fbpx
Wikipedia

Psychology of music preference

The psychology of music preference is the study of the psychological factors behind peoples' different music preferences. Music is heard by people daily in many parts of the world, and affects people in various ways from emotion regulation to cognitive development, along with providing a means for self-expression. Music training has been shown to help improve intellectual development and ability, though no connection has been found as to how it affects emotion regulation.[1] Numerous studies have been conducted to show that individual personality can have an effect on music preference, mostly using personality, though a recent meta-analysis has shown that personality in itself explains little variance in music preferences.[2] These studies are not limited to American culture, as they have been conducted with significant results in countries all over the world, including Japan,[3] Germany,[4] Spain,[5] and Brazil.[6]

Personality and music preference edit

Personality edit

The relationship between musical preference and personality has remained a long-standing topic of contention for researchers due to the variability in results and the low-predictive power that personality has demonstrated on music preferences.[2] A meta-analysis conducted by Schäfer and Mehlorn, (2017) of previous studies trying to determine if experience seeking or any of the Big-Five personality traits predicted musical preferences, revealed that the correlation coefficient between music genre and personality traits possessed a magnitude greater than 0.1 in only 6 out of the 30 studies they reviewed. Openness to experience was found to have the strongest correlation with a preference for three musical styles; however, this correlation was still relatively minor. The vast majority of the correlation coefficients were almost zero.[2]

Recent research has used data from music streaming sites instead of self-reported data to show that music genres and the user's listening habits can be used to predict the Big Five personality traits.[7] In a study conducted in 2020, researchers analyzed data from Spotify users and mapped the Big Five personality traits to different genres, moods, and derived metrics such as the number of playlists, diversity in song choice, and repetition of listening habits. Results showed an increase in correlation. Openness had a positive correlation with Atmospheric, Reggae, and Folk music, while conscientiousness had a negative correlation with Rock and "Energizing" songs such as "Happy" by Pharrell Williams.[7] In terms of derived metrics, emotional stability had a negative correlation with the listener's skip rate, and openness had a positive correlation with their song discovery rate.[7]

One shortcoming in attempting to establish a predictive relationship between music and personality is likely a result of researchers' tendency to utilize overly homogenized musical genres, failing to account for the variance within musical genres.[8] In a study conducted by Brisson and Bianchi (2021), participants were provided a musical taste inventory and asked to rate their musical genre and sub-genre preferences. For example, electronic music is considered a genre and house music is a sub-genre of electronic music. Results indicated a high degree of variance between participant genre and sub-genre preferences. Enjoying one genre or sub-genre within a broader genre category often failed to consistently predict ratings for related genres and sub-genres with another related genre or sub-genre. Ultimately, only two consistent associations between genres and sub-genres were found, calling into question the reliability of musical genres in research.[8] However, the investigation into this relationship between the influence of personality on music preference remains ongoing despite these genre-based limitations in methodology and past discrepancies in research results.

Various questionnaires have been created to both measure the big five personality traits and musical preferences. The majority of studies attempting to find the correlation between personality and musical preferences administered questionnaires to measure both traits.[9][10][5][11][12][13][4][14] Others used questionnaires to determine personality traits, and then asked participants to rate musical excerpts on scales such as liking, perceived complexity, emotions felt, and more.[9][15][16][17]

In general, the plasticity traits (openness to experience and extraversion) affect music preference more than the stability traits (agreeableness, neuroticism, and conscientiousness),[18] but each trait is still worth discussing. The personality traits have also been shown to correlate significantly with the emotional effect music has on people. Individual personality differences can help predict the emotional intensity and valence derived from music.[19]

Big Five personality traits edit

Openness to experience edit

Of all the traits, openness to experience has been shown to have the greatest effect upon genre preference.[9][10][20] In general, those rated high in openness to experience prefer music categorized as more complex and novel, such as classical, jazz, and eclecticism,[21] as well as intense and rebellious music.[22][4][10][3] In the study, reflective and complex genres included classical, blues, jazz, and folk music, while intense and rebellious genres included rock, alternative, and heavy metal music.[4] One of the facets of openness to experience is aesthetic appreciation, which is how researchers generally explain the high positive correlation between openness and liking complex music.[11]

One study looking at how personality traits affect music-induced emotion found that, of all the traits, openness to experience was the best predictor of higher emotionally intense reactions to sad and slow music. The most common feelings described from sad music were nostalgia, peacefulness, and wonder, and openness to experience correlated positively with all these feelings.[17] Sad music has also been theorized to allow for greater experience of aesthetic experiences and beauty.[17] Furthermore, individuals scoring high on openness to experience show a preference for diverse musical styles, but do not prefer popular forms of contemporary music, indicating that there are limits to this openness.[23] However, this is only true up to a certain point, as another study looked at music's ability to produce "chills" in the listeners. Although this study found that openness was the best predictor of genre preference, there is no way to use openness to experience to predict whether one will get chills from music or not. Instead, the only measure for that was frequency of listening to music and the self-designated value of the importance of music in one's life.[20]

Another study examined how openness to experience and frequency of listening are related and how they affect music preference. While listening to classical music excerpts, those rated high in openness tended to decrease in liking music faster during repeated listenings, as opposed to those scoring low in openness, who tended to like music more with repeated plays. This suggests novelty in music is an important quality for people high in openness to experience.[24]

One study had people take a personality test before and after listening to classical music with and without written lyrics in front of them. Music both with and without lyrics showed some effect on people's self-reported personality traits, most significantly in terms of openness to experience, which showed a significant increase.[25] Instead of personality affecting music preference, here classical music altered the assessment of their own personalities and made people assess themselves as more open to experience.

