fbpx
Wikipedia

Pain empathy

Pain empathy is a specific variety of empathy that involves recognizing and understanding another person's pain.

Empathy is the mental ability that allows one person to understand another person's mental and emotional state and how to effectively respond to that person. There are several cues that can communicate pain to another person: visualization of the injury-causing event, the injury itself, behavioral efforts of the injured to avoid further harm, and displays of pain and distress such as facial expressions, crying, and screaming.[1] When a person receives cues that another person is in pain, neural pain circuits within the receiver's brain are activated. From an evolutionary perspective, pain empathy is beneficial for human group survival since it provides motivation for non-injured people to offer aid to the injured and to avoid injury themselves.[citation needed]

Initiating pain empathy edit

Resonance edit

A person who perceives another person's affective state can experience changes in brain activity, known as resonance, that help initiate an empathetic response. The inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior parietal lobule are two regions of the brain associated with[clarification needed] empathy resonance.[2]

Self-other discrimination edit

In order to have empathy for another person, one must understand the context of that person's experience while maintaining a certain degree of separation from their own[clarification needed] experience. The ability to differentiate the source of an affective stimuli as originating from the self or from other another is known as self-other discrimination. Self-other discrimination is associated with[clarification needed] the extrastriate body area (EBA), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), ventral premotor cortex, and the posterior and inferior parietal cortex.[2]

Response to painful facial expressions edit

A painful facial expression is one way in which the experience of pain is communicated from one individual to another. One study measured test subjects' brain activity and facial muscle activity when they watched video clips of a variety of facial expressions including neutral emotion, joy, fear, and pain. The study found that when a subject is shown a painful facial expression, their late positive potential (LPP) is increased during the time period of 600–1000ms after the initial exposure of the stimulus.[clarification needed] This increase in LPP for painful facial expressions was higher than the increase caused by other emotional expressions. [3]

Pain processing areas of the brain (The Pain Matrix) edit

Several areas of the brain are related to the processing of pain and pain empathy. These are known as the pain matrix.

One functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study measured brain activity during the experience of painful stimuli or while observing someone else receiving a painful stimuli. The study group consisted of 16 couples, on the assumption that these individuals would have empathy for one another. One member of the couple would receive a painful stimulus via electrode to the back of their hand while their partner observed, and brain activity was measured in both participants.[4]

 
fMRI images of brain regions involved in pain processing. Borsook D, Moulton EA, Schmidt KF, Becerra LR.

First hand experience of pain edit

The activated brain regions in the person experiencing the pain firsthand in that study included: contralateral sensorimotor cortex, bilateral secondary sensorimotor cortex, contralateral posterior insula, bilateral mid and anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex, right ventrolateral and mediodorsal thalamus, brainstem, and mid and right lateral cerebellum.[4] Another fMRI study measured brain activity of an individual receiving unpredictable laser pain stimuli. This study showed that the primary and secondary sensorimotor cortex, posterior insula, and lateral thalamus are involved in processing aspects of nociceptive stimuli such as location and intensity.[5]

Observed pain in others edit

Several brain regions including the bilateral anterior insula (AI), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brainstem, and cerebellum were activated both in instances of first person painful experience and observed painful experience. The bilateral anterior insula (AI) and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) are therefore hypothesized to take part in the emotional reaction evoked from witnessing another in pain. The somatosensory region of the brain was not shown by fMRI to be excited during pain observation, rather only when the pain was experienced firsthand.[4]

One electroencephalography study recorded brain activity while subjects were shown footage of a needle penetrating a hand. There was an increase in activity in the frontal, temporal, and parietal areas of the brain when the individuals were exposed to the footage and observed pain in others. The gamma band oscillations, which reflects brain activity, increased to 50–70 Hz. The usual gamma band oscillations in the brain are around 40 Hz. The results suggest high gamma band oscillations could help provide an understanding of the neural basis of empathy for pain.[6]

Debates edit

It is unclear as to how much the pain matrix is involved in pain empathy. Some studies have found that when all neural components of the pain matrix are observed together, they are more related to one's reaction to the presence of stimuli than pain in particular.[7] The ability to empathize with the pain of others seems to be correlated only to certain parts of the pain matrix rather than the matrix as a whole.[4] Some have argued that only the affective components of the pain matrix, the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex, are activated when it comes to pain empathy.[8]

Detection methods edit

Magnetoencephalography edit

fMRI studies could not detect activity in the somatosensory cortex during pain empathy. Neuromagnetic oscillatory activity was recorded from the primary somatosensory cortex in order to determine if it is involved in pain empathy. When the left medial nerve was stimulated, post-stimulus rebounds of 10 Hz of somatosensory oscillations were quantified. These baseline somatosensory oscillations were suppressed when the subject observed a painful stimulus to a stranger. These results show that the somatosensory cortex is involved in the pain empathy response even though the activity could not be detected using fMRI techniques.[9]

Electroencephalography (EEG) edit

Mu rhythm is a neural oscillation ranging from 8–12 Hz that occurs over the sensorimotor cortex and is suppressed during motor movements.[10] Motor resonance in response to pain empathy has been quantified using EEG technology as a decrease in power of mu rhythm over the sensorimotor cortex.[11] More suppression of mu rhythm is associated to an increased empathic response.[12] A study recording from central sensorimotor EEG channels (C3, Cz, & C4) found that participants had greater suppression of mu rhythm when shown images of hands and feet in painful situations as opposed to non-painful situations.[13]

