fbpx
Wikipedia

Motion camouflage

Motion camouflage is camouflage which provides a degree of concealment for a moving object, given that motion makes objects easy to detect however well their coloration matches their background or breaks up their outlines.

Principle of motion camouflage by mimicking the optic flow of the background. An attacker flies towards a target, choosing its path so that it remains on a line between target and a real point behind the attacker; this path differs from classical pursuit, and is often shorter (as illustrated here). The attacker looms larger as it closes on target, but does not otherwise appear to move.
Animals such as frogs are very good at detecting motion,[1] making motion camouflage a priority for predators.

The principal form of motion camouflage, and the type generally meant by the term, involves an attacker's mimicking the optic flow of the background as seen by its target. This enables the attacker to approach the target while appearing to remain stationary from the target's perspective, unlike in classical pursuit (where the attacker moves straight towards the target at all times, and often appears to the target to move sideways). The attacker chooses its flight path so as to remain on the line between the target and some landmark point. The target therefore does not see the attacker move from the landmark point. The only visible evidence that the attacker is moving is its looming, the change in size as the attacker approaches.

Camouflage is sometimes facilitated by motion, as in the leafy sea dragon and some stick insects. These animals complement their passive camouflage by swaying like plants in the wind or ocean currents, delaying their recognition by predators.

First discovered in hoverflies in 1995, motion camouflage by minimising optic flow has been demonstrated in another insect order, dragonflies, as well as in two groups of vertebrates, falcons and echolocating bats. Since bats hunting at night cannot be using the strategy for camouflage, it has been named, describing its mechanism, as constant absolute target direction. This is an efficient homing strategy, and it has been suggested that anti-aircraft missiles could benefit from similar techniques.

Camouflage of approach motion edit

Many animals are highly sensitive to motion; for example, frogs readily detect small moving dark spots but ignore stationary ones.[1] Therefore, motion signals can be used to defeat camouflage.[2] Moving objects with disruptive camouflage patterns remain harder to identify than uncamouflaged objects, especially if other similar objects are nearby, even though they are detected, so motion does not completely 'break' camouflage.[3] All the same, the conspicuousness of motion raises the question of whether and how motion itself could be camouflaged. Several mechanisms are possible.[2]

 
Predators such as tigers stalk prey very slowly, to minimise motion cues.

Stealthy movements edit

One strategy is to minimise actual motion, as when predators such as tigers stalk prey by moving very slowly and stealthily. This strategy effectively avoids the need to camouflage motion.[2][4]

Minimising motion signal edit

When movement is required, one strategy is to minimise the motion signal, for example by avoiding waving limbs about and by choosing patterns that do not cause flicker when seen by the prey from straight ahead.[2] Cuttlefish may be doing this with their active camouflage by choosing to form stripes at right angles to their front-back axis, minimising motion signals that would be given by occluding and displaying the pattern as they swim.[5]

Disrupting perception of motion edit

Disrupting the attacker's perception of the target's motion was the main intended purpose of dazzle camouflage as used on ships in the First World War, though its effectiveness is disputed. This type of dazzle does not appear to be used by animals.[2]

Mimicking optic flow of background edit

 
The Australian emperor dragonfly mimics the optic flow of its background using real-point motion camouflage to enable it to approach rivals.

Some animals mimic the optic flow of the background, so that the attacker does not appear to move when seen by the target. This is the main focus of work on motion camouflage, and is often treated as synonymous with it.[2][6]

Pursuit strategies edit

An attacker can mimic the background's optic flow by choosing its flight path so as to remain on the line between the target and either some real landmark point, or a point at infinite distance (giving different pursuit algorithms). It therefore does not move from the landmark point as seen by the target, though it inevitably looms larger as it approaches. This is not the same as moving straight towards the target (classical pursuit): that results in visible sideways motion with a readily detectable difference in optic flow from the background. The strategy works whether the background is plain or textured.[6]

This motion camouflage strategy was discovered and modelled as algorithms in 1995 by M. V. Srinivasan and M. Davey while they were studying mating behaviour in hoverflies. The male hoverfly appeared to be using the tracking technique to approach prospective mates.[6] Motion camouflage has been observed in high-speed territorial battles between dragonflies, where males of the Australian emperor dragonfly, Hemianax papuensis were seen to choose their flight paths to appear stationary to their rivals in 6 of 15 encounters. They made use of both real-point and infinity-point strategies.[7][8]

 
Falcons use infinite-point motion camouflage to close on their prey.