Openness to experience is also positively correlated with frequency of intellectual or cognitive use of music, such as analyzing complex musical compositions.[11][12][14][5] Furthermore, individuals more open to experience prefer a greater number of melodic themes in a work of music.[26][13]

Conscientiousness edit

Conscientiousness is negatively correlated with intense and rebellious music, such as rock and heavy metal music.[4] While previous studies have found an association between conscientiousness and emotional regulation, these results do not apply cross-culturally—specifically, researchers did not find this association in Malaysia.[12]

Extroversion edit

Extroversion is another good predictor of music genre preference and music use. Energetic extroverts have been linked to preferences in happy, upbeat and conventional music, as well as energetic and rhythmic music, such as rap, hip hop, soul, electronic, and dance music.[11][4] Additionally, extroverts tend to listen to music more and have background music present in their lives more often.[11] One study compared introverts and extroverts to see who would be more easily distracted by background music with and without lyrics. It was assumed that since extroverts listen to background music more they would be able to tune it out better, but that was proved untrue. No matter how much music people listen to they are still equally affected and distracted by music with lyrics.[27] Cheerful music with fast tempos, many melodic themes, and vocals are also preferred by extroverts.[14][13][3] They are more likely than others to listen to music in the background while doing other activities, such as running, being with friends, or studying.[5][12][11] This group also tends to use music to counter the monotony of everyday tasks, such as ironing.[12] In a Turkish study, researchers found that extroverts preferred rock, pop, and rap because these genres facilitated dance and movement.

Another study examined music teachers and music therapists, assuming that people who like and study music would be more extroverted. The results showed that music teachers were definitely higher in extroversion than the general public. Music therapists were also higher on extroversion than introversion, though they scored significantly lower than the teachers.[26] Differences can probably be attributed to teaching being a profession more dependent on extroversion.

Agreeableness edit

Agreeable individuals preferred upbeat and conventional music.[4] Agreeableness has also been shown to correlate with more easy-going, relaxed music preferences.[28] Additionally, listeners with high agreeableness displayed an intense emotional response to music which they had never before listened to.[15] Agreeableness is also a good predictor of the emotional intensity experienced from all types of music, both positive and negative. Those scoring high in agreeableness tend to have more intense emotional reactions to all types of music.[15]

Neuroticism edit

The more neurotic a person is, the more likely they are to listen to intense and rebellious music (such as alternative, rock and heavy metal).[4] Additionally, neuroticism is positively correlated with emotional use of music.[11][5] Those who scored high in neuroticism were more likely to report use of music for emotional regulation and experience higher intensity of emotional affect, especially negative emotion.[11][12]

Individual and situational influences on musical preferences edit

Situations have been shown to influence individual’s preferences for certain types of music. Participants in a study from 1996 provided information about what music they would prefer to listen to in given situations, and indicated that the situation greatly determined their musical preferences. For example, melancholic situations called for sad and moody music, while an arousal situation would call for loud, strong rhythm, invigorating music.[29]

Gender edit

Women are more likely than men to respond to music in a more emotional way.[5] Furthermore, women prefer popular music more than men.[23] In a study of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music, researchers found that men demonstrated more of a preference for bass music than women. This preference for bass music is also correlated with antisocial and borderline personalities.[30]

Culture edit

Research has shown culture can influence musical preferences. Humans tend to enjoy culturally similar music more than culturally atypical music.[31][32] In a study conducted by McDermott and Schultz (2016), participants from three regions in Bolivia (La Paz, San Borja, and Santa Maria) and the United States were evaluated for differences in music preferences.[31] The participants from the United States were divided into two groups, with one group possessing at least two years of experience playing an instrument and the other group having a maximum of one year of experience. Participants from Bolivia were divided into groups based on their region, with each region having experienced varying levels of exposure to Western culture. The group of participants from Santa Maria, the Tsimane, were of specific interest due to the substantial differences between their music and Western music in addition to their relative isolation from Western cultural influences.[31] The participants were exposed to traditionally consonant and dissonant sounds from Western music and asked to rate how pleasant they found the sound. Results indicated that Americans, particularly trained musicians, demonstrated the strongest preference for consonances in their music.[31] The groups from La Paz and San Borja also demonstrated some preference for consonance. However, the Tsimane enjoyed both consonant and dissonant chords equally, suggesting that consonance preferences in music are developed through cultural exposure and humans have no innate preferences for certain harmonies or chords.[31]

Culture changes how music is perceived by an individual, and alters affective response to music, particularly in a social context.[32] People feel more connected with one another when listening to culturally typical music than when they listen to culturally atypical music. Previous research has suggested that listening to familiar music might also aid in establishing more significant movement predictability, which can result in more movement synchronicity between individuals, further amplifying feelings of group connection and enjoyment of music.[32] Culture also affects a person's ability to remember music.[33] A study on cross-cultural music memory was conducted by Demorest and Morrison (2016). In the study, Western participants were played six musical excerpts. Three of the excerpts were from Turkish classical music, and the other three were from Western classical music. The music was presented in three different musical contexts (original, monophonic and isochronous).Once all of the excerpts were played, participants received a memory test. The memory test results indicate that participant memory was consistently better at recalling in-cultural music than unfamiliar music.[33]

Age edit

Age is a strong factor in determining music preference.[9][34] There is also evidence that preferences and opinions toward music can change with age.[9][35] In a Canadian study concerning how adolescent music preferences relate to personality, researchers found that adolescents who preferred heavy music demonstrated low self-esteem, higher levels of discomfort within the family, and tended to feel rejected by others. Adolescents who preferred light music were preoccupied with doing the proper thing, and had difficulty balancing independence with dependence. Adolescents who had eclectic music preferences had less difficulty negotiating adolescence, and were flexible using music according to mood and particular needs at the time.[36]

Self views edit

Music preferences can also be influenced by how the individual wants to be perceived, especially in males.[11] Music preferences could be used to make self-directed identity claims. Individuals might select styles of music that reinforce their self-views. For examples, individuals with a conservative self-view preferred conventional styles of music, while individuals with an athletic self view preferred vigorous music.[14] Individuals will unconsciously push perceptions into their environments, music makes this evident. In a 1953 study, Cattell and Anderson began the process of determining musical preference through unconscious traits.[22] While their findings were inconclusive, it created a research basis for later cases. Music is a way similar to diet and physique to outwardly express internal characteristics. Rentfrow and Gosling found through their study of the seven domains that for many, music was placed quite high in the rankings.[14]

Mood edit

Active mood is another factor that affects music preference. Generally whether people are in a good or bad mood when they hear music affects how they feel about the type of music and also their emotional response.[19] On that line of thinking, aggression has been shown to improve creativity and emotional intensity derived from music. People with aggressive disorders find music to be a powerful emotional outlet.[37] Depression also impacts perceptions and emotional responses generated from music. Depressed individuals rate their music-associated episodic memories lower than non-depressed individuals.[38] Additionally, the value people put on music and frequency of listening affects their reactions to it. If people listen to a certain type of music and add emotional experience to songs or a genre in general, this increases the likelihood of enjoying the music and being emotionally affected by it.[20] This helps explain why many people might have strong reactions to music their parents listened to frequently when they were children.