Transcranial magnetic stimulation edit

Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation, when used to stimulate the motor cortex of a person observing another person's actions, increases the corticospinal excitability of associated with their motor resonance[clarification needed]. Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies show that frontal structures of the motor resonance system[clarification needed] process information about other people's physical actions.[2]

Motor evoked potential edit

Sensorimotor contagion takes place when corticospinal excitability reduces due to observing another person experiencing pain. A study by Avenanti on pain empathy in racial bias showed that when a person sees a needle being poked into the hand of another person, there is a reduced motor evoked potential in the muscle of the observer's hand.[14]

Lack of pain empathy edit

Lack of empathy occurs in several conditions including autism, schizophrenia, sadistic personality disorder, psychopathy, and sociopathy. One interpretation is that an improper ratio of cortical excitability to inhibition causes empathy defects. Brain stimulation is being investigated for its potential to alter motor resonance, pain empathy, self-other discrimination, and mentalizing as a way to treat empathy related disorders.[2]

Schizophrenia edit

Schizophrenic patients are impaired in several empathy domains. They are less able to identify emotions, take on different perspectives, and perform low level facial mirroring, and they are less sensitive in terms of affective responsiveness.[15] They are less responsive to their own pain and are less empathetic to that of others as well.[16] Schizophrenic patients exhibit neural and structural alterations in brain regions that are normally associated with the processing of empathy, such as the temporo-parietal junction and the amygdala. This is part of the reason why schizophrenic individuals have difficulties in understanding and reacting to the pain of others, impairing their ability to empathize with pain.[17]

One EEG study observed the brain activity of individuals with schizophrenia who were shown hands and feet in painful situations. The results suggested the ability to separate self from others, which is essential to empathizing with other people, is impaired in the schizophrenic sample.[17] Another study concluded that schizophrenic individuals tend to process other-related pain stimuli in a way that is usually more associated with how self-relevant stimuli are processed, and because of this these individuals are less able to differentiate between self and other.[18]

Schizophrenic subjects who participated in studies have also had difficulty assessing and discriminating between different levels of pain. For example, in one study, those with schizophrenia were less likely to be able to discriminate between ‘strong’ and ‘moderate’ pain levels than control subjects who did not have schizophrenia.[15] Subjects with schizophrenia were also less able to rate pain intensities from looking at different facial expressions.[16] The way in which these individuals empathize with pain may be caused partially by the lack of sensitivity in terms of affective processing of pain and the severity of the increased suspiciousness seen in many schizophrenics.

Those with schizophrenia are more easily disturbed by negative emotions of others (including pain), compared with healthy samples.[17] Schizophrenics tend to feel increased levels of personal distress when perceiving any sign of pain in other people.[19] This personal distress has been described as "hyper sensitivity" to the pain of others[18] that overwhelms the individual and hence impairs their ability to empathetically respond and relate to the pain of others on a personal level. The increased levels of personal distress may increase the individual's need to reduce their own discomfort, with the result that they give less attention to and empathize less with the pain of others.[19][20]

Sadistic personality disorder edit

Psychopathy is thought to be caused by normal processing of social and emotional cues, but abnormal use of these cues.[2] One fMRI study looked at the brain activity of youth with aggressive conduct disorder and socially normal youth when they observed empathy eliciting stimuli. The results showed that the aggressive conduct disorder group had activation in the amygdala and ventral striatum, which lead the researcher to believe that these subjects may get a rewarding feeling from viewing pain in others.[21]

Autism edit

Autism-spectrum disorders are characterized by an impairment in the processing of social and emotional affective cues.[2]

Juvenile psychopathy edit

Young individuals who have callous and unemotional traits (CU) exhibit a lack of empathy. If a person experiences pain empathy they will be less motivated to hurt others, since people that experience pain empathy have distress when another person is hurt.

One study showed juvenile psychopaths video clips of strangers experiencing painful stimuli. The results showed that the juvenile psychopaths processed of these pain empathy eliciting stimuli atypically in comparison with the normal juvenile controls. The central late positive potential (LPP), a late cognitive evaluative component of pain, was decreased in subjects with low CU traits. Subjects with high CU traits had both a decrease in central LPP and in frontal N120, an early affective arousal component of pain. There were also differences in the pain thresholds between normal test subjects, subjects with low CU traits, and subjects with high CU traits. The subjects with CU traits had higher pain thresholds than the controls, which suggests they were less sensitive to noxious pain. The results show the CU-trait juvenile's lack of pain empathy was due to a lack of arousal due to another person's distress rather than a lack of understanding of the other's emotional state.[22]

Bias and variation edit

There is evidence that individual empathetic responses to the pain of others are biased based on racial identity, in-group/out-group status, and position on a social hierarchy. Experiences of pain empathy also vary depending on differences in personality such as the level of threat sensitivity in an individual. An individual who is highly sensitive to threat will have more intense empathetic reactions to others in pain when compared to those who are not as sensitive.[23]

In-group bias edit

An experiment used a minimal group paradigm[clarification needed] to create two groups, the in-group and the outgroup, to which they assigned 30 subjects.[24] 30 healthy right handed subjects participated in a fMRI experimental session in which they were shown 128 photographs where right hands and feet were in painful situations. The results of the study did not report any sign of an in-group or out-group favoritism, meaning subjects did not feel higher levels of empathy based on the groups they were in.[clarification needed]

