fbpx
Wikipedia

Personality development

Personality development encompasses the dynamic construction and deconstruction of integrative characteristics that distinguish an individual in terms of interpersonal behavioral traits.[1] Personality development is ever-changing and subject to contextual factors and life-altering experiences. Personality development is also dimensional in description and subjective in nature.[2] That is, personality development can be seen as a continuum varying in degrees of intensity and change. It is subjective in nature because its conceptualization is rooted in social norms of expected behavior, self-expression, and personal growth.[3][4] The dominant viewpoint in personality psychology indicates that personality emerges early and continues to develop across one's lifespan.[5] Adult personality traits are believed to have a basis in infant temperament, meaning that individual differences in disposition and behavior appear early in life, potentially before language of conscious self-representation develop.[6] The Five Factor Model of personality maps onto the dimensions of childhood temperament.[7] This suggests that individual differences in levels of the corresponding personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) are present from young ages.[8]

Theories

The development of personality is supported and attempted to be explained by theories of personality.

Psychoanalytic Theory

The Psychoanalytic Theory of personality was developed by Sigmund Freud. This theory consists of three main ideas that make up personality, the id, the ego, and the superego. The three traits control their own sections of the psyche. Personality is developed by the three traits that make up the Psychoanalytic theory conflicting.

Trait Theory

The Trait Theory of personality is one of the main theories in the study of personality. According to this theory, traits make up personality. Traits can be described as patterns of behavior, thought, or emotion. Some commonly accepted trait theories are the Big Five personality traits and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Generally, strong correlations are seen in the levels of any given personality trait in an individual when they are retested several years later. Traits tend to become more stable after young adulthood, and changes in these traits often follow some noticeable trends with age. For example, the trait Honesty-Humility is typically seen to decrease during teenage years, then steadily rise as the individual ages. The trait conscientiousness is generally seen to increase with age, however, the level of the facet perfectionism stays fairly consistent. [9]

Social Cognitive Theory

The social cognitive theory of personality views personality development in terms of reciprocal interactionism, that is, a perspective that considers the relationship of person-society as an interactive system that defines and molds personal development. Personal interaction with other individuals, society, and nature create experiences in which self-identification is organized in relation to social environment.[10] In other words, personality traits are a function of complex cognitive strategies used to effectively maneuver through social situations. Furthermore, according to the social-cognitive perspective, cognitive processes are central to an individual's unique expression of personality traits and affective processes. Through cognitive mechanism and social competencies, individuals interpret contextual situations to derive beliefs that guide their thoughts and behaviors, thus developing an enduring pattern of personality traits.[11][10]

Evolutionary Theory

The evolutionary theory of personality development is primarily based on the evolutionary process of natural selection.[12] From the evolutionary perspective, evolution resulted in variations of the human mind. Natural selection refined these variations based on their beneficence to humans. Due to human complexity, many opposing personality traits proved to be beneficial in a variety of ways.[13] Primitive humans were collectivists due to tribe culture. The personalities of individuals within a tribe were very similar. The division of labor resulted in differentiation in personality traits in order to achieve a higher efficiency. Differentiation in personality traits increased functionality, therefore becoming adaptive through natural selection. Humans continued to develop personality and individuality through evolution.[14]

Lifespan Theory

Classic theories of personality include Freud's tripartite theory and post-Freudian theory (developmental stage theories and type theories) and indicate that most personality development occurs in childhood, stabilizing by the end of adolescence. Current lifespan perspectives that integrate theory and empirical findings dominate the research literature. The lifespan perspectives of personality are based on the plasticity principle, the principle that personality traits are open systems that can be influenced by the environment at any age.[5] Large-scale longitudinal studies have demonstrated that the most active period of personality development appears to be between the ages of 20–40.[5] Although personality grows increasingly consistent with age and typically plateaus near age 50, personality never reached a period of total stability.[15][16]