The strategy appears to work equally well in insects and in vertebrates. Simulations show that motion camouflage results in a more efficient pursuit path than classical pursuit (i.e. the motion camouflage path is shorter), whether the target flies in a straight line or chooses a chaotic path. Further, where classical pursuit requires the attacker to fly faster than the target, the motion camouflaged attacker can sometimes capture the target despite flying more slowly than it.[9][2]

In sailing, it has long been known that if the bearing from the target to the pursuer remains constant, known as constant bearing, decreasing range (CBDR), equivalent to taking a fixed reference point at infinite distance, the two vessels are on a collision course, both travelling in straight lines. In a simulation, this is readily observed as the lines between the two remain parallel at all times.[9][2]

 
Insect-hunting bats and some missiles follow an infinity-point pursuit path keeping parallel to the target ("Parallel navigation"), for its efficiency rather than for camouflage.

Echolocating bats follow an infinity-point[2] path when hunting insects in the dark. This is not for camouflage but for the efficiency of the resulting path, so the strategy is generally called constant absolute target direction (CATD);[10][11][12] it is equivalent to CBDR but allowing for the target to manoeuvre erratically.[13]

A 2014 study of falcons of different species (gyrfalcon, saker falcon, and peregrine falcon) used video cameras mounted on their heads or backs to track their approaches to prey. Comparison of the observed paths with simulations of different pursuit strategies showed that these predatory birds used a motion camouflage path consistent with CATD.[13]

The missile guidance strategy of pure proportional navigation guidance (PPNG) closely resembles the CATD strategy used by bats.[14] The biologists Andrew Anderson and Peter McOwan have suggested that anti-aircraft missiles could exploit motion camouflage to reduce their chances of being detected. They tested their ideas on people playing a computerised war game.[15] The steering laws to achieve motion camouflage have been analysed mathematically. The resulting paths turn out to be extremely efficient, often better than classical pursuit. Motion camouflage pursuit may therefore be adopted both by predators and missile engineers (as "parallel navigation", for an infinity-point algorithm) for its performance advantages.[16][17]

Attack strategies[13]
Strategy Description Camouflage effect Used by species
Classical pursuit (pursuit guidance) Move straight towards current position of target at all times (simplest strategy) None, target sees pursuer moving against background Honey bees, flies, tiger beetles[13]
Real-point motion camouflage Move towards target keeping between it and a point near pursuer's start at all times Pursuer remains stationary against background (but looms larger) Dragonflies, hoverflies[13]
Infinity-point motion camouflage
(CATD, "Parallel navigation")
Move towards target keeping line to target parallel to line between pursuer's start and target at start Pursuer remains at a constant direction in the sky Dogs, humans, hoverflies, teleost fish, bats, falcons[13]

Camouflage by motion edit

 
Cryptic stick insect Extatosoma tiaratum sways in the wind like foliage.
Preying mantises exhibiting motion camouflage

Swaying: motion crypsis or masquerade edit

Swaying behaviour is practised by highly cryptic animals such as the leafy sea dragon, the stick insect Extatosoma tiaratum, and mantises. These animals resemble vegetation with their coloration, strikingly disruptive body outlines with leaflike appendages, and the ability to sway effectively like the plants that they mimic. E. tiaratum actively sways back and forth or side to side when disturbed or when there is a gust of wind, with a frequency distribution like foliage rustling in the wind. This behaviour may represent motion crypsis, preventing detection by predators, or motion masquerade, promoting misclassification (as something other than prey), or a combination of the two, and has accordingly also been described as a form of motion camouflage.[18][19]