Racial bias edit

A transcranial magnetic stimulation experiment measured inhibition of corticospinal excitability to determine if another person's race affected a person's pain empathy. Caucasian and black participants watched video clips of a needle penetrating a muscle in the right hand of a stranger who either belonged to their racial group or the other racial group. Transcranial magnetic stimulation stimulated the left motor cortex and motor-evoked potentials were measured in the observer's first dorsal interosseus muscle of the right hand. The results of the experiment showed that when the video clip showed a hand belonging to a person of the same racial group, the measured corticospinal excitability to the right hand of the observer was reduced. This inhibition effect was not present when a subject viewed a clip of a hand belonging to a stranger outside of their racial group. This corticospinal inhibition occurs in first hand[clarification needed] experience of pain, and suggests that there is activation of the observer's sensorimotor system while witnessing painful stimuli.[14]

Social hierarchy bias edit

An experiment placed individuals in a social hierarchy based on their skills when completing a task. The subjects were then scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging while watching those of an inferior and superior status experience painful or non-painful stimulation. The results of the study concluded relative positions in a social hierarchy affects empathetic responses to pain in that individuals are more likely to be more empathetic to the pain of those in an inferior status than to those in a more superior status.[25] This suggests there is bias depending on one's position on a social hierarchy when it comes to pain empathy.

Empathy gap edit

Humans tend to underestimate the intensity of physical pain other individuals go through, which affects their empathetic reactions to the pain of others.[23]

Physicians and pain empathy response edit

Physicians are frequently exposed to people experiencing pain due to injury or illness, or have to inflict pain during the course of treatment. Physicians have to regulate their emotional response to such stimuli in order to effectively help the patient and maintain their own personal well being. Pain empathy can motivate an individual to help someone who is in pain, but repeated exposure to individuals in pain with no ability to regulate emotional arousal can cause distress.

One study sought to determine if physicians were different in their response to viewing painful stimuli. Physicians and control subjects watched video clips of a stranger being poked with a needle into the hands or feet. fMRI was used to measure the hemodynamic activity within the brain while viewing the painful stimuli. The fMRI revealed that brain areas involved in the pain matrix—somatosensory cortex, anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus (dACC), and the periaqueductal gray (PAG)—were activated in the control subject population when viewing the needle penetration videos. The physicians had activation of higher order executive functioning in the brain as shown by activation of the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortex, both involved in self-regulation, and activation of the precentral, superior parietal, and temporoparietal junction, involved with executive attention. The physicians did not have activation of the anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus (dACC), or the periaqueductal gray (PAG). The study concluded that physicians adapt to the healthcare environment by down-regulating their automatic empathetic response to patients’ pain.[26]

Pain synesthesia edit

Synesthesia is a phenomenon that occurs when sensory information in one cognitive pathway causes a sensation through another cognitive pathway. Pain synesthesia is a form of synesthesia that causes a person to experience pain when seeing pain empathetic eliciting stimuli. The most common group who report pain synesthesia are patients with phantom limb syndrome.[27]

Cultural variation edit

Evidence from several studies points to cultural variation in pain empathy. An experiment in which British participants and East Asian participants were shown a video of a needle puncturing a hand demonstrated such cultural variations.[28] The results showed British subjects demonstrated more empathetic concern and higher levels of emotional experience to do with empathy compared to the East Asian participants.

