fbpx
Wikipedia

Rosenhan experiment

The Rosenhan experiment or Thud experiment was an experiment conducted to determine the validity of psychiatric diagnosis. Participants submitted themselves for evaluation at various psychiatric institutions and feigned hallucinations in order to be accepted, but acted normally from then onward. Each was diagnosed with psychiatric disorders and were given antipsychotic medication. The study was conducted by psychologist David Rosenhan, a Stanford University professor, and published by the journal Science in 1973 under the title "On Being Sane in Insane Places".[1][2]

The main building of St. Elizabeths Hospital (1996), located in Washington, D.C., now part of the headquarters of the U.S. Department of Homeland Security, was one of the sites of the Rosenhan experiment

It is considered[by whom?] an important and influential criticism of psychiatric diagnosis, and broached the topic of wrongful involuntary commitment.[3] The experiment is said to have "accelerated the movement to reform mental institutions and to deinstitutionalize as many mental patients as possible".[4] Rosenhan along with eight other people (five men and three women) entered 12 hospitals across five states along the west coast of the US. Three of the participants were admitted for only a short period of time, and in order to obtain sufficient documented experiences, they re-applied to additional institutions.

Respondents defended psychiatry against the experiment's conclusions, saying that as psychiatric diagnosis relies largely on the patient's report of their experiences, faking their presence no more demonstrates problems with psychiatric diagnosis than lying about other medical symptoms.[5] More recently, it has been alleged that at least part of the published results were distorted or falsified.[6]

Pseudopatient experiment edit

Rosenhan described his study as having two parts. The first part involved the use of healthy associates or "pseudopatients" (three women and six men, including Rosenhan himself) who briefly feigned auditory hallucinations in an attempt to gain admission to 12 psychiatric hospitals in five states in the United States. All were admitted and diagnosed with psychiatric disorders. After admission, the pseudopatients acted normally and told staff that they no longer experienced any additional hallucinations. As a condition of their release, all the patients were forced to admit to having a mental illness and had to agree to take antipsychotic medication. The average time that the patients spent in the hospital was 19 days. All but one were diagnosed with schizophrenia "in remission" before their release.

The second part of his study involved a hospital administration challenging Rosenhan to send pseudopatients to its facility, whose staff asserted that they would be able to detect the pseudopatients. Rosenhan agreed, and in the following weeks 41 out of 193 new patients were identified as potential pseudopatients, with 19 of these receiving suspicion from at least one psychiatrist and one other staff member. Rosenhan then revealed that he sent no pseudopatients to the hospital.

While listening to a lecture by R. D. Laing, who was associated with the anti-psychiatry movement, Rosenhan conceived of the experiment as a way to test the reliability of psychiatric diagnoses.[7] The study concluded "it is clear that we cannot distinguish the sane from the insane in psychiatric hospitals" and also illustrated the dangers of dehumanization and labeling in psychiatric institutions. It suggested that the use of community mental health facilities which concentrated on specific problems and behaviors rather than psychiatric labels might be a solution, and recommended education to make psychiatric workers more aware of the social psychology of their facilities.

Rosenhan himself and seven mentally healthy associates, called "pseudopatients", attempted to gain admission to psychiatric hospitals by calling for an appointment and feigning auditory hallucinations. The hospital staff were not informed of the experiment. The pseudopatients included a psychology graduate student in his twenties, three psychologists, a pediatrician, a psychiatrist, a painter, and a housewife. None had a history of mental illness. Pseudopatients used pseudonyms, and those who worked in the mental health field were given false jobs in a different sector to avoid invoking any special treatment or scrutiny. Apart from giving false names and employment details, further biographical details were truthfully reported.

During their initial psychiatric assessment, the pseudopatients claimed to be hearing voices of the same sex as the patient which were often unclear, but which seemed to pronounce the words "empty", "hollow", or "thud", and nothing else. These words were chosen as they vaguely suggest some sort of existential crisis and for the lack of any published literature referencing them as psychotic symptoms. No other psychiatric symptoms were claimed. If admitted, the pseudopatients were instructed to "act normally", reporting that they felt fine and no longer heard voices. Hospital records obtained after the experiment indicate that all pseudopatients were characterized as friendly and cooperative by staff.