References edit

  1. ^ a b Anstis, Stuart (1 June 2015). "Seeing Isn't Believing". The Scientist. Retrieved 28 April 2017.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i Troscianko, T. (2011). Stevens, Martin; Merilaita, Sami (eds.). Animal Camouflage: Mechanisms and Function. Cambridge University Press. pp. 128–130. ISBN 978-1-139-49623-0.
  3. ^ Hall, Joanna R.; Cuthill, Innes C.; Baddeley, Roland; Shohet, Adam J.; Scott-Samuel, Nicholas E. (7 May 2013). "Camouflage, detection and identification of moving targets". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 280 (1578): 20130064. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0064. PMC 3619462. PMID 23486439.
  4. ^ Macdonald, David; Loveridge, Andrew (2010). The Biology and Conservation of Wild Felids. Oxford University Press. pp. 84–. ISBN 978-0-19-923444-8.
  5. ^ Shohet, A.J.; Baddeley, R.J.; Anderson, J.C.; Kelman, E.J.; Osorio, D. (2006). "Cuttlefish responses to visual orientation of substrates, water flow and a model of motion camouflage" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 209 (23): 4717–4723. doi:10.1242/jeb.02580. PMID 17114404. S2CID 2485460.
  6. ^ a b c Srinivasan, M. V. & Davey, M. (1995). "Strategies for active camouflage of motion". Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 259 (1354): 19–25. Bibcode:1995RSPSB.259...19S. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0004. S2CID 131341953.
  7. ^ Hopkin, Michael (June 5, 2003). "Nature News". Dragonfly Flight Tricks the Eye. Nature.com. doi:10.1038/news030602-10. Retrieved January 16, 2012.
  8. ^ Mizutani, A. K.; Chahl, J. S.; Srinivasan, M. V. (June 5, 2003). "Insect behaviour: Motion camouflage in dragonflies". Nature. 65 (423): 604. Bibcode:2003Natur.423..604M. doi:10.1038/423604a. PMID 12789327. S2CID 52871328.
  9. ^ a b Glendinning, Paul (27 January 2004). "Motion Camouflage". The Mathematics of Motion Camouflage. 271 (1538): 477–81. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2622. PMC 1691618. PMID 15129957.
  10. ^ Ghose, K'; Horiuchi, T.; Krishnaprasad, P. S.; Moss, C. (2006). "Echolocating bats use a nearly time-optimal strategy to intercept prey". PLOS Biology. 4 (5): 865–873. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040108. PMC 1436025. PMID 16605303.
  11. ^ Reddy, P. V.; Justh, E. W.; Krishnaprasad, P. S. (2007). (PDF). 2007 46th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control. pp. 1660–1665. doi:10.1109/CDC.2007.4434522. ISBN 978-1-4244-1497-0. S2CID 1114039. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-01-14.
  12. ^ Gonzalez-Bellido, Paloma T.; Fabian, Samuel T.; Nordström, Karin (December 2016). "Target detection in insects: optical, neural and behavioral optimizations". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 41: 122–128. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2016.09.001. hdl:1912/8647. PMID 27662056. S2CID 3524650.
  13. ^ a b c d e f Amador Kane, Suzanne; Zamani, Marjon (2014). "Falcons pursue prey using visual motion cues: new perspectives from animal-borne cameras". Journal of Experimental Biology. 217 (2): 225–234. doi:10.1242/jeb.092403. PMC 3898623. PMID 24431144.
  14. ^ Justh, Eric W. (2010). Armstrong, Robert E.; Drapeau, Mark D.; Loeb, Cheryl A.; Valdes, James L. (eds.). Learning Unmanned Vehicle Control from Echolocating Bats. National Defense University Press. pp. 157–168. GGKEY:YX732WBJBQN. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  15. ^ Graham-Rowe, Duncan (28 June 2003). "Dragonfly trick makes missiles harder to dodge". New Scientist. Retrieved 27 April 2017.
  16. ^ Justh, E. W.; Krishnaprasad, P. S. (2006). "Steering laws for motion camouflage". Proceedings of the Royal Society A. 462 (2076): 3629. arXiv:math/0508023. Bibcode:2006RSPSA.462.3629J. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.236.1470. doi:10.1098/rspa.2006.1742. S2CID 7992554.
  17. ^ Carey, N. E.; Ford, J. J.; Chahl, J. S. (2004). "Biologically inspired guidance for motion camouflage". 5th Asian IEEE Control Conference. ISBN 0-7803-8873-9. S2CID 34907101.
  18. ^ Bian, Xue; Elgar, Mark A.; Peters, Richard A. (2016). "The swaying behavior of Extatosoma tiaratum : motion camouflage in a stick insect?". Behavioral Ecology. 27 (1): 83–92. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv125.
  19. ^ O'Dea, J. D. (1991). "Eine zusatzliche oder alternative Funktion der 'kryptischen' Schaukelbewegung bei Gottesanbeterinnen und Stabschrecken (Mantodea, Phasmatodea)". Entomologische Zeitschrift. 101 (1–2): 25–27.