References edit

  1. ^ Jean Decety, W. I. (Ed.). (2009). The Social Neuroscience of Empathy: MIT Press.
  2. ^ a b c d e f Hetu, S.; Taschereau-Dumouchel, V.; Jackson, P. L. (2012). "Stimulating the brain to study social interactions and empathy. [Article]". Brain Stimulation. 5 (2): 95–102. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.005. PMID 22503509. S2CID 18850529.
  3. ^ Reicherts, P.; Wieser, M. J.; Gerdes, A. B. M.; Likowski, K. U.; Weyers, P.; Muhlberger, A.; Pauli, P. (2012). "Electrocortical evidence for preferential processing of dynamic pain expressions compared to other emotional expressions. [Article]". Pain. 153 (9): 1959–1964. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2012.06.017. PMID 22795483. S2CID 22450468.
  4. ^ a b c d Singer, T. S. B.; O'Doherty, J; Kaube, H; Dolan, RJ; Frith, CD. (2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain". Science. 303 (5661): 1157–1162. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A020-5. PMID 14976305. S2CID 14727944.
  5. ^ Bingel, U.; Quante, M.; Knab, R.; Bromm, B.; Weiller, C.; Buchel, C. (2003). "Single trial fMRI reveals significant contralateral bias in responses to laser pain within thalamus and somatosensory cortices. [Article]". NeuroImage. 18 (3): 740–748. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00033-2. PMID 12667851. S2CID 6291627.
  6. ^ Motoyama, Y., Ogata, K., Hoka S., & Tobimatsu, S. (2010). "P15-15 Neural substrates of empathy for pain: Simultaneous recording of high density EEG and conventional ECG". Clinical Neurophysiology. 121: S194. doi:10.1016/s1388-2457(10)60798-5. S2CID 53144387.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Michael, J. & Fardo, F. (2014). "What (If Anything) Is Shared in Pain Empathy? A Critical Discussion of De Vignemont and Jacob's Theory of the Neural Substrate of Pain Empathy". Philosophy of Science. 81 (1): 154–160. doi:10.1086/674203. hdl:2434/923844. S2CID 143192758.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Bufalari, I., Aprile, T., Avenanti, A., Di Russo, F., & Aglioti, S. (2007). "Empathy for Pain and Touch in the Human Somatosensory Cortex". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (11): 2553–2561. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl161. PMID 17205974.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. ^ Cheng, Y., Yang, C. Y., Lin, C. P., Lee, P. L., & Decety, J. (2008). "The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: A magnetoencephalography study."
  10. ^ Pfurtscheller, G; Neuper, Ch; Andrew, C; Edlinger, G (1997-06-01). "Foot and hand area mu rhythms". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 26 (1): 121–135. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00760-5. ISSN 0167-8760. PMID 9202999.
  11. ^ Moore, Matthew R.; Franz, Elizabeth A. (2017-02-01). "Mu rhythm suppression is associated with the classification of emotion in faces". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 17 (1): 224–234. doi:10.3758/s13415-016-0476-6. ISSN 1531-135X. PMID 27815729. S2CID 24027633.
  12. ^ Joyal, Christian C.; Neveu, Sarah-Michelle; Boukhalfi, Tarik; Jackson, Philip L.; Renaud, Patrice (2018). "Suppression of Sensorimotor Alpha Power Associated With Pain Expressed by an Avatar: A Preliminary EEG Study". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 12: 273. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00273. ISSN 1662-5161. PMC 6046452. PMID 30038564.
  13. ^ Arnett, Kirsten; Roach, Alexandra; Elzy, Meredith; Jelsone-Swain, Laura (2019-03-04). "Childhood emotional invalidation and right hemispheric mu suppression during a pain empathy task: An EEG study". Social Neuroscience. 14 (2): 236–250. doi:10.1080/17470919.2018.1441905. ISSN 1747-0919. PMID 29451071. S2CID 3690092.
  14. ^ a b Avenanti, A.; Sirigu, A.; Aglioti, S. M. (2010). "Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain". Current Biology. 20 (11): 1018–1022. Bibcode:2010CBio...20.1018A. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071. PMID 20537539. S2CID 7048253.
  15. ^ a b Martins, M. J., Moura, B. L., Martins, I. P., Figueira, M. L. & Prkachin, K. M. (2011). "Sensitivity to expressions of pain in schizophrenia patients". Psychiatry Research. 189 (2): 180–184. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.03.007. PMID 21470693. S2CID 26371301.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ a b Wojakiewicz, A., Januel, D., Braha, S., Prkachin, K., Danziger, N. & Bouhassira, D. (2013). "Alteration of pain recognition in schizophrenia". European Journal of Pain. 17 (9): 1385–1392. doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00310.x. PMID 23529960. S2CID 32603486.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ a b c Gonzalez-Liencres, C., Brown, E. C., Tas, C., Breidenstein, A. & Brüne, M. (2016). "Alterations in event-related potential responses to empathy for pain in schizophrenia". Psychiatry Research. 241: 14–21. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.091. PMID 27152905. S2CID 4769270.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ a b Horan, W., Jimenez, A., Lee, J., Wynn, J., Eisenberger, N., & Green, M. (2016). "Pain empathy in schizophrenia: An fMRI study". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (5): 154–160. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw002. PMC 4847698. PMID 26746181.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ a b Didehbani, N., Shad, M. U., Tamminga, C. A., Kandalaft, M. R., Allen, T. T., Chapman, S. B., & Krawczyk, D. C. (2012). "Insight and empathy in schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research. 142 (1–3): 246–247. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.010. PMID 23043874. S2CID 6863595.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2017). "Empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index". Psychiatry Research. 249: 293–303. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.033. hdl:1805/11989. PMID 28142103. S2CID 5043806.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Decety, J.; Michalska, K. J.; Akitsuki, Y.; Lahey, B. B. (2009). "Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: A functional MRI investigation". Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–211. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC 2819310. PMID 18940230.
  22. ^ Cheng, Y. W.; Hung, A. Y.; Decety, J. (2012). "Dissociation between affective sharing and emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths. [Article]". Development and Psychopathology. 24 (2): 623–636. doi:10.1017/s095457941200020x. PMID 22559135. S2CID 37377507.
  23. ^ a b Auyeung, K., & Alden, W. (2016). "Social Anxiety and Empathy for Social Pain". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 40 (1): 38–45. doi:10.1007/s10608-015-9718-0. hdl:2429/62699. S2CID 43695016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Ruckman, J., Bodden, M., Jansen, A., Kircher, T., Dobel, R. & Reif, W. (2015). "How pain empathy depends on ingroup/outgroup decisions: A functional magnet resonance imaging study". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 234 (1): 57–65. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.08.006. PMID 26323252. S2CID 7209426.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Feng, C., Li, Z., Feng, X., Wang, L., Tian, T., & Luo, Y. (2016). "Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (3): 485–495. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv135. PMC 4769637. PMID 26516169.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. ^ Decety, J.; Yang, C. Y.; Cheng, Y. W. (2010). "Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: An event-related brain potential study. [Article]". NeuroImage. 50 (4): 1676–1682. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.025. PMID 20080194. S2CID 6081243.
  27. ^ Fitzgibbon, B. M.; Giummarra, M. J.; Georgiou-Karistianis, N.; Enticott, P. G.; Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). "Shared pain: From empathy to synaesthesia. [Review]". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (4): 500–512. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.007. PMID 19857517. S2CID 14874873.
  28. ^ Atkins, D., Uskul, A., & Cooper, N. (2016). "ulture shapes empathic responses to physical and social pain". Emotion. 16 (5): 587–601. doi:10.1037/emo0000162. PMC 4976796. PMID 26950365.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)