All were admitted, to 12 psychiatric hospitals across the United States, including rundown and underfunded public hospitals in rural areas, urban university-run hospitals with excellent reputations, and one expensive private hospital. Though presented with identical symptoms, seven were diagnosed with schizophrenia at public hospitals, and one with manic-depressive psychosis, a more optimistic diagnosis with better clinical outcomes, at the private hospital. Their stays ranged from 7 to 52 days, and the average was 19 days. All but one were discharged with a diagnosis of schizophrenia "in remission", which Rosenhan considered as evidence that mental illness is perceived as an irreversible condition creating a lifelong stigma rather than a curable illness.

Despite constantly and openly taking extensive notes on the behavior of the staff and other patients, none of the pseudopatients were identified as impostors by the hospital staff, although many of the other psychiatric patients seemed to be able to correctly identify them as impostors. In the first three hospitalizations, 35 of the total of 118 patients expressed a suspicion that the pseudopatients were sane, with some suggesting that the patients were researchers or journalists investigating the hospital. Hospital notes indicated that staff interpreted much of the pseudopatients' behavior in terms of mental illness. For example, one nurse labeled the note-taking of one pseudopatient as "writing behavior" and considered it pathological. The patients' normal biographies were recast in hospital records along the lines of what was expected of schizophrenics by the then-dominant theories of its cause.

The experiment required the pseudopatients to get out of the hospital on their own by getting the hospital to release them, though a lawyer was retained to be on call for emergencies when it became clear that the pseudopatients would not ever be voluntarily released on short notice. Once admitted and diagnosed, the pseudopatients were not able to obtain their release until they agreed with the psychiatrists that they were mentally ill and began taking antipsychotic medications, which they flushed down the toilet. No staff member reported that the pseudopatients were flushing their medication down the toilets.

Rosenhan and the other pseudopatients reported an overwhelming sense of dehumanization, severe invasion of privacy, and boredom while hospitalized. Their possessions were searched randomly, and they were sometimes observed while using the toilet. They reported that though the staff seemed to be well-meaning, they generally objectified and dehumanized the patients, often discussing patients at length in their presence as though they were not there, and avoiding direct interaction with patients except as strictly necessary to perform official duties. Some attendants were prone to verbal and physical abuse of patients when other staff were not present. A group of patients waiting outside the cafeteria half an hour before lunchtime were said by a doctor to his students to be experiencing "oral-acquisitive" psychiatric symptoms. Contact with doctors averaged 6.8 minutes per day.[8]

Non-existent impostor experiment edit

For this experiment, Rosenhan used a well-known research and teaching hospital, whose staff had heard of the results of the initial study but claimed that similar errors could not be made at their institution. Rosenhan arranged with them that during a three-month period, one or more pseudopatients would attempt to gain admission and the staff would rate every incoming patient as to the likelihood they were an impostor. Out of 193 patients, 41 were considered to be impostors and a further 42 were considered suspect. In reality, Rosenhan had sent no pseudopatients; all patients suspected as impostors by the hospital staff were ordinary patients. This led to a conclusion that "any diagnostic process that lends itself too readily to massive errors of this sort cannot be a very reliable one."[2]

Impact edit

Rosenhan published his findings in Science, in which he criticized the reliability of psychiatric diagnosis and the disempowering and demeaning nature of patient care experienced by the associates in the study.[2] In addition, he described his work in a variety of news appearances, including to the BBC:

I told friends, I told my family: "I can get out when I can get out. That's all. I'll be there for a couple of days and I'll get out." Nobody knew I'd be there for two months ... The only way out was to point out that they're [the psychiatrists are] correct. They had said I was insane, "I am insane; but I am getting better." That was an affirmation of their view of me.[9]

The experiment is said to have "accelerated the movement to reform mental institutions and to deinstitutionalize as many mental patients as possible".[4]

Criticisms edit

Many respondents to the publication defended psychiatry, saying that as psychiatric diagnosis relies largely on the patient's report of their experiences, faking their presence no more demonstrates problems with psychiatric diagnosis than lying about other medical symptoms. In this vein, psychiatrist Robert Spitzer quoted Seymour S. Kety in a 1975 criticism of Rosenhan's study:[5]

If I were to drink a quart of blood and, concealing what I had done, come to the emergency room of any hospital vomiting blood, the behavior of the staff would be quite predictable. If they labeled and treated me as having a bleeding peptic ulcer, I doubt that I could argue convincingly that medical science does not know how to diagnose that condition.