Humanistic Theory

Humanistic psychology emphasizes individual choices as voluntary actions that ultimately determine personal development. Individual personalities traits, although essential to the integrated self, are only parts that make up the whole of observable human experiences. Thus, personality development is articulated in terms of purposeful action geared towards experiencing mastery of free choice. Rather than compartmentalized elements of personality traits such as feelings, thoughts, or behavior, Humanistic psychology integrates these elements as functions of being in a greater encompassing system such as societies, cultures, or interpersonal relationships. Consequently, personality development is subjected to shifts in personal meaning and individual goals of achieving an ideal self.[17]

Influencing factors

Personality traits demonstrate moderate levels of continuity, smaller but still significant normative or mean-level changes, and individual differences in change, often late into the life course.[18] This pattern is influenced by genetic, environmental, transactional, and stochastic factors.[19]

Genetics

Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the heritability of personality traits ranges from .3-.6, with a mean of .5, indicating that 50% of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences.[20] In contrast, family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of .22.[21] A study conducted on German women using an IAT (implicit association test), shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion.[22] With the effects of genetic similarity removed, children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers;[23] yet, identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together. These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development, and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics.[20]

Environmental

The weakness of shared environmental effects in shaping personality surprised many psychologists, spurring research into non-shared environmental effects, the environmental influences that distinguish siblings from one another.[24] The non-shared environment may include differential treatment by parents, individually-distinct reactions to the shared family environment, peer influences, experiences outside the family, and test error in measurement. In adults, the non-shared environment may also include the unique roles and environments experienced after leaving the family of origin.[25] Further effects of environment in adulthood are demonstrated by research suggesting that different work, marital, and family experiences are associated with personality change;[26] these effects are supported by research involving the impact of major positive and negative life events on personality.[27][28]

Gene-environment interactions

A culmination of research suggests that the development of personality occurs in relation to one's genetics, one's environment, and the interaction between one's genetics and environment.[29][30] Van Gestel and Van Broeckhoven (2003) write, “Almost by definition, complex traits originate from interplay between (multiple) genetic factors and environment.”[31] The corresponsive principle of personality development states that “life experiences may accentuate and reinforce the personality characteristics that were partially responsible for the particular environmental elicitations in the first place”.[32] This principle illustrates how gene-environment interactions maintain and reinforce personality throughout the lifespan. Three main types of gene-environment interactions are active (the process by which individuals with certain genotypes select and create environments that facilitate the expression of those genotypes), passive (the process by which genetic parents provide both the genes and the early environmental influences that contribute to the development of a characteristic in their children), and reactive (the process by which non-family individuals respond to the behavior produced by a genotype in characteristic ways).[20][27]

An example of the way environment can moderate the expression of a gene is the finding by Heath, Eaves, and Martin (1998) that marriage was a protective factor against depression in identical twins, such that the heritability of depression was as low as 29% in a married twin and as high as 51% in an unmarried twin.[33]

Stability of personality

Over the course of an individual's lifespan, the stability of their personality has been shown to be variable, although this variability levels out in adulthood. Behavioral genetics can account for the variability experienced across the lifespan. This is highly evident in the transitions between childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. From childhood to mid-adolescence, the rate of individual differences in personality increases, primarily due to environmental influences. However, genetic influences play a larger role than environmental influences in adulthood, resulting in fewer individual differences in personality between individuals who share similar genetics.[34] The stability of personality across one's lifespan is further evidenced by a longitudinal study conducted on individuals across the span of fifty years from adolescence through adulthood. The results of this longitudinal study suggested that the personality was malleable, although variations in the level of malleability stabilized in adulthood.[35]

A study published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology examined personality developing in college students based on the Big Five personality trait domains and facets within those domains. The results suggested that the rank-order stabilities of facets were high, with values greater than .50 (indicating a strong correlation); the results for trait domains were similar to individual facets.[36] High rank-order stability is further evidenced by another study that integrated personality structure, process, and development. This study included previous research that indicated high-order rank stability; it also included research that indicated variation in this stability across periods of the lifespan, such as adolescence and adulthood.[37] The stability and variation of personality is explained by a complex interaction between one's genetics and one's environment.[34]