Movement edit

Research suggests that body movement may play a role in determining musical tastes.[39] Body movement can create either positive or negative feelings towards music depending on if the movement is associated with a positive or negative affective state. A study conducted by Weigelt and Walther (2011) demonstrated that performing a positive physical movement while listening to a song led to an increased preference rating for that song compared to a negative physical movement. Movements associated with smiling muscles (both positive or negative) were found to have the largest effect on musical preference.[39]

Productiveness edit

If someone is listening to music with the ultimate goal of completing a task, their musical preference is greatly increased. The more a genre of music increases one's productiveness, the more the individual will gravitate toward that genre to complete future tasks.[40]

In turn, music can increase focus in some. It can help your brain interpret information and gain a better understanding of new things more easily. Music can engage the brain in many different ways, whether that be making one more attentive, focused, increased concentration etc.[41]

According to a 2017 study, soft, fast music was concluded to have a positive effect on productiveness. Songs with lyrics are potentially more distracting than instrumental melodies and music with other modes or tempos could possibly evoke obstructive emotions for learning. Music can improve one's mood, create a positive mindset, reduce stress etc., this can directly correlate to learning improvements.[42]

Season of the year edit

Season of the year can also affect preferences. After reflecting upon fall or winter seasons, participants preferred reflective and complex music, whereas after reflecting upon summer or spring, participants preferred energetic and rhythmic music. However, "pop" music seems to have a universal appeal, despite the season.[43]