Kety also said that psychiatrists should not necessarily be expected to assume that a patient is pretending to have mental illness, thus the study lacked realism.[10] Instead of considering realistic problems in diagnosis, such as comorbidity or differential diagnosis between disorders with similar symptoms, Rosenhan dismissed the criticism as further examples of the "experimenter effect" or "expectation bias," and support for his interpretation that he had uncovered genuine problems of diagnosis rather than being fooled by his methodology.[11]

Accusation of fraud edit

In The Great Pretender, a 2019 book on Rosenhan, author Susannah Cahalan questions the veracity and validity of the Rosenhan experiment. Examining documents left behind by Rosenhan after his death, Cahalan finds apparent distortion in the Science article: inconsistent data, misleading descriptions, and inaccurate or fabricated quotations from psychiatric records. Moreover, despite an extensive search, she is only able to identify two of the eight pseudopatients: Rosenhan himself, and a graduate student whose testimony is allegedly inconsistent with Rosenhan's description in the article. In light of Rosenhan's seeming willingness to bend the truth in other ways regarding the experiment, Cahalan questions whether some or all of the six other pseudopatients might have been simply invented by Rosenhan.[12][13] In February 2023, Andrew Scull of the University of California at San Diego published an article in the peer-reviewed journal History of Psychiatry in support of Cahalan's allegations.[6]

Related experiments edit

In 1887 American investigative journalist Nellie Bly feigned symptoms of mental illness to gain admission to a lunatic asylum and report on the terrible conditions therein. The results were published as Ten Days in a Mad-House.[14]

In 1968 Maurice K. Temerlin split 25 psychiatrists into two groups and had them listen to an actor portraying a character of normal mental health. One group was told that the actor "was a very interesting man because he looked neurotic, but actually was quite psychotic" while the other was told nothing. Sixty percent of the former group diagnosed psychoses, most often schizophrenia, while none of the control group did so.[15][16]

In 1988, Loring and Powell gave 290 psychiatrists a transcript of a patient interview and told half of them that the patient was black and the other half white; they concluded of the results that "clinicians appear to ascribe violence, suspiciousness, and dangerousness to black clients even though the case studies are the same as the case studies for the white clients."[17]

In 2004, psychologist Lauren Slater claimed to have conducted an experiment very similar to Rosenhan's for her book Opening Skinner's Box.[3] Slater wrote that she had presented herself at 9 psychiatric emergency rooms with auditory hallucinations, resulting in being diagnosed "almost every time" with psychotic depression. However, when challenged to provide evidence of actually conducting her experiment, she could not.[18] The serious methodologic and other concerns regarding Slater's work appeared as a series of responses to a journal report, in the same journal.[19]

In 2008, the BBC's Horizon science program performed a similar experiment over two episodes entitled "How Mad Are You?". The experiment involved ten subjects, five with previously diagnosed mental health conditions, and five with no such diagnosis. They were observed by three experts in mental health diagnoses and their challenge was to identify the five with mental health problems solely from their behavior, without speaking to the subjects or learning anything of their histories.[20] The experts correctly diagnosed two of the ten patients, misdiagnosed one patient, and incorrectly identified two healthy patients as having mental health problems. Unlike the other experiments listed here, however, the aim of this journalistic exercise was not to criticize the diagnostic process, but to minimize the stigmatization of the mentally ill. It aimed to illustrate that people with a previous diagnosis of a mental illness could live normal lives with their health problems not obvious to observers from their behavior.[21][22]

