadvertent self enhancement For example self enhancement can cause situations to be created to ease the pain of failure self handicapping The fabrication of such situations or excuses frequently and without awareness would be self enhancement manifested as a personality trait It is the repetitive inclination to demonstrate the motive Underlying motiveSelf enhancement at the level of an underlying motive describes the conscious desire to self enhance For example self enhancement can cause the comparison of the self to a worse other making the self seem greater in comparison strategic social comparisons The act of comparing intentionally to achieve superiority would be self enhancement manifested as an underlying motive It is the genuine desire to see the self as superior The four levels of self enhancement manifestation as defined by Sedikides amp Gregg 2008 5 vteDimensions EditBoth the extent and the type of self enhancement vary across a number of dimensions 5 Self advancement vs self protection Edit Self enhancement can occur by either self advancing or self protecting that is either by enhancing the positivity of one s self concept or by reducing the negativity of one s self concept 8 Self protection appears to be the stronger of the two motives given that avoiding negativity is of greater importance than encouraging positivity 9 However as with all motivations there are differences between individuals For example people with higher self esteem appear to favour self advancement whereas people with lower self esteem tend to self protect 10 This highlights the role of risk to not defend oneself against negativity in favour of self promotion offers the potential for losses whereas whilst one may not gain outright from self protection one does not incur the negativity either People high in self esteem tend to be greater risk takers and therefore opt for the more risky strategy of self advancement whereas those low in self esteem and risk taking hedge their bets with self protection 11 Public vs private Edit Self enhancement can occur in private or in public 12 Public self enhancement is obvious positive self presentation 13 whereas private self enhancement is unnoticeable except to the individual 14 The presence of other people i e in public self enhancement can either augment or inhibit self enhancement 15 16 Whilst self enhancement may not always take place in public it is nevertheless still influenced by the social world for example via social comparisons 17 Central vs peripheral Edit Potential areas of self enhancement differ in terms how important or central they are to a person 18 Self enhancement tends to occur more in the domains that are the most important to a person and less in more peripheral less important domains 19 20 Candid vs tactical Edit Self enhancement can occur either candidly or tactically 21 Candid self enhancement serves the purpose of immediate gratification whereas tactical self enhancement can result in potentially larger benefits from delayed gratification Tactical self enhancement is often preferred over candid self enhancement as overt self enhancement is socially displeasing for those around it 22 Narcissism is an exemplification of extreme candid self enhancement 23 Types EditSelf enhancement does not just occur at random Its incidence is often highly systematic and can occur in any number of ways in order to achieve its goal of inflating perceptions of the self Importantly we are typically unaware that we are self enhancing Awareness of self enhancing processes would highlight the facade we are trying to create revealing that the self we perceive is in fact an enhanced version of our actual self Self serving attribution bias Edit Main article Self serving bias Self enhancement can also affect the causal explanations people generate for social outcomes People have a tendency to exhibit a self serving attribution bias that is to attribute positive outcomes to one s internal disposition but negative outcomes to factors beyond one s control e g others chance or circumstance 24 In short people claim credit for their successes but deny responsibilities for their failures The self serving attribution bias is very robust occurring in public as well as in private 25 26 even when a premium is placed on honesty 27 People most commonly manifest a self serving bias when they explain the origin or events in which they personally had a hand or a stake 28 29 Explanations for moral transgressions follow similar self serving patterns 30 31 as do explanations for group behaviour 32 The ultimate attribution error 32 is the tendency to regard negative acts by one s out group and positive acts by one s in group as essential to their nature i e attributable to their internal disposition and not a product of external factors This may reflect the operation of the self serving bias refracted through social identification 33 34 Selectivity Edit Selective memory Edit Main article Mnemic neglect Selectivity within information processingSelective attentionPeople typically avoid attending to negative unflattering information at encoding 35 36 therefore its initial recognition is impaired Selective attention manifests itself in the form of an overt behaviour via selective exposure Selective exposurePeople selectively expose themselves to information that justifies important prior decisions they have made 37 This holds true so long as the information appears to be valid and the decision that was made was done so freely and is irreversible 38 Selective recallAt retrieval people bring to mind a highly biased collection of memories Selective recall occurs for behaviours that exemplify desirable personality traits 39 harmonious interpersonal relationships 40 or even health enhancing habits Affect associated with unpleasant memories also fades faster than affect associated with pleasant memories 41 vtePeople sometimes self enhance by selectively remembering their strengths rather than weaknesses This pattern of selective forgetting has been described as mnemic neglect Mnemic neglect may reflect biases in the processing of information at either encoding retrieval or retention Biases at encoding occur via selective attention and selective exposure Biases at retrieval and retention occur via selective recall The role of mnemic neglect can be emphasised or reduced by the characteristics of a certain behaviour or trait For example after receiving false feedback pertaining to a variety of behaviours participants recalled more positive behaviours than negative ones but only when the behaviours exemplified central not peripheral traits and only when feedback pertained to the self and not to others 36 Similar findings emerge when the to be recalled information is personality traits 42 relationship promoting or undermining behaviours 43 frequencies of social acts 44 and autobiographical memories 45 Selective acceptance and refutation Edit Selective acceptance involves taking as fact self flattering or ego enhancing information with little regard for its validity Selective refutation involves searching for plausible theories that enable criticism to be discredited A good example of selective acceptance and refutation in action would be Selective acceptance is the act of accepting as valid an examination on which one has performed well without consideration of alternatives whereas selective refutation would be mindfully searching for reasons to reject as invalid an examination on which one has performed poorly 46 47 Concordant with selective acceptance and refutation is the observation that people hold a more critical attitude towards blame placed upon them but a more lenient attitude to praise that they receive 48 49 People will strongly contest uncongenial information but readily accept at without question congenial information 50 51 Strategies Edit Strategic social comparisons Edit The potential directions for strategic social comparisons The social nature of the world we live in means that self evaluation cannot take place in an absolute nature comparison to other social beings is inevitable Many social comparisons occur automatically as a consequence of circumstance for example within an exam sitting social comparisons of intellect may occur to those sitting the same exam However the strength of the self enhancement motive can cause the subjective exploitation of scenarios in order to give a more favourable outcome to the self in comparisons between the self and others Such involuntary social comparisons prompt self regulatory strategies Self esteem moderates the beneficial evaluative consequences of comparisons to both inferior and superior others People with higher self esteem are more optimistic about both evading the failures and misfortunes of their inferiors and about securing the successes and good fortunes of their superiors 52 Upward social comparisons Edit An upwards social comparison involves comparing oneself to an individual perceived to be superior to or better than oneself Upwards social comparison towards someone felt to be similar to oneself can induce self enhancement through assimilation of the self and other s characteristics 53 however this only occurs when The gap between the self and the comparison target is not too large 54 The skill or success being compared is attainable 55 The comparison target is perceived as a competitor 56 Where assimilation does not occur as a result of a social comparison contrast can instead occur which can lead to upwards social