Familiarity edit

Familiarity and complexity both have interesting effects on musical preferences. As seen in other types of artistic media, an inverted U relationship is apparent when relating subjective complexity on liking music excerpts. Individuals like complexity to a certain degree, then begin to dislike the music when complexity gets too high. Furthermore, there is a clear positive monotonic relationship between familiarity and liking of music.[44]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Schellenberg, Glen E.; Mankarious, Monika (October 2012). "Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood". Emotion. 12 (5): 887–891. doi:10.1037/a0027971. PMID 22642351.
  2. ^ a b c Schäfer, Thomas; Mehlhorn, Claudia (29 April 2017). "Can personality traits predict musical style preferences? A meta-analysis". Personality and Individual Differences. 116: 265–273. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.061.
  3. ^ a b c Brown, R. A. (1 August 2012). "Music preferences and personality among Japanese university students". International Journal of Psychology. 47 (4): 259–268. doi:10.1080/00207594.2011.631544. PMID 22248342.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h Langmeyer, Alexandra; Guglhör-Rudan, Angelika; Tarnai, Christian (1 January 2012). "What Do Music Preferences Reveal About Personality?". Journal of Individual Differences. 33 (2): 119–130. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000082.
  5. ^ a b c d e f Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Gomà-i-Freixanet, Montserrat; Furnham, Adrian; Muro, Anna (1 January 2009). "Personality, self-estimated intelligence, and uses of music: A Spanish replication and extension using structural equation modeling". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 3 (3): 149–155. doi:10.1037/a0015342.
  6. ^ Herrera, Lucia; Soares-Quadros, João F. Jr; Lorenzo, Oswaldo (2018). "Music Preferences and Personality in Brazilians". Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 1488. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01488. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 6113570. PMID 30186197.
  7. ^ a b c Anderson, Ian; Gil, Santiago; Gibson, Clay; Wolf, Scott; Shapiro, Will; Semerci, Oguz; Greenberg, David M. (May 2021). ""Just the Way You Are": Linking Music Listening on Spotify and Personality". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 12 (4): 561–572. doi:10.1177/1948550620923228. ISSN 1948-5506. S2CID 233194549.
  8. ^ a b Brisson, Romain; Bianchi, Renzo (2020). "On the relevance of music genre-based analysis in research on musical tastes". Psychology of Music. 48 (6): 777–794. doi:10.1177/0305735619828810. S2CID 151126547.
  9. ^ a b c d e Bonneville-Roussy, Arielle; Rentfrow, Peter J.; Xu, Man K.; Potter, Jeff (2013). "Music through the ages: Trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 105 (4): 703–717. doi:10.1037/a0033770. PMID 23895269.
  10. ^ a b c Zweigenhaft, Richard L. (1 January 2008). "A Do Re Mi Encore". Journal of Individual Differences. 29 (1): 45–55. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.29.1.45.
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h i Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Fagan, Patrick; Furnham, Adrian (1 January 2010). "Personality and uses of music as predictors of preferences for music consensually classified as happy, sad, complex, and social". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 4 (4): 205–213. doi:10.1037/a0019210.
  12. ^ a b c d e f Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Swami, Viren; Furnham, Adrian; Maakip, Ismail (1 January 2009). "The Big Five Personality Traits and Uses of Music". Journal of Individual Differences. 30 (1): 20–27. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.30.1.20.
  13. ^ a b c Kopacz, Malgorzata (2005). "Personality and music preferences: The influence of personality traits on preferences regarding musical elements". Journal of Music Therapy. 42 (3): 216–239. doi:10.1093/jmt/42.3.216. PMID 16086606.
  14. ^ a b c d e Rentfrow, Peter J.; Gosling, Samuel D. (1 January 2003). "The do re mi's of everyday life: The structure and personality correlates of music preferences". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (6): 1236–1256. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236. PMID 12793587. S2CID 16489081.
  15. ^ a b c Ladinig, Olivia; Schellenberg, E. Glenn (1 January 2012). "Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 6 (2): 146–154. doi:10.1037/a0024671. S2CID 56162148.
  16. ^ Tekman, Hasan Gürkan; Hortaçsu, Nuran (1 October 2002). "Music and social identity: Stylistic identification as a response to musical style". International Journal of Psychology. 37 (5): 277–285. doi:10.1080/00207590244000043.
  17. ^ a b c Vuoskoski, Jonna K.; Thompson, William F.; McIlwain, Doris; Eerola, Tuomas (February 2012). "Who enjoys listening to sad music and why?". Music Perception. 29 (3): 311–317. doi:10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.311.
  18. ^ Miranda, Dave; Morizot, Julien; Gaudreau, Patrick (27 March 2012). "Personality Metatraits and Music Preferences in Adolescence: A Pilot Study". International Journal of Adolescence and Youth. 15 (4): 289–301. doi:10.1080/02673843.2010.9748036. S2CID 145681242.
  19. ^ a b Vuoskoski, Jonna K.; Eerola, Tuomas (13 July 2011). "Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences". Musicae Scientiae. 15 (2): 159–173. doi:10.1177/1029864911403367. S2CID 144079608.
  20. ^ a b c Nusbaum, E. C.; Silvia, P. J. (7 October 2010). "Shivers and Timbres: Personality and the Experience of Chills From Music" (PDF). Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2 (2): 199–204. doi:10.1177/1948550610386810. S2CID 54188929.
  21. ^ Dunn, Peter G.; de Ruyter, Boris; Bouwhuis, Don G. (16 March 2011). "Toward a better understanding of the relation between music preference, listening behavior, and personality". Psychology of Music. 40 (4): 411–428. doi:10.1177/0305735610388897. S2CID 54736250.
  22. ^ a b Rentfrow, Peter J.; Goldberg, Lewis R.; Levitin, Daniel J. (June 2011). "The Structure of Musical Preferences: A Five-Factor Model". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (6): 1139–1157. doi:10.1037/a0022406. ISSN 0022-3514. PMC 3138530. PMID 21299309.
  23. ^ a b Rawlings, D.; Ciancarelli, V. (1 October 1997). "Music Preference and the Five-Factor Model of the NEO Personality Inventory". Psychology of Music. 25 (2): 120–132. doi:10.1177/0305735697252003. S2CID 145601419.
  24. ^ Hunter, Patrick G; Schellenberg, Glen E. (20 October 2010). "Interactive effects of personality and frequency of exposure on liking for music". Personality and Individual Differences. 50 (2): 175–179. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.021.
  25. ^ Djikic, Maja (August 2011). "The effect of music and lyrics on personality". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 5 (3): 237–240. doi:10.1037/a0022313.
  26. ^ a b Steele, Anita Louise; Young, Sylvester (Spring 2011). "A descriptive study of Myers-Briggs personality types of professional music educators and music therapists with comparisons to undergraduate majors". Journal of Music Therapy. 48 (1): 55–73. doi:10.1093/jmt/48.1.55. PMID 21866713.
  27. ^ Avila, Christina; Furnham, Adrian; McClelland, Alastair (9 November 2011). "The influence of distracting familiar vocal music on cognitive performance of introverts and extraverts". Psychology of Music. 40 (1): 84–93. doi:10.1177/0305735611422672. S2CID 145340833.
  28. ^ Ruth, Nicolas; Müllensiefen, Daniel (December 2020). "Associations between music preferences and personality in female secondary school students". Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain. 30 (4): 202–211. doi:10.1037/pmu0000267. S2CID 222113433.
  29. ^ North, Adrian; Hargreaves, David (1996). "Situational influences on reported musical preference". Psychomusicology. 15 (1–2): 30–45. doi:10.1037/h0094081.
  30. ^ McCown, William; Keiser, Ross; Mulhearn, Shea; Williamson, David (1997). "The role of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music". Personality and Individual Differences. 23 (4): 543–547. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(97)00085-8.
  31. ^ a b c d e McDermott, Josh; Schultz, Alan (July 13, 2016). "Indifference to dissonance in native Amazonians reveals cultural variation in music perception". Nature. 535 (7613): 547–550. Bibcode:2016Natur.535..547M. doi:10.1038/nature18635. PMID 27409816. S2CID 205249764.
  32. ^ a b c Stupacher, Jan; Wood, Guilherme (July 2018). "Effects of cultural background and musical preference on affective social entrainment with music". Proceedings of ICMPC15/ESCOM10: 438–441.
  33. ^ a b Demorest, Steven M.; Morrison, Steven J. (June 1, 2016). "The influence of contextual cues on cultural bias in music memory". Music Perception. 33 (5): 590–600. doi:10.1525/mp.2016.33.5.590. JSTOR 26417336.
  34. ^ Barret, Frederick S.; Grimm, Kevin J.; Robins, Richard W.; Wildschut, Tim; Constantine, Sedikides; Janata, Petr (June 2010). "Music-evoked nostalgia: Affect, memory, and personality". Emotion. 10 (3): 390–403. doi:10.1037/a0019006. PMID 20515227.
  35. ^ Crowther, R; Durkin, K (1982). "Sex- and age-related differences in the musical behavior, interests and attitudes towards music of 232 secondary school students". Educational Studies. 8 (2): 131–139. doi:10.1080/0305569820080206.
  36. ^ Schwartz, Kelly; Fouts, Gregory (2003). "Music preferences, personality style, and developmental issues of adolescents". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 32 (3): 205–213. doi:10.1023/a:1022547520656. S2CID 41849910.
  37. ^ Pool, Jonathan; Odell-Miller, Helen (2011). "Aggression in music therapy and its role in creativity with reference to personality disorder". The Arts in Psychotherapy. 38 (3): 169–177. doi:10.1016/j.aip.2011.04.003.
  38. ^ Sakka, Laura; Juslin, Patrik (October 10, 2017). "Emotional reactions to music in depressed individuals". Psychology of Music. 46 (6): 862–880. doi:10.1177/0305735617730425. S2CID 148886910.
  39. ^ a b Weigelt, Oliver; Walther, Eva (February 2011). "Music is in the Muscle: How Embodied Cognition May Influence Music Preferences". Music Perception. 28 (3): 297–306. doi:10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.297.
  40. ^ Schäfer, Thomas (2016-03-17). "The Goals and Effects of Music Listening and Their Relationship to the Strength of Music Preference". PLOS ONE. 11 (3): e0151634. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1151634S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151634. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4795651. PMID 26985998.
  41. ^ Sridharan, Devarajan; Levitin, Daniel J.; Chafe, Chris H.; Berger, Jonathan; Menon, Vinod (2007-08-02). "Neural Dynamics of Event Segmentation in Music: Converging Evidence for Dissociable Ventral and Dorsal Networks". Neuron. 55 (3): 521–532. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.003. ISSN 0896-6273. PMID 17678862. S2CID 6846417.
  42. ^ Lehmann, Janina A. M.; Seufert, Tina (2017). "The Influence of Background Music on Learning in the Light of Different Theoretical Perspectives and the Role of Working Memory Capacity". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1902. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01902. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5671572. PMID 29163283.
  43. ^ Pettijohn, Terry F.; Williams, Greg M.; Carter, Tiffany C. (26 November 2010). "Music for the Seasons: Seasonal Music Preferences in College Students". Current Psychology. 29 (4): 328–345. doi:10.1007/s12144-010-9092-8. S2CID 56037002.
  44. ^ North, Adrian; Hargreaves, David (1995). "Subjective complexity, familiarity, and liking for popular music". Psychomusicology. 14 (1–2): 77–93. doi:10.1037/h0094090.