See also edit

References edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ Gaughwin, Peter (2011). "On Being Insane in Medico-Legal Places: The Importance of Taking a Complete History in Forensic Mental Health Assessment". Psychiatry, Psychology and Law. 12 (1): 298–310. doi:10.1375/pplt.12.2.298. S2CID 53771539.
  2. ^ a b c Rosenhan, David (19 January 1973). . Science. 179 (4070): 250–258. Bibcode:1973Sci...179..250R. doi:10.1126/science.179.4070.250. PMID 4683124. S2CID 146772269. Archived from the original on 17 November 2004.
  3. ^ a b Slater, Lauren (2004). Opening Skinner's Box: Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0-393-05095-5.
  4. ^ a b Kornblum, William (2011). Mitchell, Erin; Jucha, Robert; Chell, John (eds.). Sociology in a Changing World (Google Books) (9th ed.). Cengage learning. p. 195. ISBN 978-1-111-30157-6.
  5. ^ a b Spitzer, Robert (October 1975). "On pseudoscience in science, logic in remission, and psychiatric diagnosis: a critique of Rosenhan's "On being sane in insane places"". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 84 (5): 442–52. doi:10.1037/h0077124. PMID 1194504. S2CID 8688334.
  6. ^ a b Scull, Andrew (3 February 2023). "Rosenhan revisited: Successful scientific fraud". History of Psychiatry. 34 (2): 180–195. doi:10.1177/0957154X221150878. PMID 36737877. S2CID 256577099.
  7. ^ . www.youtube.com. Archived from the original on February 1, 2015.
  8. ^ Ginn, Stephen. . Frontier Psychiatrist. Archived from the original on 2021-08-21.
  9. ^ Rosenhan, D.L. et al. The Trap. An excerpt from the BBC documentary with this statement by Rosenhan can be viewed in Drug Pushers, Drug Users, Antidepressants, & School Shooters February 19, 2017, at the Wayback Machine.
  10. ^ . Archived from the original on 2012-04-05. Retrieved 2012-04-13.
  11. ^ "The Rosenhan experiment examined" 2012-05-12 at the Wayback Machine, Frontier Psychiatrist[better source needed]
  12. ^ Abbott, Alison (29 October 2019). "On the troubling trail of psychiatry's pseudopatients stunt". Nature. 574 (7780): 622–623. Bibcode:2019Natur.574..622A. doi:10.1038/d41586-019-03268-y. "But some people in the department called him a bullshitter," Kenneth Gergen says. And through her deeply researched study, Cahalan seems inclined to agree with them.
  13. ^ "Review: 'The Great Pretender,' by Susannah Cahalan". Star Tribune.
  14. ^ Bly, Nellie (1887). Ten Days in a Mad-House. New York: Ian L. Munro. ISBN 9798622408274.
  15. ^ Temerlin, Maurice (October 1968). "Suggestion effects in psychiatric diagnosis". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 147 (4): 349–353. doi:10.1097/00005053-196810000-00003. PMID 5683680. S2CID 36672611.
  16. ^ "The Myth of Psychiatric Diagnosis". www.wayneramsay.com.
  17. ^ Loring, Marti; Powell, Brian (March 1988). "Gender, race, and DSM-III: a study of the objectivity of psychiatric diagnostic behavior". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 29 (1): 1–22. doi:10.2307/2137177. JSTOR 2137177. PMID 3367027.
  18. ^ Moran, Mark (7 April 2006). "Writer Ignites Firestorm With Misdiagnosis Claims". Psychiatric News. American Psychiatric Association. 41 (7): 10–12. doi:10.1176/pn.41.7.0010. ISSN 1559-1255.
  19. ^ See Lilienfeld, Scott; Spitzer, Robert; Miller, Michael (November 11, 2005). "A Response to a Nonresponse to Criticisms of a Nonstudy: One Humorous and One Serious Rejoinder to Slater". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 193 (11): 745–746. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000185884.74792.6d. PMID 16260930. S2CID 13523722 – via insights.ovid.com. and references cited therein.
  20. ^ "BBC – Health: BBC Health – About Headroom". Archived from the original on 2012-07-19.
  21. ^ . www.spotlightradio.net. Archived from the original on 2009-05-09.
  22. ^ . July 2, 2010. Archived from the original on 2010-07-02.

Bibliography edit

  • Slater, Lauren (2004). Opening Skinner's Box: Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century. W. W. Norton. pp. 64–94. ISBN 0-393-05095-5.