comparisons providing inspiration 57 Downward social comparisons Edit Even though upwards social comparisons are the most common social comparisons 58 59 people do sometimes make downwards social comparisons Downwards social comparisons involve comparing oneself to an individual perceived to be inferior to or less skilled than the self Downwards social comparisons serve as a form of ego defence whereby the ego is inflated due to the sense of superiority gained from such downwards social comparisons 60 61 Lateral social comparisons Edit Lateral social comparisons comparisons against those perceived as equal to the self can also be self enhancing Comparisons with members of one s in group can lead be protective against low self esteem especially when the in group are disadvantaged 62 Self evaluation maintenance theory Edit Main article Self evaluation maintenance theory Self enhancement waxes and wanes as a function of one s ability level in the context of interpersonal relationships and this in turn influences interpersonal attitudes and behaviours Three factors influence the self evaluations people make 63 Closeness of a relationship comparison of one s own performance with that of another is more likely to occur and when it does is more consequential when others are close rather than distant Personal relevance of a particular ability when the domain is not relevant to oneself reflection will occur and when the domain is relevant comparison will occur Reflection one will undergo self enhancement pride when the other does well but self derogation shame when the other does poorly Comparison one will undergo self derogation humiliation if others perform well but self enhancement triumph if the other performs poorly Level of performance in that ability domain People adopt a variety of coping strategies to deal with the pressures of self evaluation Choose friends and partners who excel but not in the same domains as they do 64 Withhold information that is likely to improve the performance of others of personally relevant domains 65 Alter the relevance of performance domains by changing their self concept thus moderating the impact of the reflection and comparison processes 66 Broaden or narrow the gap between the oneself and others even by deliberately altering the difficulty of domain relevant tasks 67 Strategic construal Edit The concepts that people use to understand themselves and their social world are relatively vague 68 Consequently when making social comparisons or estimations people can easily and subtly shift their construal of the meaning of those concepts in order to self enhance Strategic construals typically increase following negative feedback 69 Numerous examples of strategic construals exist a small selection include People s interpretation of what counts as a virtue or talent is biased in favour of the attributes they possess and of what counts as a vice or deficiency in favour of attributes they lack 70 People rate personality feedback and scientific research as less credible if it implies they are susceptible to disease 48 71 Lazy people perceive the rest of the world as reasonably fit and healthy whereas frequent exercisers see their athleticism as a single unique attribute 72 Low achievers in a particular area are likely to perceive the successes of high achievers as exceptional thereby lessening the shame of their own inability 73 People think harder about any discouraging test results they receive will spend longer thinking about them are more inclined to have them confirmed and are significantly more skeptical of them 50 People do not react the same way to test results received by others however 74 When research tarnishes the reputation of groups with which people identify they search for a statistical weakness of that research 75 Strategic construals can also be more subtle People make self aggrandizing interpretations not only of their own attributes but also of others in order to appear greater by comparison Strategic construals appear to operate around one s self esteem After either positive or negative feedback people with high self esteem alter their perceptions of others typically varying their perceptions of others ability and performance in a self enhancing direction 76 Those with low self esteem however do not Self esteem level appears to moderate the use of strategic construals As well as operating as a function of self esteem level strategic construals also appear to protect self esteem levels For example members of minority groups who perform poorly in academic settings due to negative cultural attitudes towards them subsequently disengage psychologically from and dissidentify with academic pursuits in general Whilst buffering their self esteem level they jeopardise their future socioeconomic prospects 77 Strategic construals also influence the degree to which categories are believed to characterise other people There is a general tendency to assume that others share one s own characteristics 78 Nevertheless people reliably overestimate the prevalence of their shortcomings e g show enhanced false consensus effect and underestimate the prevalence of their strengths e g show a contrary false uniqueness effect 79 People perceive their flaws as relatively commonplace but their skills as unique Behavioural self handicapping Edit Main article Self handicapping Behavioural self handicapping is the act of erecting obstacles in the path of task success in order to reduce the evaluative implications that can be drawn from task performance 80 This permits self enhancement to occur in two ways 81 In the case of failure self handicapping can protect self esteem by attributing failure to obstacles that one has erected discounting In the case of success self handicapping can promote self esteem by attributing success to oneself despite the obstacles one has erected augmenting People low in self esteem opt for discounting as a self protective route to avoid being perceived as incompetent whereas people high in self esteem preferentially select augmenting as a method of self promotion to enhance their perceived competence 10 82 Self handicapping whilst predominantly a behaviour that occurs in private performance 83 is magnified in public situations 84 However self handicapping is highly risky in social situations If found out those who use it face the negative evaluation and criticism of others 85 Factors promoting behavioural self handicappingTask familiarity Uncertainty over ability to obtain a positive outcome due to experience of limited control over a similar task 86 Task complexity Holding a very fixed concrete theory of the complexity of a task 87 Insecurity Uncertainty over ability to obtain a positive outcome due to generally insecure sense of self 86 Belief Holding the belief that improvement is physically possible Importance A task or evaluation has to be important to the self in order for self handicapping to occur Feedback Negative feedback makes self handicapping more probable as it allows any damage to the ego to be rectified 88 Neuroticism High neuroticism promotes discounting 89 Conscientiousness Low conscientiousness can increase the tendency to self handicap 89 Regardless of the causes of self handicapping the self defeating end result remains the same the integrity and quality of a task outcome or evaluation is compromised in order that the meaning of that outcome appears more agreeable Behavioural self handicapping is a good demonstration of active self deception 90 Whilst task performance is important to people they do sometimes act in ways so as to paradoxically impair task performance 80 either to protect against the shame of performing poorly by creating a convenient excuse discounting or to enhance themselves by succeeding despite adversity by creating grounds for conceit augmenting 82 Furthermore self handicapping can have unintentional adverse consequences Whilst allowing the maintenance of positive self views 91 self handicapping has the cost of impairing objective performance 92 Students who report frequent use self handicapping strategies underperform relative to their aptitude with poor examination preparation mediating the effect 93 Ultimately those who readily prepare themselves for the possibility of poor task performance beforehand use the strategy of discounting less 94 Outcomes EditThe effect of self enhancement strategies is shown in the tendency of people to see themselves as having more positive qualities and a more positive future outlook than others 6 Self enhancing triad Edit Main article Positive illusions People generally hold unrealistically positive views about themselves Such flattering views can often be neatly categorised within what has become known as the Triad of Positive Illusions 95 The three illusions in question are above average effect 96 illusions of control 97 and unrealistic optimism 98 These illusions can be replicated across many situations and are highly resistant to revision Rather ironically when informed of the existence of such illusions people generally consider themselves to be less prone to them than others 99 Above average effect Edit Main article Illusory superiority The better than average effect is the most common demonstration of an above average effect It is a highly robust effect as evidenced by the fact that even when the criteria on which the self and others are judged are identical the self is still perceived more favourably 96 Things close to the self also take