pain, empathy, specific, variety, empathy, that, involves, recognizing, understanding, another, person, pain, empathy, mental, ability, that, allows, person, understand, another, person, mental, emotional, state, effectively, respond, that, person, there, seve. Pain empathy is a specific variety of empathy that involves recognizing and understanding another person s pain Empathy is the mental ability that allows one person to understand another person s mental and emotional state and how to effectively respond to that person There are several cues that can communicate pain to another person visualization of the injury causing event the injury itself behavioral efforts of the injured to avoid further harm and displays of pain and distress such as facial expressions crying and screaming 1 When a person receives cues that another person is in pain neural pain circuits within the receiver s brain are activated From an evolutionary perspective pain empathy is beneficial for human group survival since it provides motivation for non injured people to offer aid to the injured and to avoid injury themselves citation needed Contents 1 Initiating pain empathy 1 1 Resonance 1 2 Self other discrimination 1 3 Response to painful facial expressions 2 Pain processing areas of the brain The Pain Matrix 2 1 First hand experience of pain 2 2 Observed pain in others 2 3 Debates 3 Detection methods 3 1 Magnetoencephalography 3 2 Electroencephalography EEG 3 3 Transcranial magnetic stimulation 3 4 Motor evoked potential 4 Lack of pain empathy 4 1 Schizophrenia 4 2 Sadistic personality disorder 4 3 Autism 4 4 Juvenile psychopathy 5 Bias and variation 5 1 In group bias 5 2 Racial bias 5 3 Social hierarchy bias 5 4 Empathy gap 6 Physicians and pain empathy response 7 Pain synesthesia 8 Cultural variation 9 ReferencesInitiating pain empathy editResonance edit A person who perceives another person s affective state can experience changes in brain activity known as resonance that help initiate an empathetic response The inferior frontal gyrus and the inferior parietal lobule are two regions of the brain associated with clarification needed empathy resonance 2 Self other discrimination edit In order to have empathy for another person one must understand the context of that person s experience while maintaining a certain degree of separation from their own clarification needed experience The ability to differentiate the source of an affective stimuli as originating from the self or from other another is known as self other discrimination Self other discrimination is associated with clarification needed the extrastriate body area EBA posterior superior temporal sulcus pSTS temporoparietal junction TPJ ventral premotor cortex and the posterior and inferior parietal cortex 2 Response to painful facial expressions edit A painful facial expression is one way in which the experience of pain is communicated from one individual to another One study measured test subjects brain activity and facial muscle activity when they watched video clips of a variety of facial expressions including neutral emotion joy fear and pain The study found that when a subject is shown a painful facial expression their late positive potential LPP is increased during the time period of 600 1000ms after the initial exposure of the stimulus clarification needed This increase in LPP for painful facial expressions was higher than the increase caused by other emotional expressions 3 Pain processing areas of the brain The Pain Matrix editSeveral areas of the brain are related to the processing of pain and pain empathy These are known as the pain matrix One functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI study measured brain activity during the experience of painful stimuli or while observing someone else receiving a painful stimuli The study group consisted of 16 couples on the assumption that these individuals would have empathy for one another One member of the couple would receive a painful stimulus via electrode to the back of their hand while their partner observed and brain activity was measured in both participants 4 nbsp fMRI images of brain regions involved in pain processing Borsook D Moulton EA Schmidt KF Becerra LR First hand experience of pain edit The activated brain regions in the person experiencing the pain firsthand in that study included contralateral sensorimotor cortex bilateral secondary sensorimotor cortex contralateral posterior insula bilateral mid and anterior insula anterior cingulate cortex right ventrolateral and mediodorsal thalamus brainstem and mid and right lateral cerebellum 4 Another fMRI study measured brain activity of an individual receiving unpredictable laser pain stimuli This study showed that the primary and secondary sensorimotor cortex posterior insula and lateral thalamus are involved in processing aspects of nociceptive stimuli such as location and intensity 5 Observed pain in others edit Several brain regions including the bilateral anterior insula AI rostral anterior cingulate cortex ACC brainstem and cerebellum were activated both in instances of first person painful experience and observed painful experience The bilateral anterior insula AI and rostral anterior cingulate cortex ACC are therefore hypothesized to take part in the emotional reaction evoked from witnessing another in pain The somatosensory region of the brain was not shown by fMRI to be excited during pain observation rather only when the pain was experienced firsthand 4 One electroencephalography study recorded brain activity while subjects were shown footage of a needle penetrating a hand There was an increase in activity in the frontal temporal and parietal areas of the brain when the individuals were exposed to the footage and observed pain in others The gamma band oscillations which reflects brain activity increased to 50 70 Hz The usual gamma band oscillations in the brain are around 40 Hz The results suggest high gamma band oscillations could help provide an understanding of the neural basis of empathy for pain 6 Debates edit It is unclear as to how much the pain matrix is involved in pain empathy Some studies have found that when all neural components of the pain matrix are observed together they are more related to one s reaction to the presence of stimuli than pain in particular 7 The ability to empathize with the pain of others seems to be correlated only to certain parts of the pain matrix rather than the matrix as a whole 4 Some have argued that only the affective components of the pain matrix the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex are activated when it comes to pain empathy 8 Detection methods