External links edit

  • On being sane in insane places
  • Rosenhan experiment summary
  • BBC Radio 4, "Mind Changers", Series 4 Episode 1: The Pseudo-Patient Study

rosenhan, experiment, thud, experiment, experiment, conducted, determine, validity, psychiatric, diagnosis, participants, submitted, themselves, evaluation, various, psychiatric, institutions, feigned, hallucinations, order, accepted, acted, normally, from, th. The Rosenhan experiment or Thud experiment was an experiment conducted to determine the validity of psychiatric diagnosis Participants submitted themselves for evaluation at various psychiatric institutions and feigned hallucinations in order to be accepted but acted normally from then onward Each was diagnosed with psychiatric disorders and were given antipsychotic medication The study was conducted by psychologist David Rosenhan a Stanford University professor and published by the journal Science in 1973 under the title On Being Sane in Insane Places 1 2 The main building of St Elizabeths Hospital 1996 located in Washington D C now part of the headquarters of the U S Department of Homeland Security was one of the sites of the Rosenhan experimentIt is considered by whom an important and influential criticism of psychiatric diagnosis and broached the topic of wrongful involuntary commitment 3 The experiment is said to have accelerated the movement to reform mental institutions and to deinstitutionalize as many mental patients as possible 4 Rosenhan along with eight other people five men and three women entered 12 hospitals across five states along the west coast of the US Three of the participants were admitted for only a short period of time and in order to obtain sufficient documented experiences they re applied to additional institutions Respondents defended psychiatry against the experiment s conclusions saying that as psychiatric diagnosis relies largely on the patient s report of their experiences faking their presence no more demonstrates problems with psychiatric diagnosis than lying about other medical symptoms 5 More recently it has been alleged that at least part of the published results were distorted or falsified 6 Contents 1 Pseudopatient experiment 2 Non existent impostor experiment 3 Impact 3 1 Criticisms 3 2 Accusation of fraud 4 Related experiments 5 See also 6 References 6 1 Notes 6 2 Bibliography 7 External linksPseudopatient experiment editRosenhan described his study as having two parts The first part involved the use of healthy associates or pseudopatients three women and six men including Rosenhan himself who briefly feigned auditory hallucinations in an attempt to gain admission to 12 psychiatric hospitals in five states in the United States All were admitted and diagnosed with psychiatric disorders After admission the pseudopatients acted normally and told staff that they no longer experienced any additional hallucinations As a condition of their release all the patients were forced to admit to having a mental illness and had to agree to take antipsychotic medication The average time that the patients spent in the hospital was 19 days All but one were diagnosed with schizophrenia in remission before their release The second part of his study involved a hospital administration challenging Rosenhan to send pseudopatients to its facility whose staff asserted that they would be able to detect the pseudopatients Rosenhan agreed and in the following weeks 41 out of 193 new patients were identified as potential pseudopatients with 19 of these receiving suspicion from at least one psychiatrist and one other staff member Rosenhan then revealed that he sent no pseudopatients to the hospital While listening to a lecture by R D Laing who was associated with the anti psychiatry movement Rosenhan conceived of the experiment as a way to test the reliability of psychiatric diagnoses 7 The study concluded it is clear that we cannot distinguish the sane from the insane in psychiatric hospitals and also illustrated the dangers of dehumanization and labeling in psychiatric institutions It suggested that the use of community mental health facilities which concentrated on specific problems and behaviors rather than psychiatric labels might be a solution and recommended education to make psychiatric workers more aware of the social psychology of their facilities Rosenhan himself and seven mentally healthy associates called pseudopatients attempted to gain admission to psychiatric hospitals by calling for an appointment and feigning auditory hallucinations The hospital staff were not informed of the experiment The pseudopatients included a psychology graduate student in his twenties three psychologists a pediatrician a psychiatrist a painter and a housewife None had a history of mental illness Pseudopatients used pseudonyms and those who worked in the mental health field were given false jobs in a different sector to avoid invoking any special treatment or scrutiny Apart from giving false names and employment details further