External links edit

motion, camouflage, dazzle, induced, rapidly, moving, stripes, camouflage, motion, dazzle, camouflage, which, provides, degree, concealment, moving, object, given, that, motion, makes, objects, easy, detect, however, well, their, coloration, matches, their, ba. For dazzle induced by rapidly moving stripes see Camouflage Motion dazzle Motion camouflage is camouflage which provides a degree of concealment for a moving object given that motion makes objects easy to detect however well their coloration matches their background or breaks up their outlines Principle of motion camouflage by mimicking the optic flow of the background An attacker flies towards a target choosing its path so that it remains on a line between target and a real point behind the attacker this path differs from classical pursuit and is often shorter as illustrated here The attacker looms larger as it closes on target but does not otherwise appear to move Animals such as frogs are very good at detecting motion 1 making motion camouflage a priority for predators The principal form of motion camouflage and the type generally meant by the term involves an attacker s mimicking the optic flow of the background as seen by its target This enables the attacker to approach the target while appearing to remain stationary from the target s perspective unlike in classical pursuit where the attacker moves straight towards the target at all times and often appears to the target to move sideways The attacker chooses its flight path so as to remain on the line between the target and some landmark point The target therefore does not see the attacker move from the landmark point The only visible evidence that the attacker is moving is its looming the change in size as the attacker approaches Camouflage is sometimes facilitated by motion as in the leafy sea dragon and some stick insects These animals complement their passive camouflage by swaying like plants in the wind or ocean currents delaying their recognition by predators First discovered in hoverflies in 1995 motion camouflage by minimising optic flow has been demonstrated in another insect order dragonflies as well as in two groups of vertebrates falcons and echolocating bats Since bats hunting at night cannot be using the strategy for camouflage it has been named describing its mechanism as constant absolute target direction This is an efficient homing strategy and it has been suggested that anti aircraft missiles could benefit from similar techniques Contents 1 Camouflage of approach motion 1 1 Stealthy movements 1 2 Minimising motion signal 1 3 Disrupting perception of motion 1 4 Mimicking optic flow of background 1 4 1 Pursuit strategies 2 Camouflage by motion 2 1 Swaying motion crypsis or masquerade 3 References 4 External linksCamouflage of approach motion editFurther information List of camouflage methods Many animals are highly sensitive to motion for example frogs readily detect small moving dark spots but ignore stationary ones 1 Therefore motion signals can be used to defeat camouflage 2 Moving objects with disruptive camouflage patterns remain harder to identify than uncamouflaged objects especially if other similar objects are nearby even though they are detected so motion does not completely break camouflage 3 All the same the conspicuousness of motion raises the question of whether and how motion itself could be camouflaged Several mechanisms are possible 2 nbsp Predators such as tigers stalk prey very slowly to minimise motion cues Stealthy movements edit One strategy is to minimise actual motion as when predators such as tigers stalk prey by moving very slowly and stealthily This strategy effectively avoids the need to camouflage motion 2 4 Minimising motion signal edit When movement is required one strategy is to minimise the motion signal for example by avoiding waving limbs about and by choosing patterns that do not cause flicker when seen by the prey from straight ahead 2 Cuttlefish may be doing this with their active camouflage by choosing to form stripes at right angles to their front back axis minimising motion signals that would be given by occluding and displaying the pattern as they swim 5 Disrupting perception of motion edit Disrupting the attacker s perception of the target s motion was the main intended purpose of dazzle camouflage as used on ships in the First World War though its effectiveness is disputed This type of dazzle does not appear to be used by animals 2 Mimicking optic flow of background edit nbsp The Australian emperor dragonfly mimics the optic flow of its background using real point motion camouflage to enable it to approach rivals Some animals mimic the optic flow of the background so that the attacker does not