editMagnetoencephalography edit fMRI studies could not detect activity in the somatosensory cortex during pain empathy Neuromagnetic oscillatory activity was recorded from the primary somatosensory cortex in order to determine if it is involved in pain empathy When the left medial nerve was stimulated post stimulus rebounds of 10 Hz of somatosensory oscillations were quantified These baseline somatosensory oscillations were suppressed when the subject observed a painful stimulus to a stranger These results show that the somatosensory cortex is involved in the pain empathy response even though the activity could not be detected using fMRI techniques 9 Electroencephalography EEG edit Mu rhythm is a neural oscillation ranging from 8 12 Hz that occurs over the sensorimotor cortex and is suppressed during motor movements 10 Motor resonance in response to pain empathy has been quantified using EEG technology as a decrease in power of mu rhythm over the sensorimotor cortex 11 More suppression of mu rhythm is associated to an increased empathic response 12 A study recording from central sensorimotor EEG channels C3 Cz amp C4 found that participants had greater suppression of mu rhythm when shown images of hands and feet in painful situations as opposed to non painful situations 13 Transcranial magnetic stimulation edit Single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation when used to stimulate the motor cortex of a person observing another person s actions increases the corticospinal excitability of associated with their motor resonance clarification needed Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies show that frontal structures of the motor resonance system clarification needed process information about other people s physical actions 2 Motor evoked potential edit Sensorimotor contagion takes place when corticospinal excitability reduces due to observing another person experiencing pain A study by Avenanti on pain empathy in racial bias showed that when a person sees a needle being poked into the hand of another person there is a reduced motor evoked potential in the muscle of the observer s hand 14 Lack of pain empathy editLack of empathy occurs in several conditions including autism schizophrenia sadistic personality disorder psychopathy and sociopathy One interpretation is that an improper ratio of cortical excitability to inhibition causes empathy defects Brain stimulation is being investigated for its potential to alter motor resonance pain empathy self other discrimination and mentalizing as a way to treat empathy related disorders 2 Schizophrenia edit Schizophrenic patients are impaired in several empathy domains They are less able to identify emotions take on different perspectives and perform low level facial mirroring and they are less sensitive in terms of affective responsiveness 15 They are less responsive to their own pain and are less empathetic to that of others as well 16 Schizophrenic patients exhibit neural and structural alterations in brain regions that are normally associated with the processing of empathy such as the temporo parietal junction and the amygdala This is part of the reason why schizophrenic individuals have difficulties in understanding and reacting to the pain of others impairing their ability to empathize with pain 17 One EEG study observed the brain activity of individuals with schizophrenia who were shown hands and feet in painful situations The results suggested the ability to separate self from others which is essential to empathizing with other people is impaired in the schizophrenic sample 17 Another study concluded that schizophrenic individuals tend to process other related pain stimuli in a way that is usually more associated with how self relevant stimuli are processed and because of this these individuals are less able to differentiate between self and other 18 Schizophrenic subjects who participated in studies have also had difficulty assessing and discriminating between different levels of pain For example in one study those with schizophrenia were less likely to be able to discriminate between strong and moderate pain levels than control subjects who did not have schizophrenia 15 Subjects with schizophrenia were also less able to rate pain intensities from looking at different facial expressions 16 The way in which these individuals empathize with pain may be caused partially by the lack of sensitivity in terms of affective processing of pain and the severity of the increased suspiciousness seen in many schizophrenics Those with schizophrenia are more easily disturbed by negative emotions of others including pain compared with healthy samples 17 Schizophrenics tend to feel increased levels of personal distress when perceiving any sign of pain in other people 19 This personal distress has been described as hyper sensitivity to the pain of others 18 that overwhelms the individual and hence impairs their ability to empathetically respond and relate to the pain of others on a personal level The increased levels of personal distress may increase the individual s need to reduce their own discomfort with the result that they give less attention to and empathize less with the pain of others 19 20 Sadistic personality disorder edit Psychopathy is thought to be caused by normal processing of social and emotional cues but abnormal use of these cues 2 One fMRI study looked at the brain activity of youth with aggressive conduct disorder and socially normal youth when they observed empathy eliciting stimuli The results showed that the aggressive conduct disorder group had activation in the amygdala and ventral striatum which lead the researcher to believe that these subjects may get a rewarding feeling from viewing pain in others 21 Autism edit Autism spectrum disorders are characterized by an impairment in the processing of social and emotional affective cues 2 Juvenile psychopathy edit Young individuals who have callous and unemotional traits CU exhibit a lack of empathy If a person experiences pain empathy they will be less motivated to hurt others since people that experience pain empathy have distress when another person is hurt One study showed juvenile psychopaths video clips of strangers experiencing painful stimuli The results showed that the juvenile psychopaths processed of these pain empathy eliciting stimuli atypically in comparison with the normal juvenile controls The central late positive potential LPP a late cognitive evaluative component of pain was decreased in subjects with low CU traits Subjects with high CU traits had both a decrease in central LPP and in frontal N120 an early affective arousal component of