References

  1. ^ Wrzus, Cornelia (2021), "Processes of personality development: An update of the TESSERA framework", The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes, Elsevier, pp. 101–123, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-813995-0.00005-4, ISBN 9780128139950, S2CID 234237414, retrieved 2022-01-21
  2. ^ Hang, Yuzhan; Soto, Christopher J.; Lee, Billy; Mõttus, René (2021-04-02). "Social Expectations and Abilities to Meet Them as Possible Mechanisms of Youth Personality Development". dx.doi.org. doi:10.31234/osf.io/8yp6x. S2CID 236660320. Retrieved 2022-01-21.
  3. ^ BILOHUR, V. (2019). Formation and Development of Personality Established by Change and Growth. Humanities Bulletin of Zaporizhzhe State Engineering Academy, 76, 77–89.
  4. ^ Caspi A, Roberts BW (2001). "Personality development across the life course: The argument for change and continuity". Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 49–66. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1202_01. S2CID 144947217.
  5. ^ a b c Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2010). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervi (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 375-398). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  6. ^ Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). "Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122–135. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID 10653510.
  7. ^ Putnam SP, Ellis LK, Rothbart MK (2001). Eliasz A, Angleitner A (eds.). "The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence". Advances in Research on Temperament. Germany: Pabst Science: 165–182.
  8. ^ Deal JE, Halverson CF, Havill V, Martin R (2005). "Temperament factors as longitudinal predictors of young adult personality". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 51 (3): 315–334. doi:10.1353/mpq.2005.0015. S2CID 143953664.
  9. ^ Ashton, Michael (2018). Individual Differences and Personality. 125 London Wall, London EC2Y 5AS, United Kingdom: Elsevier. pp. 91–106. ISBN 978-0-12-809845-5.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  10. ^ a b Cervone, Daniel; Shadel, William G.; Jencius, Simon (February 2001). "Social-Cognitive Theory of Personality Assessment". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 5 (1): 33–51. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_3. ISSN 1088-8683. S2CID 16223943.
  11. ^ "The Social Cognitive Theory". sphweb.bumc.bu.edu. Retrieved 2020-04-30.
  12. ^ Hettema PJ, Deary IJ (11 November 2013). Foundations of Personality. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 7. ISBN 978-94-011-1660-2.
  13. ^ Penke L, Denissen JJ, Miller GF (August 2007). "The Evolution of Personality Variation in Humans and Other Animals" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 21 (5): 549–587. doi:10.1002/per.629. ISSN 0890-2070.
  14. ^ Bell, M. G. (2010, January 1). Consciousness: The Evolution Of The Self And Personal Individuality. Retrieved November 1, 2014, from www.agenthuman.com/product/evolution_self_personal_individuality.html#selfevol
  15. ^ Roberts BW, Mroczek D (February 2008). "Personality Trait Change in Adulthood". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (1): 31–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. PMC 2743415. PMID 19756219.
  16. ^ Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF (January 2000). "The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID 10668348.
  17. ^ Taylor, Eugene (July 2000). ""What is Man, Psychologist, that Thou Art So Unmindful of him?": Henry A. Murray on the Historical Relation between Classical Personality Theory and Humanistic Psychology". Journal of Humanistic Psychology. 40 (3): 29–42. doi:10.1177/0022167800403003. ISSN 0022-1678. S2CID 143696226.
  18. ^ Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt T (2001). "The kids are alright: Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (4): 670–683. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.670. PMID 11642353.