appear to move when seen by the target This is the main focus of work on motion camouflage and is often treated as synonymous with it 2 6 Pursuit strategies edit An attacker can mimic the background s optic flow by choosing its flight path so as to remain on the line between the target and either some real landmark point or a point at infinite distance giving different pursuit algorithms It therefore does not move from the landmark point as seen by the target though it inevitably looms larger as it approaches This is not the same as moving straight towards the target classical pursuit that results in visible sideways motion with a readily detectable difference in optic flow from the background The strategy works whether the background is plain or textured 6 This motion camouflage strategy was discovered and modelled as algorithms in 1995 by M V Srinivasan and M Davey while they were studying mating behaviour in hoverflies The male hoverfly appeared to be using the tracking technique to approach prospective mates 6 Motion camouflage has been observed in high speed territorial battles between dragonflies where males of the Australian emperor dragonfly Hemianax papuensis were seen to choose their flight paths to appear stationary to their rivals in 6 of 15 encounters They made use of both real point and infinity point strategies 7 8 nbsp Falcons use infinite point motion camouflage to close on their prey The strategy appears to work equally well in insects and in vertebrates Simulations show that motion camouflage results in a more efficient pursuit path than classical pursuit i e the motion camouflage path is shorter whether the target flies in a straight line or chooses a chaotic path Further where classical pursuit requires the attacker to fly faster than the target the motion camouflaged attacker can sometimes capture the target despite flying more slowly than it 9 2 In sailing it has long been known that if the bearing from the target to the pursuer remains constant known as constant bearing decreasing range CBDR equivalent to taking a fixed reference point at infinite distance the two vessels are on a collision course both travelling in straight lines In a simulation this is readily observed as the lines between the two remain parallel at all times 9 2 nbsp Insect hunting bats and some missiles follow an infinity point pursuit path keeping parallel to the target Parallel navigation for its efficiency rather than for camouflage Echolocating bats follow an infinity point 2 path when hunting insects in the dark This is not for camouflage but for the efficiency of the resulting path so the strategy is generally called constant absolute target direction CATD 10 11 12 it is equivalent to CBDR but allowing for the target to manoeuvre erratically 13 A 2014 study of falcons of different species gyrfalcon saker falcon and peregrine falcon used video cameras mounted on their heads or backs to track their approaches to prey Comparison of the observed paths with simulations of different pursuit strategies showed that these predatory birds used a motion camouflage path consistent with CATD 13 The missile guidance strategy of pure proportional navigation guidance PPNG closely resembles the CATD strategy used by bats 14 The biologists Andrew Anderson and Peter McOwan have suggested that anti aircraft missiles could exploit motion camouflage to reduce their chances of being detected They tested their ideas on people playing a computerised war game 15 The steering laws to achieve motion camouflage have been analysed mathematically The resulting paths turn out to be extremely efficient often better than classical pursuit Motion camouflage pursuit may therefore be adopted both by predators and missile engineers as parallel navigation for an infinity point algorithm for its performance advantages 16 17 Attack strategies 13 Strategy Description Camouflage effect Used by speciesClassical pursuit pursuit guidance Move straight towards current position of target at all times simplest strategy None target sees pursuer moving against background Honey bees flies tiger beetles 13 Real point motion camouflage Move towards target keeping between it and a point near pursuer s start at all times Pursuer remains stationary against background but looms larger Dragonflies hoverflies 13 Infinity point motion camouflage CATD Parallel navigation Move towards target keeping line to target parallel to line between pursuer s start and target at start Pursuer remains at a constant direction in the sky Dogs humans hoverflies teleost fish bats falcons 13 Camouflage by motion edit nbsp Cryptic stick insect Extatosoma tiaratum sways in the wind like foliage source source source source source source source Preying mantises exhibiting