pain There were also differences in the pain thresholds between normal test subjects subjects with low CU traits and subjects with high CU traits The subjects with CU traits had higher pain thresholds than the controls which suggests they were less sensitive to noxious pain The results show the CU trait juvenile s lack of pain empathy was due to a lack of arousal due to another person s distress rather than a lack of understanding of the other s emotional state 22 Bias and variation editThere is evidence that individual empathetic responses to the pain of others are biased based on racial identity in group out group status and position on a social hierarchy Experiences of pain empathy also vary depending on differences in personality such as the level of threat sensitivity in an individual An individual who is highly sensitive to threat will have more intense empathetic reactions to others in pain when compared to those who are not as sensitive 23 In group bias edit An experiment used a minimal group paradigm clarification needed to create two groups the in group and the outgroup to which they assigned 30 subjects 24 30 healthy right handed subjects participated in a fMRI experimental session in which they were shown 128 photographs where right hands and feet were in painful situations The results of the study did not report any sign of an in group or out group favoritism meaning subjects did not feel higher levels of empathy based on the groups they were in clarification needed Racial bias edit A transcranial magnetic stimulation experiment measured inhibition of corticospinal excitability to determine if another person s race affected a person s pain empathy Caucasian and black participants watched video clips of a needle penetrating a muscle in the right hand of a stranger who either belonged to their racial group or the other racial group Transcranial magnetic stimulation stimulated the left motor cortex and motor evoked potentials were measured in the observer s first dorsal interosseus muscle of the right hand The results of the experiment showed that when the video clip showed a hand belonging to a person of the same racial group the measured corticospinal excitability to the right hand of the observer was reduced This inhibition effect was not present when a subject viewed a clip of a hand belonging to a stranger outside of their racial group This corticospinal inhibition occurs in first hand clarification needed experience of pain and suggests that there is activation of the observer s sensorimotor system while witnessing painful stimuli 14 Social hierarchy bias edit An experiment placed individuals in a social hierarchy based on their skills when completing a task The subjects were then scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging while watching those of an inferior and superior status experience painful or non painful stimulation The results of the study concluded relative positions in a social hierarchy affects empathetic responses to pain in that individuals are more likely to be more empathetic to the pain of those in an inferior status than to those in a more superior status 25 This suggests there is bias depending on one s position on a social hierarchy when it comes to pain empathy Empathy gap edit Humans tend to underestimate the intensity of physical pain other individuals go through which affects their empathetic reactions to the pain of others 23 Physicians and pain empathy response editPhysicians are frequently exposed to people experiencing pain due to injury or illness or have to inflict pain during the course of treatment Physicians have to regulate their emotional response to such stimuli in order to effectively help the patient and maintain their own personal well being Pain empathy can motivate an individual to help someone who is in pain but repeated exposure to individuals in pain with no ability to regulate emotional arousal can cause distress One study sought to determine if physicians were different in their response to viewing painful stimuli Physicians and control subjects watched video clips of a stranger being poked with a needle into the hands or feet fMRI was used to measure the hemodynamic activity within the brain while viewing the painful stimuli The fMRI revealed that brain areas involved in the pain matrix somatosensory cortex anterior insula dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus dACC and the periaqueductal gray PAG were activated in the control subject population when viewing the needle penetration videos The physicians had activation of higher order executive functioning in the brain as shown by activation of the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortex both involved in self regulation and activation of the precentral superior parietal and temporoparietal junction involved with executive attention The physicians did not have activation of the anterior insula dorsal anterior cingulated nucleus dACC or the periaqueductal gray PAG The study concluded that physicians adapt to the healthcare environment by down regulating their automatic empathetic response to patients pain 26 Pain synesthesia editSynesthesia is a phenomenon that occurs when sensory information in one cognitive pathway causes a sensation through another cognitive pathway Pain synesthesia is a form of synesthesia that causes a person to experience pain when seeing pain empathetic eliciting stimuli The most common group who report pain synesthesia are patients with phantom limb syndrome 27 Cultural variation editEvidence from several studies points to cultural variation in pain empathy An experiment in which British participants and East Asian participants were shown a video of a needle puncturing a hand demonstrated such cultural variations 28 The results showed British subjects demonstrated more empathetic concern and higher levels of emotional experience to do with empathy compared to the East Asian participants References edit Jean Decety W I Ed 2009 The Social Neuroscience of Empathy MIT Press a b c d e f Hetu S Taschereau Dumouchel V Jackson P L 2012 Stimulating the brain to study social interactions and empathy Article Brain Stimulation 5 2 95 102 doi 10 1016 j brs 2012 03 005 PMID 22503509 S2CID 18850529 Reicherts P Wieser M J Gerdes A B M Likowski K U Weyers P Muhlberger A Pauli P 2012 Electrocortical evidence for preferential processing of dynamic pain expressions compared to other emotional expressions Article Pain 153 9 1959 1964 doi 10 1016 j pain 2012 06 017 PMID 22795483 S2CID 22450468 a b c d Singer T S B O Doherty J Kaube H Dolan RJ Frith CD 2004 Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain Science 303 