on the perceived superiority of the above average effect People value both their close relationships 100 101 and their personal possessions 102 above those of others However where an outcome is perceived as highly skilled people often err on the side of caution and display a worse than average effect The majority of people would rate themselves as below average in unicycling ability for example The three related divisions of the self enhancing triad The illusory nature of the above average effect comes from the fact that not everyone can be above average otherwise the average would not be the average The majority of people rating themselves as being better than the majority of people does not quite seem plausible and in some situations is 100 impossible Where a distribution is symmetrical i e mean median mode it is statistically impossible for the majority of people to be above average as whichever of the three averages is taken all are equal to the 50th percentile 103 In a non symmetrical distribution i e mean lt median lt mode or mode lt median lt mean it is statistically impossible for the majority of people to be above average when the average is taken to be the median as the median represents the 50th percentile or the midpoint of the data 103 However in a non symmetrical distribution where the average is taken to be either the mean or the mode the above average effect can be statistically plausible In some situations the majority of people can be above average People show self enhancement in the form of the above average effect in many different ways It is typical for people to profess to be above average at a task yielding positive or desirable outcomes and below average at a task yielding negative or undesirable outcomes Some of the wide variety of documented examples of the above average effect include observations that Most university students regard themselves as well above the 50th percentile in exhibiting social grace athletic prowess and leadership abilities 104 105 106 Even 12th percentile achievers in domains such as grammar and logic consider themselves to be of 62nd percentile achievement 107 94 of university professors believe their teaching ability to be above average 108 University students in the UK and the US regard themselves as above average drivers 109 Even drivers hospitalised after causing accidents persist in believing they are no worse than regular drivers 110 Even when informed about the above average effect people rate themselves as less susceptible to such biases than others 111 Illusions of control Edit Main article Illusion of control People overestimate the level of control they have over outcomes and contingencies 112 seeing their actions as influential even when they are in fact inconsequential 113 Also people stand by their apparent conviction that they can influence the outcomes of inherently random systems for example lotteries especially when such systems possess features typically associated with skill based tasks Even when a degree of contingency does exist between actions and outcomes people still reliably overestimate the strength of that contingency 113 Unrealistic optimism Edit Main article Optimism bias People typically believe that their life will hold a greater number of positive experiences and fewer negative experiences than the lives of similar others 98 114 115 They have the same unrealistic optimism but to a lesser degree for others who are closely linked such as romantic partners and close personal friends 116 Unrealistic optimism is apparent in people s behaviours and beliefs across many different situations People can both overestimate their ability to predict the future 117 and underestimate how long it will take them to complete a variety of tasks 118 People also overestimate the accuracy of their social predictions 119 and interpret probability adverbs to award higher values for personal positive outcomes and lower values for personal negative outcomes 120 Smokers rather alarmingly underestimate their risk of cancer relative to both non smokers and even in comparison with fellow smokers 121 Benefits and costs to the individual Edit There is controversy over whether self enhancement is adaptive or maladaptive 122 123 A single operationalisation of self enhancement can be influenced by a variety of motives and thus can be coordinated with both positive and negative outcomes 124 Those who misperceive their performance self enhancers and self effacers tend to have a lower academic achievement lower subsequent performance These results appear to be culturally universal 125 Surely it s a false assumption to relate self enhancement to depression If self enhancement is taken to mean rendering more positive judgments of oneself than of others then outcomes are frequently favourable 126 127 If self enhancement is taken to mean the rendering of more positive judgements of oneself than others render then outcomes are often untoward 128 129 Which definition is better at measuring self enhancement has been disputed as rating oneself more positively than one rates others is not seen as self enhancement by some researchers 130 In some studies self enhancement has been shown to have strong positive links with good mental health 131 and in others with bad mental health 128 Self enhancing can also have social costs Whilst promoting resilience amongst survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks those who self enhanced were rated as having decreased social adaptation and honesty by friends and family 132 Constraints EditPlausibility Edit Self enhancement thrives upon the vagueness or ambiguity of evidence Where criteria are rigidly defined self enhancement typically reduces For example the above average effect decreases as clarity and definition of the defined trait increases 133 The easier it is to verify a behaviour or trait the less that trait will be subject to self enhancement The plausibility of a trait or characteristic given real world evidence moderates the degree to which the self enhancement of that trait occurs Selectively recalling instances of desirable traits is moderated by one s actual standing on those traits in reality 39 When plausibility reduces the impact of self enhancement undesirable evidence often has to be accepted albeit reluctantly This typically occurs when all possible interpretations of the evidence in question have been made 75 The reason for this unwilling acceptance is to maintain effective social functioning where unqualified self aggrandizement would otherwise prevent it 134 People will continue to self enhance so long as they think they can get away with it 135 136 The constraint of plausibility on self enhancement exists because self enhancing biases cannot be exploited Self enhancement works only under the assumption of rationality to admit to self enhancing totally undermines any conclusions one can draw and any possibility of believing its facade since according to legit rational processes it functions as a genuinely verifiable and accredited improvement 137 Mood Edit Both positive and negative moods can reduce the presence of the self enhancement motive The effects of mood on self enhancement can be explained by a negative mood making the use self enhancing tactics harder and a positive mood making their use less necessary in the first place The onset of a positive mood can make people more receptive to negative diagnostic feedback Past successes are reviewed with expectation of receiving such positive feedback presumably to buffer their mood 138 Depression has quite a well evidenced link with a decrease in the motive to self enhance Depressives are less able to self enhance in response to negative feedback than non depressive controls 139 140 Having a depressive disposition decreases the discrepancy between one s own estimates of one s virtues and the estimates of a neutral observer namely by increasing modesty 141 142 Illusions of control are moderated by melancholy 143 However whilst the self ratings of depressives are more in line with those of neutral observers than the self ratings of normals the self ratings of normals are more in line with those of friends and family than the self ratings of depressives 141 Social context and relationships Edit The presence of the motive to self enhance is dependent on many social situations and the relationships shared with the people in them Many different materialisations of self enhancement can occur depending on such social contexts The self enhancement motive is weaker during interactions with close and significant others When friends or previous strangers whose intimacy levels have been enhanced cooperate on a task they do not exhibit a self serving attribution bias Casual acquaintances and true strangers however do exhibit a self serving attribution bias 135 Where no self serving bias is exhibited in a relationship a betrayal of trust in the relationship will reinstate the self serving bias This corresponds to findings that relationship satisfaction is inversely correlated with the betrayal of trust 144 Both mutual liking and expectation of reciprocity appear to mediate graciousness in the presence of others 145 Whilst people have a tendency to self present boastfully in front of strangers this inclination disappears in the presence of friends 136 Others close to the self are generally more highly evaluated than more distant others 146 Culture Edit Psychological functioning is moderated by the influence of culture 147 