fbpx
Wikipedia

Magical thinking

Magical thinking, or superstitious thinking,[1] is the belief that unrelated events are causally connected despite the absence of any plausible causal link between them, particularly as a result of supernatural effects.[1][2][3] Examples include the idea that personal thoughts can influence the external world without acting on them, or that objects must be causally connected if they resemble each other or have come into contact with each other in the past.[1][2][4] Magical thinking is a type of fallacious thinking and is a common source of invalid causal inferences.[3][5] Unlike the confusion of correlation with causation, magical thinking does not require the events to be correlated.[3]

The precise definition of magical thinking may vary subtly when used by different theorists or among different fields of study. In anthropology, the posited causality is between religious ritual, prayer, sacrifice, or the observance of a taboo, and an expected benefit or recompense.

In psychology, magical thinking is the belief that one's thoughts by themselves can bring about effects in the world or that thinking something corresponds with doing it.[6] These beliefs can cause a person to experience an irrational fear of performing certain acts or having certain thoughts because of an assumed correlation between doing so and threatening calamities.[1]

In psychiatry, magical thinking defines false beliefs about the capability of thoughts, actions or words to cause or prevent undesirable events.[7] It is a commonly observed symptom in thought disorder, schizotypal personality disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder.[8][9][10]

Anthropology

In religion, folk religion, and superstitious beliefs, the posited causality is between religious ritual, prayer, meditation, trances, sacrifice, incantation, curses, benediction, faith healing, or the observance of a taboo, and an expected benefit or recompense. The use of a lucky charm or ritual, for example, is assumed to increase the probability that one will perform at a level so that one can achieve a desired goal or outcome.[11]

Researchers have identified two possible principles as the formal causes of the attribution of false causal relationships:

Prominent Victorian theorists identified associative thinking (a common feature of practitioners of magic) as a characteristic form of irrationality. As with all forms of magical thinking, association-based and similarities-based notions of causality are not always said to be the practice of magic by a magician. For example, the doctrine of signatures held that similarities between plant parts and body parts indicated their efficacy in treating diseases of those body parts, and was a part of Western medicine during the Middle Ages. This association-based thinking is a vivid example of the general human application of the representativeness heuristic.[12]

Edward Burnett Tylor coined the term "associative thinking",[13] characterizing it as pre-logical,[citation needed] in which the "magician's folly" is in mistaking an imagined connection with a real one. The magician believes that thematically linked items can influence one another by virtue of their similarity.[14] For example, in E. E. Evans-Pritchard's account, members of the Azande tribe[15] believe that rubbing crocodile teeth on banana plants can invoke a fruitful crop. Because crocodile teeth are curved (like bananas) and grow back if they fall out, the Azande observe this similarity and want to impart this capacity of regeneration to their bananas. To them, the rubbing constitutes a means of transference.

Sir James Frazer (1854–1941) elaborated upon Tylor's principle by dividing magic into the categories of sympathetic and contagious magic. The latter is based upon the law of contagion or contact, in which two things that were once connected retain this link and have the ability to affect their supposedly related objects, such as harming a person by harming a lock of his hair. Sympathetic magic and homeopathy operate upon the premise that "like affects like", or that one can impart characteristics of one object to a similar object. Frazer believed that some individuals think the entire world functions according to these mimetic, or homeopathic, principles.[16]

In How Natives Think (1925), Lucien Lévy-Bruhl describes a similar notion of mystical, "collective representations". He too sees magical thinking as fundamentally different from a Western style of thought. He asserts that in these representations, "primitive" people's "mental activity is too little differentiated for it to be possible to consider ideas or images of objects by themselves apart from the emotions and passions which evoke those ideas or are evoked by them".[17] Lévy-Bruhl explains that the indigenous people commit the post hoc, ergo propter hoc fallacy, in which people observe that x is followed by y, and conclude that x has caused y.[18] He believes that this fallacy is institutionalized in native culture and is committed regularly and repeatedly.

Despite the view that magic is less than rational and entails an inferior concept of causality, in The Savage Mind (1966), Claude Lévi-Strauss suggested that magical procedures are relatively effective in exerting control over the environment. This outlook has generated alternative theories of magical thinking, such as the symbolic and psychological approaches, and softened the contrast between "educated" and "primitive" thinking: "Magical thinking is no less characteristic of our own mundane intellectual activity than it is of Zande curing practices."[19][n 1]

Types

Direct effect

Bronisław Malinowski's Magic, Science and Religion (1954) discusses another type of magical thinking, in which words and sounds are thought to have the ability to directly affect the world.[20] This type of wish fulfillment thinking can result in the avoidance of talking about certain subjects ("speak of the devil and he'll appear"), the use of euphemisms instead of certain words, or the belief that to know the "true name" of something gives one power over it, or that certain chants, prayers, or mystical phrases will bring about physical changes in the world. More generally, it is magical thinking to take a symbol to be its referent or an analogy to represent an identity.