  19. ^ Fraley RC, Roberts BW (January 2005). "Patterns of continuity: a dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course". Psychological Review. 112 (1): 60–74. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.60. PMID 15631588.
  20. ^ a b c Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO (1997). "Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits". Psychological Assessment. 9 (4): 349–360. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349. S2CID 197657764.
  21. ^ Vukasović, Tena; Bratko, Denis (2015). "Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies". Psychological Bulletin. 141 (4): 769–785. doi:10.1037/bul0000017. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 25961374.
  22. ^ Osinsky, Roman; Alexander, Nina; Schmitz, Anja; Kuepper, Yvonne; Müller, Eva; Beer, Laura; Koeppe, Lena; Hennig, Jürgen (January 2010). "Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality". Journal of Individual Differences. 31 (3): 115–123. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000007. ISSN 1614-0001.
  23. ^ Turkheimer, E. & Waldron, M. (2000). Non shared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review.
  24. ^ Plomin R, Daniels D (June 2011). "Why are children in the same family so different from one another?". International Journal of Epidemiology. 40 (3): 563–82. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00055941. PMC 3147063. PMID 21807642.
  25. ^ Plomin R, Asbury K, Dunn J (April 2001). "Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later". Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 46 (3): 225–33. doi:10.1177/070674370104600302. PMID 11320676.
  26. ^ Roberts BW, Wood D, Smith JL (2005). "Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development". Journal of Research in Personality. 39: 166–184. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.08.002.
  27. ^ a b Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). "Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  28. ^ Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535. S2CID 43717734.
  29. ^ Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. hdl:20.500.11820/7cbc3cf8-c406-4216-a805-a0b131b143dd. ISSN 1099-0984. S2CID 148637952.
  30. ^ South SC, Krueger RF (2008). "An interactionist perspective on genetic and environmental contributions to personality". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2 (2): 929–948. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00062.x.
  31. ^ Van Gestel S, Van Broeckhoven C (October 2003). "Genetics of personality: are we making progress?". Molecular Psychiatry. 8 (10): 840–52. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001367. PMID 14515135.
  32. ^ Donnellan, M. B. & Robins, R. W. (2009). The development of personality across the lifespan. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 191-204). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  33. ^ Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Martin NG (September 1998). "Interaction of marital status and genetic risk for symptoms of depression". Twin Research. 1 (3): 119–22. doi:10.1375/136905298320566249. PMID 10051335.
  34. ^ a b Mõttus R, Briley DA, Zheng A, Mann FD, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, et al. (September 2019). "Kids becoming less alike: A behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 635–658. doi:10.1037/pspp0000194. PMC 6687565. PMID 30920282.
  35. ^ Damian, Rodica Ioana; Spengler, Marion; Sutu, Andreea; Roberts, Brent W. (September 2019). "Sixteen going on sixty-six: A longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 674–695. doi:10.1037/pspp0000210. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30113194. S2CID 52013579.
  36. ^ Klimstra, Theo A.; Noftle, Erik E.; Luyckx, Koen; Goossens, Luc; Robins, Richard W. (August 2018). "Personality development and adjustment in college: A multifaceted, cross-national view" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 115 (2): 338–361. doi:10.1037/pspp0000205. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30024246. S2CID 51699611.
  37. ^ Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. hdl:20.500.11820/7cbc3cf8-c406-4216-a805-a0b131b143dd. ISSN 1099-0984. S2CID 148637952.