148 149 There is much evidence to support a culture specific view of self enhancement Westerners typically Easterners typically Prioritise intradependence Prioritise interdependencePlace greater importance on individualistic values Place greater importance on collectivistic valuesHave more inflated ratings of their own merits Have less inflated ratings of their own merits 150 Emphasise internal attributes Emphasise relational attributes 151 Show self enhancement that overshadows self criticism Show self criticism that overshadows self enhancement 151 Give spontaneously more positive self descriptions Give spontaneously more negative self descriptions 152 Make fewer self deprecatory social comparisons Make more self deprecatory social comparisons 153 Hold more unrealistically optimistic views of the future Hold fewer unrealistically optimistic views of the future 154 Display a self serving attributional bias Do not display a self serving attributional bias 155 Show a weak desire to self improve via self criticism Show a strong desire to self improve via self criticism 156 Are eager to conclude better performance than a classmate Are reluctant to conclude better performance than a classmate 157 Reflexively discount negative feedback Readily acknowledge negative feedback 158 Persist more after initial success Persist more after initial failure 159 Consider tasks in which they succeed to be most diagnostic Consider tasks in which they fail to be most diagnostic 159 Self enhance on the majority of personality dimensions Self enhance only on some personality dimensions 160 Self enhance on individualistic attributes Self enhance on collectivist attributes 20 161 Self enhancement appears to be a phenomenon largely limited to Western cultures where social ties are looser than in the East This is concordant with empirical evidence highlighting relationship closeness as a constraint on self enhancement 162 The self improvement motive as an aspiration towards a possible self 163 may also moderate a variety of psychological processes in both independent and interdependent cultures 164 There are nevertheless signs that self enhancement is not completely absent in interdependent cultures Chinese schoolchildren rate themselves highly on the dimension of competence 165 and Taiwanese employees rate themselves more favourably than their employers do 166 both of which show self enhancing tendencies in Eastern cultures One possible explanation for the observed differences in self enhancement between cultures is that they may occur through differences in how candidly of tactically the motive to self enhance is acted upon and not due to variations in the strength of motive 21 Alternatively self enhancement may be represented only in terms of the characteristics that are deemed important by individuals as they strive to fulfil their culturally prescribed roles The issue over whether self enhancement is universal or specific to Western cultures has been contested within modern literature by two researchers Constantine Sedikides and Steven Heine Sedikides argues that self enhancement is universal and that different cultures self enhance in domains important in their culture Heine on the other hand describes self enhancement as a predominantly Western motive 20 167 168 169 170 171 172 Other motives Edit It is an exaggeration to say that self enhancement is the dominant self evaluation motive Many controversies exist regarding the distinction between the self evaluation motives and there are situations in which motives asides from self enhancement assume priority The self assessment motive is often contrasted with the self enhancement motive due to the relative adaptiveness of each approach within social interactions The self verification motive is often challenged by supporters of the self enhancement as being unfeasible as it often appears implausible The self improvement motive is often taken to be the physical manifestation of the self enhancement motive i e the act of attaining desired positive self views Where the truth about oneself worsens or varies it gradually becomes less feasible to satisfy all motives simultaneously In an attempt to compare the self evaluation motives excluding self improvement a self reflection task was employed Participants were asked to choose the question they would most likely ask themselves in order to determine whether they possessed a certain personality trait On the whole people self enhanced more than they self assessed or self verified People chose higher diagnosticity questions concerning central positive traits than central negative ones and answered yes more often to central positive than negative questions Also people self verified more than the self assessed and chose more questions overall concerning relatively certain central traits than relatively uncertain peripheral traits 173 Other factors Edit Cognitive load Where people are in situations of great cognitive load the tendency to self enhance increases almost as if instinctive People are quicker to agree with possessing positive traits and slower to reject having negative traits 174 175 Modifiability Where a trait or characteristic is seen as unchangeable people are more self enhancing versus perceiving the trait to be modifiable 176 Diagnosticity Where a trait or characteristic is seen as highly diagnostic people are less likely to self enhance for fear of being caught out in the process of an erroneous attempt at self enhancement as neurosis 135 136 See also EditIdentity social science Individualistic culture Basking in reflected glory Memory Raison oblige theory Self control Self categorization theory Self determination theory Self evaluation maintenance theory Self knowledge psychology References EditNotes Edit Sedikides C Strube M J 1995 The Multiply Motivated Self Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 12 1330 1335 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 561 6126 doi 10 1177 01461672952112010 ISSN 0146 1672 S2CID 34670867 The self enhancement motive refers to people s desire to enhance the positivity or decrease the negativity of the self concept Beauregard Keith S Dunning David 1998 Turning up the contrast Self enhancement motives prompt egocentric contrast effects in social judgments Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 3 606 621 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 3 606 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 9523408 Krueger J 1998 Enhancement Bias in Descriptions of Self and Others Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24 5 505 516 doi 10 1177 0146167298245006 ISSN 0146 1672 S2CID 144483633 Wills Thomas A 1981 Downward comparison principles in social psychology Psychological Bulletin 90 2 245 271 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 90 2 245 ISSN 0033 2909 a b c Sedikides Constantine Gregg Aiden P 2008 Self Enhancement Food for Thought PDF Perspectives on Psychological Science 3 2 102 116 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6916 2008 00068 x ISSN 1745 6916 PMID 26158877 S2CID 5171543 a b Kunda 1999 pp 485 486 Kunda 1999 pp 465 466 Arkin R M 1981 Self presentation styles In J Tedeschi Ed Impression management theory and research pp 311 333 New York Academic Press Baumeister R F Bratslavsky Ellen Finkenauer Catrin amp Vohs Kathleen D 2001 Bad is stronger than good PDF Review of General Psychology 5 4 323 370 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 5 4 323 S2CID 13154992 a b Tice Dianne M 1991 Esteem protection or enhancement Self handicapping motives and attributions differ by trait self esteem Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 5 711 725 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 60 5 711 ISSN 0022 3514 Josephs R A Larrick RP Steele CM Nisbett RE 1992 Protecting the self from the negative consequences of risky decisions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62 1 26 37 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 62 1 26 PMID 1538314 Brown J D amp Gallagher Frances M 1992 Coming to terms with failure Private self enhancement and public self effacement Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 28 3 22 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 92 90029 J Leary M R amp Kowalski Robin M 1990 Impression management A literature review and two component model Psychological Bulletin 107 34 47 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 463 776 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 107 1 34 S2CID 15886705 Greenwald A G amp Breckler S J 1985 To whom is the self presented in B E Schlenker ed The self and social life New York McGraw Hill pp 126 145 Leary M R Tchividjian LR amp Kraxberger BE 1994 Self presentation can be hazardous to your health Impression management and health risk Health Psychology 13 6 461 470 doi 10 1037 0278 6133 13 6 461 PMID 7889900 Sedikides C Campbell W K Reeder G amp Elliot A J 1998 The self serving bias in relational context Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 2 378 79 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 2 378 S2CID 67763602 Klein W M 1997 Objective standards are not enough Affective self evaluative and behavioural responses to social comparison information Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 4 763 774 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 4 763 PMID 9108694 Crocker J amp Wolfe C T 2001 Contingencies of self worth Psychological Review 108 3 592 623 