biographical details were truthfully reported During their initial psychiatric assessment the pseudopatients claimed to be hearing voices of the same sex as the patient which were often unclear but which seemed to pronounce the words empty hollow or thud and nothing else These words were chosen as they vaguely suggest some sort of existential crisis and for the lack of any published literature referencing them as psychotic symptoms No other psychiatric symptoms were claimed If admitted the pseudopatients were instructed to act normally reporting that they felt fine and no longer heard voices Hospital records obtained after the experiment indicate that all pseudopatients were characterized as friendly and cooperative by staff All were admitted to 12 psychiatric hospitals across the United States including rundown and underfunded public hospitals in rural areas urban university run hospitals with excellent reputations and one expensive private hospital Though presented with identical symptoms seven were diagnosed with schizophrenia at public hospitals and one with manic depressive psychosis a more optimistic diagnosis with better clinical outcomes at the private hospital Their stays ranged from 7 to 52 days and the average was 19 days All but one were discharged with a diagnosis of schizophrenia in remission which Rosenhan considered as evidence that mental illness is perceived as an irreversible condition creating a lifelong stigma rather than a curable illness Despite constantly and openly taking extensive notes on the behavior of the staff and other patients none of the pseudopatients were identified as impostors by the hospital staff although many of the other psychiatric patients seemed to be able to correctly identify them as impostors In the first three hospitalizations 35 of the total of 118 patients expressed a suspicion that the pseudopatients were sane with some suggesting that the patients were researchers or journalists investigating the hospital Hospital notes indicated that staff interpreted much of the pseudopatients behavior in terms of mental illness For example one nurse labeled the note taking of one pseudopatient as writing behavior and considered it pathological The patients normal biographies were recast in hospital records along the lines of what was expected of schizophrenics by the then dominant theories of its cause The experiment required the pseudopatients to get out of the hospital on their own by getting the hospital to release them though a lawyer was retained to be on call for emergencies when it became clear that the pseudopatients would not ever be voluntarily released on short notice Once admitted and diagnosed the pseudopatients were not able to obtain their release until they agreed with the psychiatrists that they were mentally ill and began taking antipsychotic medications which they flushed down the toilet No staff member reported that the pseudopatients were flushing their medication down the toilets Rosenhan and the other pseudopatients reported an overwhelming sense of dehumanization severe invasion of privacy and boredom while hospitalized Their possessions were searched randomly and they were sometimes observed while using the toilet They reported that though the staff seemed to be well meaning they generally objectified and dehumanized the patients often discussing patients at length in their presence as though they were not there and avoiding direct interaction with patients except as strictly necessary to perform official duties Some attendants were prone to verbal and physical abuse of patients when other staff were not present A group of patients waiting outside the cafeteria half an hour before lunchtime were said by a doctor to his students to be experiencing oral acquisitive psychiatric symptoms Contact with doctors averaged 6 8 minutes per day 8 Non existent impostor experiment editFor this experiment Rosenhan used a well known research and teaching hospital whose staff had heard of the results of the initial study but claimed that similar errors could not be made at their institution Rosenhan arranged with them that during a three month period one or more pseudopatients would attempt to gain admission and the staff would rate every incoming patient as to the likelihood they were an impostor Out of 193 patients 41 were considered to be impostors and a further 42 were considered suspect In reality Rosenhan had sent no pseudopatients all patients suspected as impostors by the hospital staff were ordinary patients This led to a conclusion that any diagnostic process that lends itself too readily to massive errors of this sort cannot be a very reliable one 2 Impact editThis section needs additional citations to secondary or tertiary sourcessuch as review articles monographs or textbooks Please also establish the relevance for any primary research articles cited Unsourced or poorly sourced material may be challenged and removed November 2019 Learn how and when to remove this template message Rosenhan published