personality, development, confused, with, personal, development, encompasses, dynamic, construction, deconstruction, integrative, characteristics, that, distinguish, individual, terms, interpersonal, behavioral, traits, ever, changing, subject, contextual, fac. Not to be confused with personal development Personality development encompasses the dynamic construction and deconstruction of integrative characteristics that distinguish an individual in terms of interpersonal behavioral traits 1 Personality development is ever changing and subject to contextual factors and life altering experiences Personality development is also dimensional in description and subjective in nature 2 That is personality development can be seen as a continuum varying in degrees of intensity and change It is subjective in nature because its conceptualization is rooted in social norms of expected behavior self expression and personal growth 3 4 The dominant viewpoint in personality psychology indicates that personality emerges early and continues to develop across one s lifespan 5 Adult personality traits are believed to have a basis in infant temperament meaning that individual differences in disposition and behavior appear early in life potentially before language of conscious self representation develop 6 The Five Factor Model of personality maps onto the dimensions of childhood temperament 7 This suggests that individual differences in levels of the corresponding personality traits neuroticism extraversion openness to experience agreeableness and conscientiousness are present from young ages 8 Contents 1 Theories 1 1 Psychoanalytic Theory 1 2 Trait Theory 1 3 Social Cognitive Theory 1 4 Evolutionary Theory 1 5 Lifespan Theory 1 6 Humanistic Theory 2 Influencing factors 2 1 Genetics 2 2 Environmental 2 3 Gene environment interactions 3 Stability of personality 4 ReferencesTheories EditThe development of personality is supported and attempted to be explained by theories of personality Psychoanalytic Theory Edit The Psychoanalytic Theory of personality was developed by Sigmund Freud This theory consists of three main ideas that make up personality the id the ego and the superego The three traits control their own sections of the psyche Personality is developed by the three traits that make up the Psychoanalytic theory conflicting Trait Theory Edit The Trait Theory of personality is one of the main theories in the study of personality According to this theory traits make up personality Traits can be described as patterns of behavior thought or emotion Some commonly accepted trait theories are the Big Five personality traits and the HEXACO model of personality structure Generally strong correlations are seen in the levels of any given personality trait in an individual when they are retested several years later Traits tend to become more stable after young adulthood and changes in these traits often follow some noticeable trends with age For example the trait Honesty Humility is typically seen to decrease during teenage years then steadily rise as the individual ages The trait conscientiousness is generally seen to increase with age however the level of the facet perfectionism stays fairly consistent 9 Social Cognitive Theory Edit The social cognitive theory of personality views personality development in terms of reciprocal interactionism that is a perspective that considers the relationship of person society as an interactive system that defines and molds personal development Personal interaction with other individuals society and nature create experiences in which self identification is organized in relation to social environment 10 In other words personality traits are a function of complex cognitive strategies used to effectively maneuver through social situations Furthermore according to the social cognitive perspective cognitive processes are central to an individual s unique expression of personality traits and affective processes Through cognitive mechanism and social competencies individuals interpret contextual situations to derive beliefs that guide their thoughts and behaviors thus developing an enduring pattern of personality traits 11 10 Evolutionary Theory Edit The evolutionary theory of personality development is primarily based on the evolutionary process of natural selection 12 From the evolutionary perspective evolution resulted in variations of the human mind Natural selection refined these variations based on their beneficence to humans Due to human complexity many opposing personality traits proved to be beneficial in a variety of ways 13 Primitive humans were collectivists due to tribe culture The personalities of individuals within a tribe were very similar The division of labor resulted in differentiation in personality traits in order to achieve a higher efficiency Differentiation in personality traits increased functionality therefore becoming adaptive through natural selection Humans continued to develop personality and individuality through evolution 14 Lifespan Theory Edit Classic theories of personality include Freud s tripartite theory and