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 108 3 593 PMID 11488379 Crocker J 2002 Contingencies of self worth Implications for self regulation and psychological vulnerability Self and Identity 1 2 143 149 doi 10 1080 152988602317319320 S2CID 142982721 a b c Sedikides Constantine Gaertner Lowell Toguchi Yoshiyasu 2003 Pancultural self enhancement Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 1 60 79 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 84 1 60 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 12518971 a b Sedikides C Strube M 1997 Self Evaluation To Thine Own Self Be Good To Thine Own Self Be Sure To Thine Own Self Be True and To Thine Own Self be Better Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Volume 29 Advances in Experimental Social Psychology vol 29 pp 209 269 doi 10 1016 S0065 2601 08 60018 0 ISBN 9780120152292 ISSN 0065 2601 In M P Zanna Ed Advances in experimental social psychology Vol 29 pp 209 269 New York Academic Press Sedikides C Gregg A P amp Hart C M 2007 The importance of being modest in C Sedikides amp S Spencer eds The self Frontiers in social psychology New York Psychology Press pp 163 184 Vazire S amp Funder D C Funder DC 2006 Impulsivity and the self defeating behaviour of narcissists Personality and Social Psychology Review 10 2 154 165 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 419 6963 doi 10 1207 s15327957pspr1002 4 PMID 16768652 S2CID 1924100 Mezulis Amy H Abramson Lyn Y Hyde Janet S Hankin Benjamin L 2004 Is There a Universal Positivity Bias in Attributions A Meta Analytic Review of Individual Developmental and Cultural Differences in the Self Serving Attributional Bias Psychological Bulletin 130 5 711 747 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 130 5 711 ISSN 0033 2909 PMID 15367078 Schlenker Barry R Miller Rowland S 1977 Egocentrism in groups Self serving biases or logical information processing Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35 10 755 764 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 35 10 755 ISSN 0022 3514 Greenberg J Pyszczynski Tom Solomon Sheldon 1982 The self serving attributional bias Beyond self presentation Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18 1 56 67 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 82 90081 6 ISSN 0022 1031 Riess Marc Rosenfeld Paul Melburg Valerie Tedeschi James T 1981 Self serving attributions Biased private perceptions and distorted public descriptions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41 2 224 231 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 41 2 224 ISSN 0022 3514 Campbell W K amp Sedikides Constantine 1999 Self threat magnifies the self serving bias A meta analytic integration Review of General Psychology 3 23 43 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 3 1 23 S2CID 144756539 Zuckerman M 1979 Attribution of success and failure revisited of The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory Journal of Personality 47 2 245 287 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1979 tb00202 x Baumeister R F Stillwell A amp Wotman SR 1990 Victim and perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict Autobiographical narratives about anger Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 5 994 1005 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 59 5 994 PMID 2266485 S2CID 28145593 Gonzales M H Pederson J H Manning D J amp Wetter D W Pederson Julie Haugen Manning Debra J Wetter David W 1990 Pardon my gaffe Effects of sex status and consequence severity on accounts Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 4 610 621 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 4 610 a b Pettigrew T F 2001 The ultimate attribution error Extending Allport s cognitive analysis of prejudice in M A Hogg amp D Abrams eds Intergroup relations Essential readings Philadelphia PA Psychology Press Taylor amp Francis pp 162 173 Ciladini R B Richard j Borden Avril Thorne Marcus Randall Walker Stephen Freeman amp Lloyd Reynolds Sloan 1976 Basking in reflected glory Three football field studies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34 3 366 375 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 34 3 366 Gramzow R H Gaertner L amp Sedikides C 2001 Memory for ingroup and outgroup information in a minimal group context The self as an informational base Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 2 188 205 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 80 2 188 PMID 11220440 Baumeister R F amp Cairns KJ 1982 Repression and self presentation When audiences interfere with self deceptive strategies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62 5 851 862 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 62 5 851 PMID 1593424 a b Sedikides C amp Green JD 2000 On the self protective nature of inconsistency negativity management Using the person memory paradigm to examine self referent memory Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 6 906 922 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 79 6 906 PMID 11138760 Festinger L 1957 A theory of cognitive dissonance Stanford CA Stanford University Press Frey D 1986 Recent research on selective exposure to information In L Bercowitz Ed Advances in experimental social psychology pp 41 80 New York Academic Press a b Sanitioso R Kunda Z amp Fong GT 1990 Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 2 229 241 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 59 2 229 PMID 2213492 Murray S L amp Holmes John G 1993 Seeing virtues in faults Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives in close relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 4 707 722 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 65 4 707 Walker W R Skowronski John J amp Thompson Charles P 2003 Life is pleasant and memory helps to keep it that way Review of General Psychology 7 2 203 210 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 7 2 203 S2CID 43179740 Mischel W Ebbesen EB amp Zeis AM 1976 Determinants of selective memory about the self Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 44 1 92 103 doi 10 1037 0022 006X 44 1 92 PMID 1245637 van Lange P Rusbult Caryl E Semin Goossens Astrid Gorts Carien A amp Stalpers Mirjam 1999 Being better than others but otherwise perfectly normal Perceptions of uniqueness and similarity in close relationships Personal Relationships 6 3 269 289 doi 10 1111 j 1475 6811 1999 tb00192 x Gosling Samuel D John Oliver P Craik Kenneth H Robins Richard W 1998 Do people know how they behave Self reported act frequencies compared with on line codings by observers Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 5 1337 1349 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 5 1337 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 9599447 Skowronski J J Betz Andrew L Thompson Charles P amp Shannon Laura 1991 Social memory in everyday life Recall of self events and other events Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 6 831 843 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 60 6 831 Arkin R M amp Maruyama Geoffrey M 1979 Attribution affect and college exam performance Journal of Educational Psychology 71 85 93 doi 10 1037 0022 0663 71 1 85 Greenwald A G 2002 Constructs in student ratings of instructors in H I Braun amp D N Douglas eds The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement Mahwah NJ Lawrence Erlbaum pp 277 297 a b Ditto P H amp Boardman A F 1995 Perceived accuracy of favourable and unfavourable psychological feedback Basic and Applied Social Psychology 16 1 2 137 157 doi 10 1080 01973533 1995 9646106 Pyszczynski T amp Greenberg Jeff 1987 toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference A biased hypothesis testing model Advances in Experimental Social Psychology vol 20 pp 297 341 doi 10 1016 S0065 2601 08 60417 7 ISBN 9780120152209 a b Ditto P H amp Lopez David F 1992 Motivated skepticism Use of differential decision criteria for preferred and non preferred conclusions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63 4 568 584 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 63 4 568 Ditto P H Scepansky James A Munro Geoffrey D Apanovitch Anne Marie amp Lockhart Lisa K 1998 Motivated sensitivity to preference inconsistent information Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 53 69 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 75 1 53 Buunk B P Collins RL Taylor SE Vanyperen NW amp Dakof GA 1990 The affective consequences of social comparisons Either directions has its ups and downs Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 6 1238 1249 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 59 6 1238 PMID 2283590 Collins R L 1996 For better for worse The impact of upwards social comparison on self evaluation Psychological Bulletin 119 51 69 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 119 1 51 Wheeler L 1966 Motivation as a determinant of upwards social comparison Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 2 27 31 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 66 90062 X Lockwood P amp Kunda Ziva 1997 Superstars and me Predicting the impact of role models on self Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 91 103 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 578 7014 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 73 1 91 Wood J V 1989 Theory and research concerning social comparison of personality attributes Psychological Bulletin 106 2 231 248 