psychology, music, preference, psychology, music, preference, study, psychological, factors, behind, peoples, different, music, preferences, music, heard, people, daily, many, parts, world, affects, people, various, ways, from, emotion, regulation, cognitive, . The psychology of music preference is the study of the psychological factors behind peoples different music preferences Music is heard by people daily in many parts of the world and affects people in various ways from emotion regulation to cognitive development along with providing a means for self expression Music training has been shown to help improve intellectual development and ability though no connection has been found as to how it affects emotion regulation 1 Numerous studies have been conducted to show that individual personality can have an effect on music preference mostly using personality though a recent meta analysis has shown that personality in itself explains little variance in music preferences 2 These studies are not limited to American culture as they have been conducted with significant results in countries all over the world including Japan 3 Germany 4 Spain 5 and Brazil 6 Contents 1 Personality and music preference 1 1 Personality 1 2 Big Five personality traits 1 2 1 Openness to experience 1 2 2 Conscientiousness 1 2 3 Extroversion 1 2 4 Agreeableness 1 2 5 Neuroticism 2 Individual and situational influences on musical preferences 2 1 Gender 2 2 Culture 2 3 Age 2 4 Self views 2 5 Mood 2 6 Movement 2 7 Productiveness 2 8 Season of the year 2 9 Familiarity 3 See also 4 ReferencesPersonality and music preference editPersonality edit The relationship between musical preference and personality has remained a long standing topic of contention for researchers due to the variability in results and the low predictive power that personality has demonstrated on music preferences 2 A meta analysis conducted by Schafer and Mehlorn 2017 of previous studies trying to determine if experience seeking or any of the Big Five personality traits predicted musical preferences revealed that the correlation coefficient between music genre and personality traits possessed a magnitude greater than 0 1 in only 6 out of the 30 studies they reviewed Openness to experience was found to have the strongest correlation with a preference for three musical styles however this correlation was still relatively minor The vast majority of the correlation coefficients were almost zero 2 Recent research has used data from music streaming sites instead of self reported data to show that music genres and the user s listening habits can be used to predict the Big Five personality traits 7 In a study conducted in 2020 researchers analyzed data from Spotify users and mapped the Big Five personality traits to different genres moods and derived metrics such as the number of playlists diversity in song choice and repetition of listening habits Results showed an increase in correlation Openness had a positive correlation with Atmospheric Reggae and Folk music while conscientiousness had a negative correlation with Rock and Energizing songs such as Happy by Pharrell Williams 7 In terms of derived metrics emotional stability had a negative correlation with the listener s skip rate and openness had a positive correlation with their song discovery rate 7 One shortcoming in attempting to establish a predictive relationship between music and personality is likely a result of researchers tendency to utilize overly homogenized musical genres failing to account for the variance within musical genres 8 In a study conducted by Brisson and Bianchi 2021 participants were provided a musical taste inventory and asked to rate their musical genre and sub genre preferences For example electronic music is considered a genre and house music is a sub genre of electronic music Results indicated a high degree of variance between participant genre and sub genre preferences Enjoying one genre or sub genre within a broader genre category often failed to consistently predict ratings for related genres and sub genres with another related genre or sub genre Ultimately only two consistent associations between genres and sub genres were found calling into question the reliability of musical genres in research 8 However the investigation into this relationship between the influence of personality on music preference remains ongoing despite these genre based limitations in methodology and past discrepancies in research results Various questionnaires have been created to both measure the big five personality traits and musical preferences The majority of studies attempting to find the correlation between personality and musical preferences administered questionnaires to measure both traits 9 10 5 11 12 13 4 14 Others used questionnaires to determine personality traits and then asked participants to rate musical excerpts on scales such as liking perceived complexity emotions felt and more 9 15 16 17 In general the plasticity traits openness to experience and extraversion affect music preference more than the stability traits agreeableness neuroticism and conscientiousness 18 but each trait is still worth discussing The personality traits have also been shown to correlate significantly with the emotional effect music has on people Individual personality differences can help predict the emotional intensity and valence derived from music 19 Big Five personality traits edit Main article Big Five personality traits Openness to experience edit Of all the traits openness to experience has been shown to have the greatest effect upon genre preference 9 10 20 In general those rated high in openness to experience prefer music categorized as more complex and novel such as classical jazz and eclecticism 21 as well as intense and rebellious music 22 4 10 3 In the study reflective and complex genres included classical blues jazz and folk music while intense and rebellious genres included rock alternative and heavy metal music 4 One of the facets of openness to experience is aesthetic appreciation which is how researchers generally explain the high positive correlation between openness and liking complex music 11 One study looking at how personality traits affect music induced emotion found that of all the traits openness to experience was the best predictor of higher emotionally intense reactions to sad and slow music The most common feelings described from sad music were nostalgia peacefulness and wonder and openness to experience correlated positively with all these feelings 17 Sad music has also been theorized to allow for greater experience of aesthetic experiences and beauty 17 Furthermore individuals scoring high on openness to experience show a preference for diverse musical styles but do not prefer popular forms of contemporary music indicating that there are limits to this openness 23 However this is only true up to a certain point as another study looked at music s ability to produce chills in the listeners Although this study found that openness was the best predictor of genre preference there is no way to use openness to experience to predict whether one will get chills from music or not Instead the only measure for that was frequency of listening to music and the self designated value of the importance of music in one s life 20 Another study examined how openness to experience and frequency of listening are related and how they affect music preference While listening to classical music excerpts those rated high in openness tended to decrease in liking music faster during repeated listenings as opposed to those scoring low