post Freudian theory developmental stage theories and type theories and indicate that most personality development occurs in childhood stabilizing by the end of adolescence Current lifespan perspectives that integrate theory and empirical findings dominate the research literature The lifespan perspectives of personality are based on the plasticity principle the principle that personality traits are open systems that can be influenced by the environment at any age 5 Large scale longitudinal studies have demonstrated that the most active period of personality development appears to be between the ages of 20 40 5 Although personality grows increasingly consistent with age and typically plateaus near age 50 personality never reached a period of total stability 15 16 Humanistic Theory Edit Humanistic psychology emphasizes individual choices as voluntary actions that ultimately determine personal development Individual personalities traits although essential to the integrated self are only parts that make up the whole of observable human experiences Thus personality development is articulated in terms of purposeful action geared towards experiencing mastery of free choice Rather than compartmentalized elements of personality traits such as feelings thoughts or behavior Humanistic psychology integrates these elements as functions of being in a greater encompassing system such as societies cultures or interpersonal relationships Consequently personality development is subjected to shifts in personal meaning and individual goals of achieving an ideal self 17 Influencing factors EditPersonality traits demonstrate moderate levels of continuity smaller but still significant normative or mean level changes and individual differences in change often late into the life course 18 This pattern is influenced by genetic environmental transactional and stochastic factors 19 Genetics Edit Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the heritability of personality traits ranges from 3 6 with a mean of 5 indicating that 50 of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences 20 In contrast family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of 22 21 A study conducted on German women using an IAT implicit association test shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion 22 With the effects of genetic similarity removed children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers 23 yet identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics 20 Environmental Edit The weakness of shared environmental effects in shaping personality surprised many psychologists spurring research into non shared environmental effects the environmental influences that distinguish siblings from one another 24 The non shared environment may include differential treatment by parents individually distinct reactions to the shared family environment peer influences experiences outside the family and test error in measurement In adults the non shared environment may also include the unique roles and environments experienced after leaving the family of origin 25 Further effects of environment in adulthood are demonstrated by research suggesting that different work marital and family experiences are associated with personality change 26 these effects are supported by research involving the impact of major positive and negative life events on personality 27 28 Gene environment interactions Edit A culmination of research suggests that the development of personality occurs in relation to one s genetics one s environment and the interaction between one s genetics and environment 29 30 Van Gestel and Van Broeckhoven 2003 write Almost by definition complex traits originate from interplay between multiple genetic factors and environment 31 The corresponsive principle of personality development states that life experiences may accentuate and reinforce the personality characteristics that were partially responsible for the particular environmental elicitations in the first place 32 This principle illustrates how gene environment interactions maintain and reinforce personality throughout the lifespan Three main types of gene environment interactions are active the process by which individuals with certain genotypes select and create environments that facilitate the expression of those genotypes passive the process by which genetic parents provide both the genes and the early environmental influences that contribute to the development of a characteristic in their children and reactive the process by which non family individuals respond to the behavior produced by a genotype in characteristic ways 20 27 An example of the way environment can moderate the expression of a gene is the finding by Heath Eaves and Martin 1998 that marriage was a protective factor against depression in identical twins such that the heritability of depression was as low as 29 in a married twin and as high as 51 in an unmarried twin 33 Stability of personality EditOver the course of an individual s lifespan the stability of their personality has been shown to be variable although this variability