motion camouflageSwaying motion crypsis or masquerade edit Swaying behaviour is practised by highly cryptic animals such as the leafy sea dragon the stick insect Extatosoma tiaratum and mantises These animals resemble vegetation with their coloration strikingly disruptive body outlines with leaflike appendages and the ability to sway effectively like the plants that they mimic E tiaratum actively sways back and forth or side to side when disturbed or when there is a gust of wind with a frequency distribution like foliage rustling in the wind This behaviour may represent motion crypsis preventing detection by predators or motion masquerade promoting misclassification as something other than prey or a combination of the two and has accordingly also been described as a form of motion camouflage 18 19 References edit a b Anstis Stuart 1 June 2015 Seeing Isn t Believing The Scientist Retrieved 28 April 2017 a b c d e f g h i Troscianko T 2011 Stevens Martin Merilaita Sami eds Animal Camouflage Mechanisms and Function Cambridge University Press pp 128 130 ISBN 978 1 139 49623 0 Hall Joanna R Cuthill Innes C Baddeley Roland Shohet Adam J Scott Samuel Nicholas E 7 May 2013 Camouflage detection and identification of moving targets Proceedings of the Royal Society B 280 1578 20130064 doi 10 1098 rspb 2013 0064 PMC 3619462 PMID 23486439 Macdonald David Loveridge Andrew 2010 The Biology and Conservation of Wild Felids Oxford University Press pp 84 ISBN 978 0 19 923444 8 Shohet A J Baddeley R J Anderson J C Kelman E J Osorio D 2006 Cuttlefish responses to visual orientation of substrates water flow and a model of motion camouflage PDF Journal of Experimental Biology 209 23 4717 4723 doi 10 1242 jeb 02580 PMID 17114404 S2CID 2485460 a b c Srinivasan M V amp Davey M 1995 Strategies for active camouflage of motion Proc R Soc Lond B 259 1354 19 25 Bibcode 1995RSPSB 259 19S doi 10 1098 rspb 1995 0004 S2CID 131341953 Hopkin Michael June 5 2003 Nature News Dragonfly Flight Tricks the Eye Nature com doi 10 1038 news030602 10 Retrieved January 16 2012 Mizutani A K Chahl J S Srinivasan M V June 5 2003 Insect behaviour Motion camouflage in dragonflies Nature 65 423 604 Bibcode 2003Natur 423 604M doi 10 1038 423604a PMID 12789327 S2CID 52871328 a b Glendinning Paul 27 January 2004 Motion Camouflage The Mathematics of Motion Camouflage 271 1538 477 81 doi 10 1098 rspb 2003 2622 PMC 1691618 PMID 15129957 Ghose K Horiuchi T Krishnaprasad P S Moss C 2006 Echolocating bats use a nearly time optimal strategy to intercept prey PLOS Biology 4 5 865 873 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0040108 PMC 1436025 PMID 16605303 Reddy P V Justh E W Krishnaprasad P S 2007 Motion camouflage with sensorimotor delay PDF 2007 46th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control pp 1660 1665 doi 10 1109 CDC 2007 4434522 ISBN 978 1 4244 1497 0 S2CID 1114039 Archived from the original PDF on 2018 01 14 Gonzalez Bellido Paloma T Fabian Samuel T Nordstrom Karin December 2016 Target detection in insects optical neural and behavioral optimizations Current Opinion in Neurobiology 41 122 128 doi 10 1016 j conb 2016 09 001 hdl 1912 8647 PMID 27662056 S2CID 3524650 a b c d e f Amador Kane Suzanne Zamani Marjon 2014 Falcons pursue prey using visual motion cues new perspectives from animal borne cameras Journal of Experimental Biology 217 2 225 234 doi 10 1242 jeb 092403 PMC 3898623 PMID 24431144 Justh Eric W 2010 Armstrong Robert E Drapeau Mark D Loeb Cheryl A Valdes James L eds Learning Unmanned Vehicle Control from Echolocating Bats National Defense University Press pp 157 168 GGKEY YX732WBJBQN a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a work ignored help Graham Rowe Duncan 28 June 2003 Dragonfly trick makes missiles harder to dodge New Scientist Retrieved 27 April 2017 Justh E W Krishnaprasad P S 2006 Steering laws for motion camouflage Proceedings of the Royal Society A 462 2076 3629 arXiv math 0508023 Bibcode 2006RSPSA 462 3629J CiteSeerX 10 1 1 236 1470 doi 10 1098 rspa 2006 1742 S2CID 7992554 Carey N E Ford J J Chahl J S 2004 Biologically inspired guidance for motion camouflage 5th Asian IEEE Control Conference ISBN 0 7803 8873 9 S2CID 34907101 Bian Xue Elgar Mark A Peters Richard A 2016 The swaying behavior of Extatosoma tiaratum motion camouflage in a stick insect Behavioral Ecology 27 1 83 92 doi 10 1093 beheco arv125 O Dea J D 1991 Eine zusatzliche oder alternative Funktion der kryptischen Schaukelbewegung bei Gottesanbeterinnen und Stabschrecken Mantodea Phasmatodea Entomologische Zeitschrift 101 1 2 25 27 External links edit Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Motion camouflage amp oldid 1202654865 Pursuit strategies, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.