his findings in Science in which he criticized the reliability of psychiatric diagnosis and the disempowering and demeaning nature of patient care experienced by the associates in the study 2 In addition he described his work in a variety of news appearances including to the BBC I told friends I told my family I can get out when I can get out That s all I ll be there for a couple of days and I ll get out Nobody knew I d be there for two months The only way out was to point out that they re the psychiatrists are correct They had said I was insane I am insane but I am getting better That was an affirmation of their view of me 9 The experiment is said to have accelerated the movement to reform mental institutions and to deinstitutionalize as many mental patients as possible 4 Criticisms edit Many respondents to the publication defended psychiatry saying that as psychiatric diagnosis relies largely on the patient s report of their experiences faking their presence no more demonstrates problems with psychiatric diagnosis than lying about other medical symptoms In this vein psychiatrist Robert Spitzer quoted Seymour S Kety in a 1975 criticism of Rosenhan s study 5 If I were to drink a quart of blood and concealing what I had done come to the emergency room of any hospital vomiting blood the behavior of the staff would be quite predictable If they labeled and treated me as having a bleeding peptic ulcer I doubt that I could argue convincingly that medical science does not know how to diagnose that condition Kety also said that psychiatrists should not necessarily be expected to assume that a patient is pretending to have mental illness thus the study lacked realism 10 Instead of considering realistic problems in diagnosis such as comorbidity or differential diagnosis between disorders with similar symptoms Rosenhan dismissed the criticism as further examples of the experimenter effect or expectation bias and support for his interpretation that he had uncovered genuine problems of diagnosis rather than being fooled by his methodology 11 Accusation of fraud edit In The Great Pretender a 2019 book on Rosenhan author Susannah Cahalan questions the veracity and validity of the Rosenhan experiment Examining documents left behind by Rosenhan after his death Cahalan finds apparent distortion in the Science article inconsistent data misleading descriptions and inaccurate or fabricated quotations from psychiatric records Moreover despite an extensive search she is only able to identify two of the eight pseudopatients Rosenhan himself and a graduate student whose testimony is allegedly inconsistent with Rosenhan s description in the article In light of Rosenhan s seeming willingness to bend the truth in other ways regarding the experiment Cahalan questions whether some or all of the six other pseudopatients might have been simply invented by Rosenhan 12 13 In February 2023 Andrew Scull of the University of California at San Diego published an article in the peer reviewed journal History of Psychiatry in support of Cahalan s allegations 6 Related experiments editIn 1887 American investigative journalist Nellie Bly feigned symptoms of mental illness to gain admission to a lunatic asylum and report on the terrible conditions therein The results were published as Ten Days in a Mad House 14 In 1968 Maurice K Temerlin split 25 psychiatrists into two groups and had them listen to an actor portraying a character of normal mental health One group was told that the actor was a very interesting man because he looked neurotic but actually was quite psychotic while the other was told nothing Sixty percent of the former group diagnosed psychoses most often schizophrenia while none of the control group did so 15 16 In 1988 Loring and Powell gave 290 psychiatrists a transcript of a patient interview and told half of them that the patient was black and the other half white they concluded of the results that clinicians appear to ascribe violence suspiciousness and dangerousness to black clients even though the case studies are the same as the case studies for the white clients 17 In 2004 psychologist Lauren Slater claimed to have conducted an experiment very similar to Rosenhan s for her book Opening Skinner s Box 3 Slater wrote that she had presented herself at 9 psychiatric emergency rooms with auditory hallucinations resulting in being diagnosed almost every time with psychotic depression However when challenged to provide evidence of actually conducting her experiment she could not 18 The serious methodologic and other concerns regarding Slater s work appeared as a series of responses to a journal report in the same journal 19 In 2008 the BBC s Horizon science program performed a similar experiment over two episodes entitled How Mad Are You The experiment involved ten subjects five with previously diagnosed mental health conditions and five with no such diagnosis They were observed by three experts in mental health diagnoses and their challenge was to identify the five with mental