levels out in adulthood Behavioral genetics can account for the variability experienced across the lifespan This is highly evident in the transitions between childhood adolescence and adulthood From childhood to mid adolescence the rate of individual differences in personality increases primarily due to environmental influences However genetic influences play a larger role than environmental influences in adulthood resulting in fewer individual differences in personality between individuals who share similar genetics 34 The stability of personality across one s lifespan is further evidenced by a longitudinal study conducted on individuals across the span of fifty years from adolescence through adulthood The results of this longitudinal study suggested that the personality was malleable although variations in the level of malleability stabilized in adulthood 35 A study published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology examined personality developing in college students based on the Big Five personality trait domains and facets within those domains The results suggested that the rank order stabilities of facets were high with values greater than 50 indicating a strong correlation the results for trait domains were similar to individual facets 36 High rank order stability is further evidenced by another study that integrated personality structure process and development This study included previous research that indicated high order rank stability it also included research that indicated variation in this stability across periods of the lifespan such as adolescence and adulthood 37 The stability and variation of personality is explained by a complex interaction between one s genetics and one s environment 34 References Edit Wrzus Cornelia 2021 Processes of personality development An update of the TESSERA framework The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes Elsevier pp 101 123 doi 10 1016 b978 0 12 813995 0 00005 4 ISBN 9780128139950 S2CID 234237414 retrieved 2022 01 21 Hang Yuzhan Soto Christopher J Lee Billy Mottus Rene 2021 04 02 Social Expectations and Abilities to Meet Them as Possible Mechanisms of Youth Personality Development dx doi org doi 10 31234 osf io 8yp6x S2CID 236660320 Retrieved 2022 01 21 BILOHUR V 2019 Formation and Development of Personality Established by Change and Growth Humanities Bulletin of Zaporizhzhe State Engineering Academy 76 77 89 Caspi A Roberts BW 2001 Personality development across the life course The argument for change and continuity Psychological Inquiry 12 2 49 66 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli1202 01 S2CID 144947217 a b c Roberts B W Wood D amp Caspi A 2010 The development of personality traits in adulthood In O P John R W Robins amp L A Pervi Eds Handbook of personality Theory and research 3rd ed pp 375 398 New York NY Guilford Press Rothbart MK Ahadi SA Evans DE 2000 Temperament and personality Origins and outcomes Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 1 122 135 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 1 122 PMID 10653510 Putnam SP Ellis LK Rothbart MK 2001 Eliasz A Angleitner A eds The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence Advances in Research on Temperament Germany Pabst Science 165 182 Deal JE Halverson CF Havill V Martin R 2005 Temperament factors as longitudinal predictors of young adult personality Merrill Palmer Quarterly 51 3 315 334 doi 10 1353 mpq 2005 0015 S2CID 143953664 Ashton Michael 2018 Individual Differences and Personality 125 London Wall London EC2Y 5AS United Kingdom Elsevier pp 91 106 ISBN 978 0 12 809845 5 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location link a b Cervone Daniel Shadel William G Jencius Simon February 2001 Social Cognitive Theory of Personality Assessment Personality and Social Psychology Review 5 1 33 51 doi 10 1207 S15327957PSPR0501 3 ISSN 1088 8683 S2CID 16223943 The Social Cognitive Theory sphweb bumc bu edu Retrieved 2020 04 30 Hettema PJ Deary IJ 11 November 2013 Foundations of Personality Springer Science amp Business Media p 7 ISBN 978 94 011 1660 2 Penke L Denissen JJ Miller GF August 2007 The Evolution of Personality Variation in Humans and Other Animals PDF European Journal of Personality 21 5 549 587 doi 10 1002 per 629 ISSN 0890 2070 Bell M G 2010 January 1 Consciousness The Evolution Of The Self And Personal Individuality Retrieved November 1 2014 from www agenthuman com product evolution self personal individuality html selfevol Roberts BW Mroczek D February 2008 Personality Trait Change in Adulthood Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 1 31 35 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2008 00543 x PMC 2743415 PMID 19756219 Roberts BW DelVecchio WF January 2000 The rank order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age a quantitative review of longitudinal studies Psychological Bulletin 126 1 3 25 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 126 1 3 PMID 10668348 Taylor Eugene July 2000 What is Man Psychologist that Thou Art So Unmindful of him Henry A Murray on the Historical Relation