5661 1157 1162 Bibcode 2004Sci 303 1157S doi 10 1126 science 1093535 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 A020 5 PMID 14976305 S2CID 14727944 Bingel U Quante M Knab R Bromm B Weiller C Buchel C 2003 Single trial fMRI reveals significant contralateral bias in responses to laser pain within thalamus and somatosensory cortices Article NeuroImage 18 3 740 748 doi 10 1016 s1053 8119 02 00033 2 PMID 12667851 S2CID 6291627 Motoyama Y Ogata K Hoka S amp Tobimatsu S 2010 P15 15 Neural substrates of empathy for pain Simultaneous recording of high density EEG and conventional ECG Clinical Neurophysiology 121 S194 doi 10 1016 s1388 2457 10 60798 5 S2CID 53144387 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Michael J amp Fardo F 2014 What If Anything Is Shared in Pain Empathy A Critical Discussion of De Vignemont and Jacob s Theory of the Neural Substrate of Pain Empathy Philosophy of Science 81 1 154 160 doi 10 1086 674203 hdl 2434 923844 S2CID 143192758 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Bufalari I Aprile T Avenanti A Di Russo F amp Aglioti S 2007 Empathy for Pain and Touch in the Human Somatosensory Cortex Cerebral Cortex 17 11 2553 2561 doi 10 1093 cercor bhl161 PMID 17205974 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Cheng Y Yang C Y Lin C P Lee P L amp Decety J 2008 The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations A magnetoencephalography study Pfurtscheller G Neuper Ch Andrew C Edlinger G 1997 06 01 Foot and hand area mu rhythms International Journal of Psychophysiology 26 1 121 135 doi 10 1016 S0167 8760 97 00760 5 ISSN 0167 8760 PMID 9202999 Moore Matthew R Franz Elizabeth A 2017 02 01 Mu rhythm suppression is associated with the classification of emotion in faces Cognitive Affective amp Behavioral Neuroscience 17 1 224 234 doi 10 3758 s13415 016 0476 6 ISSN 1531 135X PMID 27815729 S2CID 24027633 Joyal Christian C Neveu Sarah Michelle Boukhalfi Tarik Jackson Philip L Renaud Patrice 2018 Suppression of Sensorimotor Alpha Power Associated With Pain Expressed by an Avatar A Preliminary EEG Study Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12 273 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2018 00273 ISSN 1662 5161 PMC 6046452 PMID 30038564 Arnett Kirsten Roach Alexandra Elzy Meredith Jelsone Swain Laura 2019 03 04 Childhood emotional invalidation and right hemispheric mu suppression during a pain empathy task An EEG study Social Neuroscience 14 2 236 250 doi 10 1080 17470919 2018 1441905 ISSN 1747 0919 PMID 29451071 S2CID 3690092 a b Avenanti A Sirigu A Aglioti S M 2010 Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other race pain Current Biology 20 11 1018 1022 Bibcode 2010CBio 20 1018A doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 03 071 PMID 20537539 S2CID 7048253 a b Martins M J Moura B L Martins I P Figueira M L amp Prkachin K M 2011 Sensitivity to expressions of pain in schizophrenia patients Psychiatry Research 189 2 180 184 doi 10 1016 j psychres 2011 03 007 PMID 21470693 S2CID 26371301 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Wojakiewicz A Januel D Braha S Prkachin K Danziger N amp Bouhassira D 2013 Alteration of pain recognition in schizophrenia European Journal of Pain 17 9 1385 1392 doi 10 1002 j 1532 2149 2013 00310 x PMID 23529960 S2CID 32603486 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b c Gonzalez Liencres C Brown E C Tas C Breidenstein A amp Brune M 2016 Alterations in event related potential responses to empathy for pain in schizophrenia Psychiatry Research 241 14 21 doi 10 1016 j psychres 2016 04 091 PMID 27152905 S2CID 4769270 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Horan W Jimenez A Lee J Wynn J Eisenberger N amp Green M 2016 Pain empathy in schizophrenia An fMRI study Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11 5 154 160 doi 10 1093 scan nsw002 PMC 4847698 PMID 26746181 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Didehbani N Shad M U Tamminga C A Kandalaft M R Allen T T Chapman S B amp Krawczyk D C 2012 Insight and empathy in schizophrenia Schizophrenia Research 142 1 3 246 247 doi 10 1016 j schres 2012 09 010 PMID 23043874 S2CID 6863595 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Bonfils K A Lysaker P H Minor K S amp Salyers M P 2017 Empathy in schizophrenia A meta analysis of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index Psychiatry Research 249 293 303 doi 10 1016 j psychres 2016 12 033 hdl 1805 11989 PMID 28142103 S2CID 5043806 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Decety J Michalska K J Akitsuki Y Lahey B B 2009 Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder A functional MRI investigation Biological Psychology 80 2 203 211 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2008 09 004 PMC 2819310 PMID 18940230 Cheng Y W Hung A Y Decety J 2012 Dissociation between affective sharing and emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths Article Development and Psychopathology 24 2 623 636 doi 10 1017 s095457941200020x PMID 22559135 S2CID 37377507 a b Auyeung K amp Alden W 2016 Social Anxiety and Empathy for Social Pain Cognitive Therapy and Research 40 1 38 45 doi 10 1007 s10608 015 9718 0 hdl 2429 62699 S2CID 43695016 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Ruckman J Bodden M Jansen A Kircher T Dobel R amp Reif W 2015 How pain empathy depends on ingroup outgroup decisions A functional magnet resonance imaging study Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging 234 1 57 65 doi 10 1016 j pscychresns 2015 08 006 PMID 26323252 S2CID 7209426 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Feng C Li Z Feng X Wang L Tian T amp Luo Y 2016 Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11 3 485 495 doi 10 1093 scan nsv135 PMC 4769637 PMID 26516169 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Decety J Yang C Y Cheng Y W 2010 Physicians down regulate their pain empathy response An event related brain potential study Article NeuroImage 50 4 1676 1682 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2010 01 025 PMID 20080194 S2CID 6081243 Fitzgibbon B M Giummarra M J Georgiou Karistianis N Enticott P G Bradshaw J L 2010 Shared pain From empathy to synaesthesia Review Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 4 500 512 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2009 10 007 PMID 19857517 S2CID 14874873 Atkins D Uskul A amp Cooper N 2016 ulture shapes empathic responses to physical and social pain Emotion 16 5 587 601 doi 10 1037 emo0000162 PMC 4976796 PMID 26950365 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Pain empathy amp oldid 1221210988, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.