in openness who tended to like music more with repeated plays This suggests novelty in music is an important quality for people high in openness to experience 24 One study had people take a personality test before and after listening to classical music with and without written lyrics in front of them Music both with and without lyrics showed some effect on people s self reported personality traits most significantly in terms of openness to experience which showed a significant increase 25 Instead of personality affecting music preference here classical music altered the assessment of their own personalities and made people assess themselves as more open to experience Openness to experience is also positively correlated with frequency of intellectual or cognitive use of music such as analyzing complex musical compositions 11 12 14 5 Furthermore individuals more open to experience prefer a greater number of melodic themes in a work of music 26 13 Conscientiousness edit Conscientiousness is negatively correlated with intense and rebellious music such as rock and heavy metal music 4 While previous studies have found an association between conscientiousness and emotional regulation these results do not apply cross culturally specifically researchers did not find this association in Malaysia 12 Extroversion edit Extroversion is another good predictor of music genre preference and music use Energetic extroverts have been linked to preferences in happy upbeat and conventional music as well as energetic and rhythmic music such as rap hip hop soul electronic and dance music 11 4 Additionally extroverts tend to listen to music more and have background music present in their lives more often 11 One study compared introverts and extroverts to see who would be more easily distracted by background music with and without lyrics It was assumed that since extroverts listen to background music more they would be able to tune it out better but that was proved untrue No matter how much music people listen to they are still equally affected and distracted by music with lyrics 27 Cheerful music with fast tempos many melodic themes and vocals are also preferred by extroverts 14 13 3 They are more likely than others to listen to music in the background while doing other activities such as running being with friends or studying 5 12 11 This group also tends to use music to counter the monotony of everyday tasks such as ironing 12 In a Turkish study researchers found that extroverts preferred rock pop and rap because these genres facilitated dance and movement Another study examined music teachers and music therapists assuming that people who like and study music would be more extroverted The results showed that music teachers were definitely higher in extroversion than the general public Music therapists were also higher on extroversion than introversion though they scored significantly lower than the teachers 26 Differences can probably be attributed to teaching being a profession more dependent on extroversion Agreeableness edit Agreeable individuals preferred upbeat and conventional music 4 Agreeableness has also been shown to correlate with more easy going relaxed music preferences 28 Additionally listeners with high agreeableness displayed an intense emotional response to music which they had never before listened to 15 Agreeableness is also a good predictor of the emotional intensity experienced from all types of music both positive and negative Those scoring high in agreeableness tend to have more intense emotional reactions to all types of music 15 Neuroticism edit The more neurotic a person is the more likely they are to listen to intense and rebellious music such as alternative rock and heavy metal 4 Additionally neuroticism is positively correlated with emotional use of music 11 5 Those who scored high in neuroticism were more likely to report use of music for emotional regulation and experience higher intensity of emotional affect especially negative emotion 11 12 Individual and situational influences on musical preferences editSituations have been shown to influence individual s preferences for certain types of music Participants in a study from 1996 provided information about what music they would prefer to listen to in given situations and indicated that the situation greatly determined their musical preferences For example melancholic situations called for sad and moody music while an arousal situation would call for loud strong rhythm invigorating music 29 Gender edit Women are more likely than men to respond to music in a more emotional way 5 Furthermore women prefer popular music more than men 23 In a study of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music researchers found that men demonstrated more of a preference for bass music than women This preference for bass music is also correlated with antisocial and borderline personalities 30 Culture edit Research has shown culture can influence musical preferences Humans tend to enjoy culturally similar music more than culturally atypical music 31 32 In a study conducted by McDermott and Schultz 2016 participants from three regions in Bolivia La Paz San Borja and Santa Maria and the United States were evaluated for differences in music preferences 31 The participants from the United States were divided into two groups with one group possessing at least two years of experience playing an instrument and the other group having a maximum of one year of experience Participants from Bolivia were divided into groups based on their region with each region having experienced varying levels of exposure to Western culture The group of participants from Santa Maria the Tsimane were of specific interest due to the substantial differences between their music and Western music in addition to their relative isolation from Western cultural influences 31 The participants were exposed to traditionally consonant and dissonant sounds from Western music and asked to rate how pleasant they found the sound Results indicated that Americans particularly trained musicians demonstrated the strongest preference for consonances in their music 31 The groups from La Paz and San Borja also demonstrated some preference for consonance However the Tsimane enjoyed both consonant and dissonant chords equally suggesting that consonance preferences in music are developed through cultural exposure and humans have no innate preferences for certain harmonies or chords 31 Culture changes how music is perceived by an individual and alters affective response to music particularly in a social context 32 People feel more connected with one another when listening to culturally typical music than when they listen to culturally atypical music Previous research has suggested that listening to familiar music might also aid in establishing more significant movement predictability which can result in more movement synchronicity between individuals further amplifying feelings of group connection and enjoyment of music 32 Culture also affects a person s ability to remember music 33 A study on cross cultural music memory was conducted by Demorest and Morrison 2016 In the study Western participants were played six musical excerpts Three of the excerpts were from Turkish classical music and the other three were from Western classical music The music was presented in three different musical contexts original monophonic and isochronous Once all of the excerpts were played participants received a memory test The memory test results indicate that participant memory was consistently better at recalling in cultural music than