between Classical Personality Theory and Humanistic Psychology Journal of Humanistic Psychology 40 3 29 42 doi 10 1177 0022167800403003 ISSN 0022 1678 S2CID 143696226 Roberts BW Caspi A Moffitt T 2001 The kids are alright Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 4 670 683 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 4 670 PMID 11642353 Fraley RC Roberts BW January 2005 Patterns of continuity a dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course Psychological Review 112 1 60 74 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 112 1 60 PMID 15631588 a b c Harkness AR Lilienfeld SO 1997 Individual differences science for treatment planning Personality traits Psychological Assessment 9 4 349 360 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 9 4 349 S2CID 197657764 Vukasovic Tena Bratko Denis 2015 Heritability of personality A meta analysis of behavior genetic studies Psychological Bulletin 141 4 769 785 doi 10 1037 bul0000017 ISSN 1939 1455 PMID 25961374 Osinsky Roman Alexander Nina Schmitz Anja Kuepper Yvonne Muller Eva Beer Laura Koeppe Lena Hennig Jurgen January 2010 Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality Journal of Individual Differences 31 3 115 123 doi 10 1027 1614 0001 a000007 ISSN 1614 0001 Turkheimer E amp Waldron M 2000 Non shared environment A theoretical methodological and quantitative review Plomin R Daniels D June 2011 Why are children in the same family so different from one another International Journal of Epidemiology 40 3 563 82 doi 10 1017 s0140525x00055941 PMC 3147063 PMID 21807642 Plomin R Asbury K Dunn J April 2001 Why are children in the same family so different Nonshared environment a decade later Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 46 3 225 33 doi 10 1177 070674370104600302 PMID 11320676 Roberts BW Wood D Smith JL 2005 Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development Journal of Research in Personality 39 166 184 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2004 08 002 a b Jeronimus BF Riese H Sanderman R Ormel J October 2014 Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences a five wave 16 year study to test reciprocal causation Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107 4 751 64 doi 10 1037 a0037009 PMID 25111305 Jeronimus BF Ormel J Aleman A Penninx BW Riese H November 2013 Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism Psychological Medicine 43 11 2403 15 doi 10 1017 s0033291713000159 PMID 23410535 S2CID 43717734 Baumert Anna Schmitt Manfred Perugini Marco Johnson Wendy Blum Gabriela Borkenau Peter Costantini Giulio Denissen Jaap J A Fleeson William Grafton Ben Jayawickreme Eranda 2017 Integrating Personality Structure Personality Process and Personality Development PDF European Journal of Personality 31 5 503 528 doi 10 1002 per 2115 hdl 20 500 11820 7cbc3cf8 c406 4216 a805 a0b131b143dd ISSN 1099 0984 S2CID 148637952 South SC Krueger RF 2008 An interactionist perspective on genetic and environmental contributions to personality Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 2 929 948 doi 10 1111 j 1751 9004 2007 00062 x Van Gestel S Van Broeckhoven C October 2003 Genetics of personality are we making progress Molecular Psychiatry 8 10 840 52 doi 10 1038 sj mp 4001367 PMID 14515135 Donnellan M B amp Robins R W 2009 The development of personality across the lifespan In P J Corr amp G Matthews Eds The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology pp 191 204 New York NY Cambridge University Press Heath AC Eaves LJ Martin NG September 1998 Interaction of marital status and genetic risk for symptoms of depression Twin Research 1 3 119 22 doi 10 1375 136905298320566249 PMID 10051335 a b Mottus R Briley DA Zheng A Mann FD Engelhardt LE Tackett JL et al September 2019 Kids becoming less alike A behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 117 3 635 658 doi 10 1037 pspp0000194 PMC 6687565 PMID 30920282 Damian Rodica Ioana Spengler Marion Sutu Andreea Roberts Brent W September 2019 Sixteen going on sixty six A longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 117 3 674 695 doi 10 1037 pspp0000210 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 30113194 S2CID 52013579 Klimstra Theo A Noftle Erik E Luyckx Koen Goossens Luc Robins Richard W August 2018 Personality development and adjustment in college A multifaceted cross national view PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 115 2 338 361 doi 10 1037 pspp0000205 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 30024246 S2CID 51699611 Baumert Anna Schmitt Manfred Perugini Marco Johnson Wendy Blum Gabriela Borkenau Peter Costantini Giulio Denissen Jaap J A Fleeson William Grafton Ben Jayawickreme Eranda 2017 Integrating Personality Structure Personality Process and Personality Development PDF European Journal of Personality 31 5 503 528 doi 10 1002 per 2115 hdl 20 500 11820 7cbc3cf8 c406 4216 a805 a0b131b143dd ISSN 1099 0984 S2CID 148637952 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Personality development amp oldid 1147852193, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.