health problems solely from their behavior without speaking to the subjects or learning anything of their histories 20 The experts correctly diagnosed two of the ten patients misdiagnosed one patient and incorrectly identified two healthy patients as having mental health problems Unlike the other experiments listed here however the aim of this journalistic exercise was not to criticize the diagnostic process but to minimize the stigmatization of the mentally ill It aimed to illustrate that people with a previous diagnosis of a mental illness could live normal lives with their health problems not obvious to observers from their behavior 21 22 See also edit nbsp Science portal nbsp Medicine portal nbsp Psychiatry portal nbsp Psychology portalAnti psychiatry Movement against psychiatric treatment Involuntary commitment Compulsory hospitalization Labeling theory Labeling people changes their behavior Psychiatric hospital Undercover journalismReferences editNotes edit Gaughwin Peter 2011 On Being Insane in Medico Legal Places The Importance of Taking a Complete History in Forensic Mental Health Assessment Psychiatry Psychology and Law 12 1 298 310 doi 10 1375 pplt 12 2 298 S2CID 53771539 a b c Rosenhan David 19 January 1973 On being sane in insane places Science 179 4070 250 258 Bibcode 1973Sci 179 250R doi 10 1126 science 179 4070 250 PMID 4683124 S2CID 146772269 Archived from the original on 17 November 2004 a b Slater Lauren 2004 Opening Skinner s Box Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century W W Norton ISBN 0 393 05095 5 a b Kornblum William 2011 Mitchell Erin Jucha Robert Chell John eds Sociology in a Changing World Google Books 9th ed Cengage learning p 195 ISBN 978 1 111 30157 6 a b Spitzer Robert October 1975 On pseudoscience in science logic in remission and psychiatric diagnosis a critique of Rosenhan s On being sane in insane places Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84 5 442 52 doi 10 1037 h0077124 PMID 1194504 S2CID 8688334 a b Scull Andrew 3 February 2023 Rosenhan revisited Successful scientific fraud History of Psychiatry 34 2 180 195 doi 10 1177 0957154X221150878 PMID 36737877 S2CID 256577099 YouTube www youtube com Archived from the original on February 1 2015 Ginn Stephen The Rosenhan experiment examined Frontier Psychiatrist Archived from the original on 2021 08 21 Rosenhan D L et al The Trap An excerpt from the BBC documentary with this statement by Rosenhan can be viewed in Drug Pushers Drug Users Antidepressants amp School Shooters Archived February 19 2017 at the Wayback Machine Key Study Being Sane an Insane Places Archived from the original on 2012 04 05 Retrieved 2012 04 13 The Rosenhan experiment examined Archived 2012 05 12 at the Wayback Machine Frontier Psychiatrist better source needed Abbott Alison 29 October 2019 On the troubling trail of psychiatry s pseudopatients stunt Nature 574 7780 622 623 Bibcode 2019Natur 574 622A doi 10 1038 d41586 019 03268 y But some people in the department called him a bullshitter Kenneth Gergen says And through her deeply researched study Cahalan seems inclined to agree with them Review The Great Pretender by Susannah Cahalan Star Tribune Bly Nellie 1887 Ten Days in a Mad House New York Ian L Munro ISBN 9798622408274 Temerlin Maurice October 1968 Suggestion effects in psychiatric diagnosis The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 147 4 349 353 doi 10 1097 00005053 196810000 00003 PMID 5683680 S2CID 36672611 The Myth of Psychiatric Diagnosis www wayneramsay com Loring Marti Powell Brian March 1988 Gender race and DSM III a study of the objectivity of psychiatric diagnostic behavior Journal of Health and Social Behavior 29 1 1 22 doi 10 2307 2137177 JSTOR 2137177 PMID 3367027 Moran Mark 7 April 2006 Writer Ignites Firestorm With Misdiagnosis Claims Psychiatric News American Psychiatric Association 41 7 10 12 doi 10 1176 pn 41 7 0010 ISSN 1559 1255 See Lilienfeld Scott Spitzer Robert Miller Michael November 11 2005 A Response to a Nonresponse to Criticisms of a Nonstudy One Humorous and One Serious Rejoinder to Slater The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 193 11 745 746 doi 10 1097 01 nmd 0000185884 74792 6d PMID 16260930 S2CID 13523722 via insights ovid com and references cited therein BBC Health BBC Health About Headroom Archived from the original on 2012 07 19 How Mad Are You www spotlightradio net Archived from the original on 2009 05 09 How Mad Are You Spotlight July 2 2010 Archived from the original on 2010 07 02 Bibliography edit Slater Lauren 2004 Opening Skinner s Box Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century W W Norton pp 64 94 ISBN 0 393 05095 5 External links edit nbsp Look up pseudopatient in Wiktionary the free dictionary On being sane in insane places Rosenhan experiment summary BBC Radio 4 Mind Changers Series 4 Episode 1 The Pseudo Patient Study Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Rosenhan experiment amp oldid 1204902426, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.