unfamiliar music 33 Age edit Age is a strong factor in determining music preference 9 34 There is also evidence that preferences and opinions toward music can change with age 9 35 In a Canadian study concerning how adolescent music preferences relate to personality researchers found that adolescents who preferred heavy music demonstrated low self esteem higher levels of discomfort within the family and tended to feel rejected by others Adolescents who preferred light music were preoccupied with doing the proper thing and had difficulty balancing independence with dependence Adolescents who had eclectic music preferences had less difficulty negotiating adolescence and were flexible using music according to mood and particular needs at the time 36 Self views edit Music preferences can also be influenced by how the individual wants to be perceived especially in males 11 Music preferences could be used to make self directed identity claims Individuals might select styles of music that reinforce their self views For examples individuals with a conservative self view preferred conventional styles of music while individuals with an athletic self view preferred vigorous music 14 Individuals will unconsciously push perceptions into their environments music makes this evident In a 1953 study Cattell and Anderson began the process of determining musical preference through unconscious traits 22 While their findings were inconclusive it created a research basis for later cases Music is a way similar to diet and physique to outwardly express internal characteristics Rentfrow and Gosling found through their study of the seven domains that for many music was placed quite high in the rankings 14 Mood edit Active mood is another factor that affects music preference Generally whether people are in a good or bad mood when they hear music affects how they feel about the type of music and also their emotional response 19 On that line of thinking aggression has been shown to improve creativity and emotional intensity derived from music People with aggressive disorders find music to be a powerful emotional outlet 37 Depression also impacts perceptions and emotional responses generated from music Depressed individuals rate their music associated episodic memories lower than non depressed individuals 38 Additionally the value people put on music and frequency of listening affects their reactions to it If people listen to a certain type of music and add emotional experience to songs or a genre in general this increases the likelihood of enjoying the music and being emotionally affected by it 20 This helps explain why many people might have strong reactions to music their parents listened to frequently when they were children Movement edit Research suggests that body movement may play a role in determining musical tastes 39 Body movement can create either positive or negative feelings towards music depending on if the movement is associated with a positive or negative affective state A study conducted by Weigelt and Walther 2011 demonstrated that performing a positive physical movement while listening to a song led to an increased preference rating for that song compared to a negative physical movement Movements associated with smiling muscles both positive or negative were found to have the largest effect on musical preference 39 Productiveness edit If someone is listening to music with the ultimate goal of completing a task their musical preference is greatly increased The more a genre of music increases one s productiveness the more the individual will gravitate toward that genre to complete future tasks 40 In turn music can increase focus in some It can help your brain interpret information and gain a better understanding of new things more easily Music can engage the brain in many different ways whether that be making one more attentive focused increased concentration etc 41 According to a 2017 study soft fast music was concluded to have a positive effect on productiveness Songs with lyrics are potentially more distracting than instrumental melodies and music with other modes or tempos could possibly evoke obstructive emotions for learning Music can improve one s mood create a positive mindset reduce stress etc this can directly correlate to learning improvements 42 Season of the year edit Season of the year can also affect preferences After reflecting upon fall or winter seasons participants preferred reflective and complex music whereas after reflecting upon summer or spring participants preferred energetic and rhythmic music However pop music seems to have a universal appeal despite the season 43 Familiarity edit Familiarity and complexity both have interesting effects on musical preferences As seen in other types of artistic media an inverted U relationship is apparent when relating subjective complexity on liking music excerpts Individuals like complexity to a certain degree then begin to dislike the music when complexity gets too high Furthermore there is a clear positive monotonic relationship between familiarity and liking of music 44 See also editBig Five personality traits Culture in music cognition Music psychologyReferences edit Schellenberg Glen E Mankarious Monika October 2012 Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood Emotion 12 5 887 891 doi 10 1037 a0027971 PMID 22642351 a b c Schafer Thomas Mehlhorn Claudia 29 April 2017 Can personality traits predict musical style preferences A meta analysis Personality and Individual Differences 116 265 273 doi 10 1016 j paid 2017 04 061 a b c Brown R A 1 August 2012 Music preferences and personality among Japanese university students International Journal of Psychology 47 4 259 268 doi 10 1080 00207594 2011 631544 PMID 22248342 a b c d e f g h Langmeyer Alexandra Guglhor Rudan Angelika Tarnai Christian 1 January 2012 What Do Music Preferences Reveal About Personality Journal of Individual Differences 33 2 119 130 doi 10 1027 1614 0001 a000082 a b c d e f Chamorro Premuzic Tomas Goma i Freixanet Montserrat Furnham Adrian Muro Anna 1 January 2009 Personality self estimated intelligence and uses of music A Spanish replication and extension using structural equation modeling Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 3 3 149 155 doi 10 1037 a0015342 Herrera Lucia Soares Quadros Joao F Jr Lorenzo Oswaldo 2018 Music Preferences and Personality in Brazilians Frontiers in Psychology 9 1488 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2018 01488 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 6113570 PMID 30186197 a b c Anderson Ian Gil Santiago Gibson Clay Wolf Scott Shapiro Will Semerci Oguz Greenberg David M May 2021 Just the Way You Are Linking Music Listening on Spotify and Personality Social Psychological and Personality Science 12 4 561 572 doi 10 1177 1948550620923228 ISSN 1948 5506 S2CID 233194549 a b Brisson Romain Bianchi Renzo 2020 On the relevance of music genre based analysis in research on musical tastes Psychology of Music 48 6 777 794 doi 10 1177 0305735619828810 S2CID 151126547 a b c d e Bonneville Roussy Arielle Rentfrow Peter J Xu Man K Potter Jeff 2013 Music through the ages Trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105 4 703 717 doi 10 1037 a0033770 PMID 23895269 a b c Zweigenhaft Richard L 1 January 2008 A Do Re Mi Encore Journal of Individual Differences 29 1 45 55 doi 10 1027 1614 0001 29 1 45 a b c d e f g h i Chamorro Premuzic Tomas Fagan Patrick Furnham Adrian 1 January 2010 Personality and uses of music as predictors of preferences for music consensually classified as happy sad complex and social Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 