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 456 7776 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 106 2 231 Collins R L 2000 Among the better ones Upward assimilation in social comparison in J Suls amp L Wheeler eds Handbook of social comparison New York Kluwer Academic Plenum pp 159 172 Gruder C L 1971 Determinants of social comparison choices Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 1 5 473 489 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 71 90010 2 Miller D T Turnbull William amp McFarland Cathy 1988 Particularistic and universalistic evaluation in the social comparison process Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55 6 908 917 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 55 6 908 Biernat M amp Billings L S 2001 Standards expectancies and social comparisons in A Tesser amp N Schwartz eds Blackwell handbook of social psychology Intraindividual processes Oxford Blackwell Publishing pp 257 283 Suls J Wills T A eds 1991 Social comparison Contemporary theory and research Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum Crocker J Voelkl K Testa M amp Major B 1991 Social stigma and self esteem The self protective properties of stigma Psychological Review 96 4 803 808 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 96 4 608 Tesser A 1988 Towards a self evaluation maintenance model of social behaviour In L Berkowitz Ed Advances in experimental social psychology vol 21 pp 181 227 New York Academic Press Beach S R H amp Tesser Abraham 1993 Decision making power and marital satisfaction A self evaluation maintenance perspective Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 12 4 471 494 doi 10 1521 jscp 1993 12 4 471 Pemberton M amp Sedikides C 2001 When do individuals help close others improve Extending the self evaluation maintenance model to future comparisons Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 2 234 246 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 2 234 PMID 11519929 Tesser A amp Paulhus Del 1983 The definition of self Private and public self evaluation maintenance strategies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44 4 672 682 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 44 4 672 Tesser A amp Smith Jonathan 1980 Some effects of task relevance and friendship on helping You don t always help the one you like Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 16 6 582 590 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 80 90060 8 Cantor N amp Mischel Walter 1979 Prototypicality and personality Effects on free recall and personality impressions Journal of Research in Personality 13 2 187 205 doi 10 1016 0092 6566 79 90030 8 Dunning D Leuenberger Ann amp Sherman David A 1995 A new look at motivated inference Are self serving theories of success a product of motivational forces Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 58 68 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 69 1 58 S2CID 53358618 Dunning D Perie M amp Story AL 1991 Self serving prototypes of social categories Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 6 957 968 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 61 6 957 PMID 1774633 Kunda Z 1987 Motivated interference Self serving generation and evaluation of causal theories Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 4 636 647 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 53 4 636 Dunning D amp Cohen Geoffrey L 1992 Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities in social judgment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63 3 341 355 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 63 3 341 Alicke M D Loschiavo FM Zerbst J amp Zhang S 1997 The person who outperforms me is a genius Maintaining perceived competence in upward social comparison Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 4 781 789 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 73 4 781 PMID 9325593 S2CID 1291407 Ditto P H Munro GD Apanovitch AM Scepansky JA amp Lockhart LK 2003 Spontaneous skepticism The interplay or motivation and expectation in responses to favourable and unfavourable medical diagnoses Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29 9 1120 1132 doi 10 1177 0146167203254536 PMID 15189608 S2CID 6555808 a b Doosje B Spears Russell amp Koomen Willem 1995 When bad isn t all bad The strategic use of sample information in generalization and stereotyping Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 4 642 655 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 69 4 642 Dunning D amp Beauregard Keith S 2000 Regulating impressions of others to affirm images of the self Social Cognition 18 2 198 222 doi 10 1521 soco 2000 18 2 198 Crocker J Major B amp Steele C 1998 Social stigma in D T Gilbert S T Fiske amp G Lindzey eds The handbook of social psychology 4th ed New York McGraw Hill pp 504 553 Ross L Greene David amp House Pamela 1977 The false consensus effect An attributional bias in self perception and social perception processes Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 13 3 279 301 doi 10 1016 0022 1031 77 90049 X Mullen B amp Goethals George R 1990 Social projection actual consensus and valence British Journal of Social Psychology 29 3 279 282 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8309 1990 tb00907 x a b Jones E E amp Berglas S 1978 Control of attributions about the self through self handicapping strategies The appeal of alcohol and underachievement Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 4 2 200 206 doi 10 1177 014616727800400205 S2CID 146420423 Feick D L amp Rhodewalt Frederick 1997 The double edged sword of self handicapping Discounting augmentation and the protection and enhancement of self esteem Motivation and Emotion 21 2 147 163 doi 10 1023 A 1024434600296 S2CID 141218684 a b Rhodewalt F Morf Carolyn Hazlett Susan amp Fairfield Marita 1991 Self handicapping The role of discounting and augmentation in the preservation of self esteem Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 122 131 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 61 1 122 Rhodewalt F amp Fairfield Marita 1991 Claimed self handicaps and the self handicapper The relation of reduction in intended effort to performance Journal of Research in Personality 25 4 402 417 doi 10 1016 0092 6566 91 90030 T Tice D M amp Baumeister Roy F 1990 Self esteem self handicapping and self presentation The strategy of inadequate practice Journal of Personality 58 2 443 464 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1990 tb00237 x Rhodewalt F Sanbonmatsu D M Tschanz B Feick D L amp Waller A 1995 Self handicapping and interpersonal trade offs The effects of claimed self handicaps on observers performance evaluations and feedback Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 10 1042 1050 doi 10 1177 01461672952110005 S2CID 143465519 a b Arkin R M amp Oleson K C 1998 Self handicapping in J Darley amp J Cooper eds Attributional and social interaction The legacy of Edward E Jones Washington DC American Psychological Association pp 313 348 Dweck C S 1999 Self theories Their role in motivation personality and development Philadelphia PA Psychology Press Rhodewalt F amp Tragakis M 2002 Self handicapping and the social self The costs and rewards of interpersonal self construction in J Forgas amp K Williams eds The social self Cognitive interpersonal and intergroup perspectives Philadelphia PA Psychology Press pp 121 143 a b Ross S R Canada Kelli E amp Rausch Marcus K 2002 Self handicapping and the Five Factor model of personality Mediation between neuroticism and conscientiousness Personality and Individual Differences 32 7 1173 1184 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 01 00079 4 Giannetti E 2001 Lies we live by The art of self deception London Bloomsbury McCria S M amp Hirt E R 2001 The role of ability judgments in self handicapping Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 10 1378 1389 doi 10 1177 01461672012710013 S2CID 2174958 Zuckerman M amp Tsai FF 2005 Costs of self handicapping Journal of Personality 73 2 411 442 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2005 00314 x PMID 15745436 Zuckerman M Kieffer SC amp Knee CR 1998 Consequences of self handicapping Effects on coping academic performance and adjustment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 6 1619 1628 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 6 1619 PMID 9654762 Arndt J Schimel J Greenberg J amp Pyszczynski T 2002 The intrinsic self and defensiveness Evidence that activating the intrinsic self reduces self handicapping and conformity Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 5 671 683 doi 10 1177 0146167202288011 S2CID 145296762 Taylor S E amp Brown JD 1988 Illusions and well being A social psychological perspective on mental health Psychological Bulletin 103 2 193 210 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 103 2 193 PMID 3283814 S2CID 762759 a b Alicke M D Vredenburg Debbie S Hiatt Matthew amp Govorun Olesya 2001 The better than myself effect Motivation and Emotion 25 7 22 doi 10 1023 A 1010655705069 S2CID 140785247 Fenton O Creevy M Nicholson Nigel Soane Emma amp Willman Paul 2003 Trading on illusion Unrealistic perceptions of control and trading performance Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology 76 53 68 doi 10 1348 096317903321208880 a b Helweg Larsen M amp Shepperd J A 2001 Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates A review of the literature Personality and Social Psychology Review 51 74 95 doi 10 1207 S15327957PSPR0501 5 S2CID 30461688 Pronin E Gilovich T amp Ross L 2004 Objectivity in the eye of the beholder Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others Psychological Review 111 3 781 799 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 111 3 781 PMID 15250784 Murray S L 1999 The quest for conviction Motivated cognition in romantic relationships Psychological Inquiry 10 23 34 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli1001 3 Rusbult C E Van Lange PA Wildschut T Yovetich NA amp Verette J 2000 Perceived superiority in close relationships Why it exists and persists Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 4 521 545 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 79 4 521 hdl 1871 17595 PMID 11045737 Nesselroade K P Beggan J K Beggan James K amp Allison Scott T 1999 Possession enhancement in an interpersonal context An extension of the mere ownership effect Psychology and Marketing 16 21 34 doi 10 1002 SICI 1520 6793 199901 16 1 lt 21 AID MAR2 gt 3 0 CO 2 9 a b Brown J 1998 The self New York McGraw Hill Alicke M D 1985 Global self evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49 6 1621 1630 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 49 6 1621 College Board 1976 1977 Student descriptive questionnaire Princeton NJ Educational Testing Service Dunning D Meyerowitz Judith A amp Holzberg Amy D 1989 Ambiguity and self evaluation The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self serving assessments of ability Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 6 1082 1090 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 57 6 1082 Kruger J amp Dunning D 1999 Unskilled and unaware of it How difficulties in recognising one s own incompetence lead to inflated self assessments Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77 6 1121 1134 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 77 6 1121 PMID 10626367 Cross P 1977 Not can but will college teaching be improved New Directions for Higher Education 1977 17 1 15 doi 10 1002 he 36919771703 Svenson O 1981 Are we less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers Acta Psychologica 47 2 143 151 doi 10 1016 0001 6918 81 90005 6 Preston C E amp Harris S 1965 Psychology of drivers in traffic accidents Journal of Applied Psychology 49 4 284 288 doi 10 1037 h0022453 PMID 5826671 Pronin E Yin D Y amp Ross L 2002 The bias blind spot Perceptions of bias in self versus others Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 3 369 381 doi 10 1177 0146167202286008 S2CID 14259317 Langer E J 1975 The illusion of control Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32 2 311 328 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 32 2 311 S2CID 30043741 a b Jenkins H M amp Ward WC 1965 Judgments of contingency between response and outcome Psychological Monographs 79 1 17 doi 10 1037 h0093874 PMID 14300511 Weinstein N D 1980 Unrealistic optimism about future events Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 5 806 829 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 39 5 806 Weinstein N D amp Klein WM 1995 Resistance of personal risk perceptions to debiasing manipulation Health Psychology 14 2 132 140 doi 10 1037 0278 6133 14 2 132 PMID 7789348 S2CID 25474023 Regan P C Snyder M amp Kassin S M 1995 Unrealistic optimism Self enhancement or person positivity Personality and Social Psychology Review 21 10 1073 1082 doi 10 1177 01461672952110008 S2CID 144119515 Vallone R P Griffin DW Lin S amp Ross L 1990 Overconfident predictions of future actions and outcomes by self and others Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 4 582 592 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 4 582 PMID 2348360 Buehler R Griffin Dale amp Ross Michael 1994 Exploring the planning fallacy Why people underestimate their task completion times Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 3 366 381 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 67 3 366 S2CID 4222578 Dunning D Griffin DW Milojkovic JD amp Ross L 1990 The overconfidence effect in social predictions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 4 568 581 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 4 568 PMID 2348359 Smits T amp Hoorens V Hoorens Vera 2005 How probable is probably It depends on whom you re talking about Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18 2 83 96 doi 10 1002 bdm 485 S2CID 144967476 Weinstein N D Marcus SE amp Moser RP 2005 Smokers unrealistic optimism about their risk Tobacco Control 14 1 55 59 doi 10 1136 tc 2004 008375 PMC 1747991 PMID 15735301 Sedikides C Gregg A P amp Hart C M 2007 The self Frontiers in social psychology New York Psychology Press pp 163 184 Chang E C Ed 2007 Self criticism and self enhancement Theory research and clinical implications Washington DC American Psychological Association Gramzow R H Elliot Andrew J Asher Evan amp McGregor Holly A 2003 Self evaluation bias and academic performance Some ways and some reasons why Journal of Research in Personality 37 2 41 61 doi 10 1016 S0092 6566 02 00535 4 Young Hoon Kim Chiu Chiu Zou Zhimin 2010 Know Thyself Misperceptions of Actual Performance Undermine Achievement Motivation Future Performance and Subjective Well Being Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99 3 395 409 doi 10 1037 a0020555 PMID 20804261 Swann W B Jr Chang Schneider C amp Larsen Mcclarty K 2007 Do our self views matter Self concept and self esteem in everyday life American Psychologist 62 2 84 94 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 62 2 84 PMID 17324034 S2CID 13589535 Trzesniewski K Donnellan MB Moffitt TE Robins RW Poulton R amp Caspi A 2006 Low self esteem during adolescence predicts poor health criminal behaviour and limited economic prospects during adulthood Developmental Psychology 42 2 381 390 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 42 2 381 PMID 16569175 a b Colvin C R Block Jack amp Funder David C 1995 Overly positive self evaluations and personality Negative implications for mental health Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 6 1152 1162 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 68 6 1152 PMID 7608859 Paulhus D L 1998 Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self enhancement A mixed blessing Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 5 1197 1208 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 5 1197 PMID 9599439 Paulhus Delroy L Aliye Kurt August 2008 Moderators of the adaptiveness of self enhancement Operationalization motivational domain adjustment facet and evaluator Journal of Research in Personality 42 4 839 853 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2007 11 005 Taylor S E Lerner JS Sherman DK Sage RM amp McDowell NK 2003 Portrait of the self enhancer Well adjusted and well liked or maladjusted and friendless Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 1 165 176 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 84 1 165 PMID 12518977 Bonanno G A Rennicke C amp Dekel S 2005 Self enhancement among high exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attack Resilience or social maladjustment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88 6 984 998 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 88 6 984 PMID 15982117 van Lange P A M amp Sedikides Constantine 1998 Being more honest but not necessarily more intelligent than others Generality and explanations for the Muhammad Ali effect PDF European Journal of Social Psychology 28 4 675 680 doi 10 1002 SICI 1099 0992 199807 08 28 4 lt 675 AID EJSP883 gt 3 0 CO 2 5 Morf C C Carolyn C amp Rhodewalt Frederick 2001 Unraveling the Paradoxes of Narcissism A Dynamic Self Regulatory Processing Model Psychological Inquiry 12 4 177 196 doi 10 1207 S15327965PLI1204 1 S2CID 2004430 a b c Sedikides C Herbst KC Hardin DP amp Dardis GJ 2002 Accountability as a deterrent to self enhancement The search for mechanisms Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83 3 592 605 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 83 3 592 PMID 12219856 a b c Tice D M Butler J L Muraven M B amp Stillwell A M 1995 When modesty prevails Differential favourability of self presentation to friends and strangers Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 6 443 464 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 69 6 1120 Gilbert D T Pinel E C Wilson T D Blumberg S J amp Wheatley T P 1998 Immune neglect A source of durability bias in affective forecasting Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 3 617 638 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 75 3 617 PMID 9781405 Trope Y amp Neter E 1994 Reconciling competing motives in self evaluation The role of self control in feedback seeking Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66 4 646 657 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 66 4 646 PMID 8189345 Blaine B amp Crocker J 1993 Self esteem and self serving biases in reactions to positive and negative events An integrative review in R F Baumeister ed Self esteem The puzzle of low self regard New York Plenum Press pp 55 85 Kuiper Nicholas A 1978 Depression and causal attributions for success and failure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 3 236 246 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 36 3 236 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 650382 a b Campbell Jennifer D Fehr Beverley 1990 Self esteem and perceptions of conveyed