4 4 205 213 doi 10 1037 a0019210 a b c d e f Chamorro Premuzic Tomas Swami Viren Furnham Adrian Maakip Ismail 1 January 2009 The Big Five Personality Traits and Uses of Music Journal of Individual Differences 30 1 20 27 doi 10 1027 1614 0001 30 1 20 a b c Kopacz Malgorzata 2005 Personality and music preferences The influence of personality traits on preferences regarding musical elements Journal of Music Therapy 42 3 216 239 doi 10 1093 jmt 42 3 216 PMID 16086606 a b c d e Rentfrow Peter J Gosling Samuel D 1 January 2003 The do re mi s of everyday life The structure and personality correlates of music preferences Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 6 1236 1256 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 84 6 1236 PMID 12793587 S2CID 16489081 a b c Ladinig Olivia Schellenberg E Glenn 1 January 2012 Liking unfamiliar music Effects of felt emotion and individual differences Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 6 2 146 154 doi 10 1037 a0024671 S2CID 56162148 Tekman Hasan Gurkan Hortacsu Nuran 1 October 2002 Music and social identity Stylistic identification as a response to musical style International Journal of Psychology 37 5 277 285 doi 10 1080 00207590244000043 a b c Vuoskoski Jonna K Thompson William F McIlwain Doris Eerola Tuomas February 2012 Who enjoys listening to sad music and why Music Perception 29 3 311 317 doi 10 1525 mp 2012 29 3 311 Miranda Dave Morizot Julien Gaudreau Patrick 27 March 2012 Personality Metatraits and Music Preferences in Adolescence A Pilot Study International Journal of Adolescence and Youth 15 4 289 301 doi 10 1080 02673843 2010 9748036 S2CID 145681242 a b Vuoskoski Jonna K Eerola Tuomas 13 July 2011 Measuring music induced emotion A comparison of emotion models personality biases and intensity of experiences Musicae Scientiae 15 2 159 173 doi 10 1177 1029864911403367 S2CID 144079608 a b c Nusbaum E C Silvia P J 7 October 2010 Shivers and Timbres Personality and the Experience of Chills From Music PDF Social Psychological and Personality Science 2 2 199 204 doi 10 1177 1948550610386810 S2CID 54188929 Dunn Peter G de Ruyter Boris Bouwhuis Don G 16 March 2011 Toward a better understanding of the relation between music preference listening behavior and personality Psychology of Music 40 4 411 428 doi 10 1177 0305735610388897 S2CID 54736250 a b Rentfrow Peter J Goldberg Lewis R Levitin Daniel J June 2011 The Structure of Musical Preferences A Five Factor Model Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100 6 1139 1157 doi 10 1037 a0022406 ISSN 0022 3514 PMC 3138530 PMID 21299309 a b Rawlings D Ciancarelli V 1 October 1997 Music Preference and the Five Factor Model of the NEO Personality Inventory Psychology of Music 25 2 120 132 doi 10 1177 0305735697252003 S2CID 145601419 Hunter Patrick G Schellenberg Glen E 20 October 2010 Interactive effects of personality and frequency of exposure on liking for music Personality and Individual Differences 50 2 175 179 doi 10 1016 j paid 2010 09 021 Djikic Maja August 2011 The effect of music and lyrics on personality Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 5 3 237 240 doi 10 1037 a0022313 a b Steele Anita Louise Young Sylvester Spring 2011 A descriptive study of Myers Briggs personality types of professional music educators and music therapists with comparisons to undergraduate majors Journal of Music Therapy 48 1 55 73 doi 10 1093 jmt 48 1 55 PMID 21866713 Avila Christina Furnham Adrian McClelland Alastair 9 November 2011 The influence of distracting familiar vocal music on cognitive performance of introverts and extraverts Psychology of Music 40 1 84 93 doi 10 1177 0305735611422672 S2CID 145340833 Ruth Nicolas Mullensiefen Daniel December 2020 Associations between music preferences and personality in female secondary school students Psychomusicology Music Mind and Brain 30 4 202 211 doi 10 1037 pmu0000267 S2CID 222113433 North Adrian Hargreaves David 1996 Situational influences on reported musical preference Psychomusicology 15 1 2 30 45 doi 10 1037 h0094081 McCown William Keiser Ross Mulhearn Shea Williamson David 1997 The role of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music Personality and Individual Differences 23 4 543 547 doi 10 1016 s0191 8869 97 00085 8 a b c d e McDermott Josh Schultz Alan July 13 2016 Indifference to dissonance in native Amazonians reveals cultural variation in music perception Nature 535 7613 547 550 Bibcode 2016Natur 535 547M doi 10 1038 nature18635 PMID 27409816 S2CID 205249764 a b c Stupacher Jan Wood Guilherme July 2018 Effects of cultural background and musical preference on affective social entrainment with music Proceedings of ICMPC15 ESCOM10 438 441 a b Demorest Steven M Morrison Steven J June 1 2016 The influence of contextual cues on cultural bias in music memory Music Perception 33 5 590 600 doi 10 1525 mp 2016 33 5 590 JSTOR 26417336 Barret Frederick S Grimm Kevin J Robins Richard W Wildschut Tim Constantine Sedikides Janata Petr June 2010 Music evoked nostalgia Affect memory and personality Emotion 10 3 390 403 doi 10 1037 a0019006 PMID 20515227 Crowther R Durkin K 1982 Sex and age related differences in the musical behavior interests and attitudes towards music of 232 secondary school students Educational Studies 8 2 131 139 doi 10 1080 0305569820080206 Schwartz Kelly Fouts Gregory 2003 Music preferences personality style and developmental issues of adolescents Journal of Youth and Adolescence 32 3 205 213 doi 10 1023 a 1022547520656 S2CID 41849910 Pool Jonathan Odell Miller Helen 2011 Aggression in music therapy and its role in creativity with reference to personality disorder The Arts in Psychotherapy 38 3 169 177 doi 10 1016 j aip 2011 04 003 Sakka Laura Juslin Patrik October 10 2017 Emotional reactions to music in depressed individuals Psychology of Music 46 6 862 880 doi 10 1177 0305735617730425 S2CID 148886910 a b Weigelt Oliver Walther Eva February 2011 Music is in the Muscle How Embodied Cognition May Influence Music Preferences Music Perception 28 3 297 306 doi 10 1525 mp 2011 28 3 297 Schafer Thomas 2016 03 17 The Goals and Effects of Music Listening and Their Relationship to the Strength of Music Preference PLOS ONE 11 3 e0151634 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1151634S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0151634 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4795651 PMID 26985998 Sridharan Devarajan Levitin Daniel J Chafe Chris H Berger Jonathan Menon Vinod 2007 08 02 Neural Dynamics of Event Segmentation in Music Converging Evidence for Dissociable Ventral and Dorsal Networks Neuron 55 3 521 532 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2007 07 003 ISSN 0896 6273 PMID 17678862 S2CID 6846417 Lehmann Janina A M Seufert Tina 2017 The Influence of Background Music on Learning in the Light of Different Theoretical Perspectives and the Role of Working Memory Capacity Frontiers in Psychology 8 1902 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2017 01902 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 5671572 PMID 29163283 Pettijohn Terry F Williams Greg M Carter Tiffany C 26 November 2010 Music for the Seasons Seasonal Music Preferences in College Students Current Psychology 29 4 328 345 doi 10 1007 s12144 010 9092 8 S2CID 56037002 North Adrian Hargreaves David 1995 Subjective complexity familiarity and liking for popular music Psychomusicology 14 1 2 77 93 doi 10 1037 h0094090 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Psychology of music preference amp oldid 1198449716, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.