impressions Is negative affectivity associated with greater realism Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 1 122 133 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 1 122 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 2308069 Lewinsohn Peter M Mischel Walter Chaplin William Barton Russell 1980 Social competence and depression The role of illusory self perceptions Journal of Abnormal Psychology 89 2 203 212 doi 10 1037 0021 843X 89 2 203 ISSN 0021 843X PMID 7365132 Alloy L B 1988 Depressive realism Four theoretical perspectives In L B Alloy Ed Cognitive processes in depression pp 223 265 New York Guilford Press Fincham F D amp Bradbury T N 1989 The impact of attributions in marriage An individual difference analysis Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 6 69 85 doi 10 1177 026540758900600105 S2CID 145745264 Clark M S amp Mills Judson 1979 Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 12 24 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 37 1 12 Murray S L Holmes J G Griffin D W 1996a 1996 The benefits of positive illusions Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 79 98 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 70 1 79 Fiske A P Kitayama S Markus H R amp Nisbett R E 1998 The cultural matrix of social psychology in D T Gilbert S T Fiske amp G Lindzey eds The handbook of social psychology 4th ed Boston MA McGraw Hill pp 915 981 Markus H R amp Kitayama Shinobu 1991 Culture and the self Implications for cognition emotion and motivation Psychological Review 98 2 224 253 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 98 2 224 Triandis H C amp Suh EM 2002 Cultural influences on personality Annual Review of Psychology 53 133 160 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135200 PMID 11752482 Kitayama S Markus HR Matsumoto H Norasakkunkit V 1997 Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self Self enhancement in the United States and self criticism in Japan Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 6 1245 1267 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 72 6 1245 PMID 9177018 a b Kitayama S Markus H R amp Lieberman C 1995a The collective construction of self esteem Implications for culture self and emotion in R Russell J Fernandez Dols T Manstead amp J Wellenkamp eds Everyday conception of emotion An introduction to the psychology anthropology and linguistics of emotion Dordrecht The Netherlands Kluwer Academic pp 523 550 Kanagawa C Cross S E amp Markus H R 2001 Who am I The cultural psychology of the conceptual self Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 90 103 doi 10 1177 0146167201271008 S2CID 145634514 Takata T 1987 Self depreciative tendencies in self evaluation through social comparison Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 27 27 36 doi 10 2130 jjesp 27 27 Heine S J amp Lehman Darrin R 1995 Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism Does the west feel more invulnerable than the east Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 4 595 607 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 68 4 595 Kitayama S Takagi H amp Matsumoto H 1995b Seiko to shippai no kiin nihonteki jiko no bunkashinrigaku Causal attributions of success and failure Cultural psychology of Japanese selves Japanese Psychological Review 38 247 280 Heine S H Lehman DR Markus HR amp Kitayama S 1999 Is there a universal need for positive self regard Psychological Review 106 4 766 794 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 106 4 766 PMID 10560328 Heine S J Takata T amp Lehman D R 2000 Beyond self presentation Evidence for self criticism among Japanese Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26 71 78 doi 10 1177 0146167200261007 S2CID 40878324 Heine S J Kitayama S amp Lehman D R 2001a 2001 Cultural Differences in Self Evaluation Japanese Readily Accept Negative Self Relevant Information Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 32 4 434 443 doi 10 1177 0022022101032004004 S2CID 40475406 a b Heine S J Kitayama S Lehman D R Takata T Ide E Leung C amp Matsumoto H 2001b 2001 Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America An investigation of self improving motivations and malleable selves Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 4 599 615 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 4 599 PMID 11642348 Yik M S M Bond M H amp Paulhus D L 1998 Do Chinese self enhance or self efface It s a matter of domain Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24 4 399 406 doi 10 1177 0146167298244006 S2CID 145725445 Kurman J 2001 Self enhancement Is it restricted to individualistic cultures Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 12 1705 1716 doi 10 1177 01461672012712013 S2CID 145763977 Sedikides C Campbell W Keith Reeder Glenn D amp Elliot Andrew J 2002 W Strobe amp M Hewstone eds The self in relationships Whether how and when close others put the self in its place PDF European Review of Social Psychology 12 237 265 doi 10 1080 14792772143000076 S2CID 146180762 Markus H R amp Nurius Paula 1986 Possible selves American Psychologist 41 9 954 969 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 41 9 954 Sedikides C 1999 A multiplicity of motives The case of self improvement Psychological Inquiry 9 64 65 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli1001 10 Falbo T Poston D L Triscari R S amp Zhang X 1997 Self enhancing illusions among Chinese schoolchildren Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 28 2 172 191 doi 10 1177 0022022197282003 S2CID 145500149 Fahr J Dobbins Gregory H amp Cheng BOR Shiuan 1991 Cultural relativity in action A comparison of self ratings made by Chinese and U S workers Personnel Psychology 44 129 147 doi 10 1111 j 1744 6570 1991 tb00693 x S2CID 144606718 Gaertner L Sedikides C amp Chang K 2008 On pancultural self enhancement Well adjusted Taiwanese self enhance on personally valued traits PDF Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 39 4 463 477 doi 10 1177 0022022108318431 S2CID 30238954 Heine S J Kitayama Shinobu amp Hamamura Takeshi 2007 Inclusion of additional studies yields different conclusions Comment on Sedikides Gaertner amp Vevea 2005 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Asian Journal of Social Psychology 10 2 49 58 doi 10 1111 j 1467 839X 2007 00211 x hdl 2027 42 75100 Sedikides C Gaertner L amp Vevea J L Gaertner Lowell Vevea Jack L 2007 Inclusion of theory relevant moderators yield the same conclusions as Sedikides Gaertner and Vevea 2005 A meta analytical reply to Heine Kitayama and Hamamura 2007 PDF Asian Journal of Social Psychology 10 2 59 67 doi 10 1111 j 1467 839X 2007 00212 x Heine S J Kitayama S amp Hamamura T 2007b 2007 Which studies test whether self enhancement is pancultural Reply to Sedikides Gaertner and Vevea 2007 Asian Journal of Social Psychology 10 3 198 200 doi 10 1111 j 1467 839X 2007 00226 x hdl 2027 42 75225 Sedikides C Gaertner L amp Vevea J L Gaertner L Vevea JL 2005 Pancultural self enhancement reloaded A meta analytic reply to Heine 2005 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89 4 539 551 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 550 7957 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 89 4 539 PMID 16287417 Heine S J 2005 Where is the evidence for pancultural self enhancement A reply to Sedikides Gaertner amp Toguchi 2003 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89 4 531 538 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 89 4 531 PMID 16287416 Sedikides Constantine 1993 Assessment enhancement and verification determinants of the self evaluation process Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 2 317 338 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 65 2 317 ISSN 1939 1315 Paulhus D L Graf Peter amp Van Selst Mark 1989 Attentional load increases the positivity of self presentation Social Cognition 7 4 389 400 doi 10 1521 soco 1989 7 4 389 Paulhus D L amp Levitt Karen 1987 Desirable responding triggered by affect Automatic egotism Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 2 245 259 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 52 2 245 Roese N J amp Olson JM 2007 Better stronger faster Self serving judgment affect regulation and the optimal vigilance hypothesis Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 2 124 141 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6916 2007 00033 x PMC 2429993 PMID 18552989 Sources Edit Baumeister Roy F 1999 The Self in Social Psychology Psychology Press ISBN 978 0 86377 572 7 Hogg Michael A Vaughan Graham M 2008 Social Psychology Pearson Education ISBN 978 0 13 206931 1 Kunda Ziva 1999 Social Cognition Making Sense of People MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 61143 5 OCLC 40618974Further reading EditEdward Chin Ho Chang Self Criticism and Self Enhancement American Psychological Association 2008 ISBN 978 1 4338 0115 0 Hogg Michael A Cooper Joel 2003 The Sage Handbook of Social Psychology Sage ISBN 978 0 7619 6636 4 Mark R Leary amp June Price Tangney Handbook of Self and Identity Guilford Press 2005 ISBN 978 1 59385 237 5External links EditSedikides vs Heine debate videos Constantine Sedikides Home Page Home Page of Steven Heine University of Southampton s Centre for Research on Self and Identity Home Page International Society for Self and Identity Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Self enhancement amp oldid 1134980291, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.