Sigmund Freud believed that magical thinking was produced by cognitive developmental factors. He described practitioners of magic as projecting their mental states onto the world around them, similar to a common phase in child development.[21] From toddlerhood to early school age, children will often link the outside world with their internal consciousness, e.g. "It is raining because I am sad."

Symbolic approaches

Another theory of magical thinking is the symbolic approach. Leading thinkers of this category, including Stanley J. Tambiah, believe that magic is meant to be expressive, rather than instrumental. As opposed to the direct, mimetic thinking of Frazer, Tambiah asserts that magic utilizes abstract analogies to express a desired state, along the lines of metonymy or metaphor.[22]

An important question raised by this interpretation is how mere symbols could exert material effects. One possible answer lies in John L. Austin's concept of performativity, in which the act of saying something makes it true, such as in an inaugural or marital rite.[23] Other theories propose that magic is effective because symbols are able to affect internal psycho-physical states. They claim that the act of expressing a certain anxiety or desire can be reparative in itself.[24]

Causes

 
A healing ritual (the laying on of hands)

According to theories of anxiety relief and control, people turn to magical beliefs when there exists a sense of uncertainty and potential danger, and with little access to logical or scientific responses to such danger. Magic is used to restore a sense of control over circumstance. In support of this theory, research indicates that superstitious behavior is invoked more often in high stress situations, especially by people with a greater desire for control.[25][26]

Another potential reason for the persistence of magic rituals is that the rituals prompt their own use by creating a feeling of insecurity and then proposing themselves as precautions.[27] Boyer and Liénard propose that in obsessive-compulsive rituals — a possible clinical model for certain forms of magical thinking — focus shifts to the lowest level of gestures, resulting in goal demotion. For example, an obsessive-compulsive cleaning ritual may overemphasize the order, direction, and number of wipes used to clean the surface. The goal becomes less important than the actions used to achieve the goal, with the implication that magic rituals can persist without efficacy because the intent is lost within the act.[27] Alternatively, some cases of harmless "rituals" may have positive effects in bolstering intent, as may be the case with certain pre-game exercises in sports.[28]

Some scholars believe that magic is effective psychologically. They cite the placebo effect and psychosomatic disease as prime examples of how our mental functions exert power over our bodies.[29] Similarly, Robin Horton suggests that engaging in magical practices surrounding healing can relieve anxiety, which could have a significant positive physical effect. In the absence of advanced health care, such effects would play a relatively major role, thereby helping to explain the persistence and popularity of such practices.[30][31]

Phenomenological approach

Ariel Glucklich tries to understand magic from a subjective perspective, attempting to comprehend magic on a phenomenological, experientially based level. Glucklich seeks to describe the attitude that magical practitioners feel what he calls "magical consciousness" or the "magical experience". He explains that it is based upon "the awareness of the interrelatedness of all things in the world by means of simple but refined sense perception."[32]

Another phenomenological model is that of Gilbert Lewis, who argues that "habit is unthinking". He believes that those practicing magic do not think of an explanatory theory behind their actions any more than the average person tries to grasp the pharmaceutical workings of aspirin.[33] When the average person takes an aspirin, he does not know how the medicine chemically functions. He takes the pill with the premise that there is proof of efficacy. Similarly, many who avail themselves of magic do so without feeling the need to understand a causal theory behind it.

Cultural differences

Robin Horton maintains that the difference between the thinking of Western and of non-Western peoples is predominantly "idiomatic". He says that the members of both cultures use the same practical common-sense, and that both science and magic are ways beyond basic logic by which people formulate theories to explain whatever occurs. However, non-Western cultures use the idiom of magic and have community spiritual figures, and therefore non-Westerners turn to magical practices or to a specialist in that idiom. Horton sees the same logic and common-sense in all cultures, but notes that their contrasting ontological idioms lead to cultural practices which seem illogical to observers whose own culture has correspondingly contrasting norms. He explains, "[T]he layman's grounds for accepting the models propounded by the scientist are often no different from the young African villager's ground for accepting the models propounded by one of his elders."[34] Along similar lines, Michael F. Brown argues that the Aguaruna of Peru see magic as a type of technology, no more supernatural than their physical tools. Brown says that the Aguaruna utilize magic in an empirical manner; for example, they discard any magical stones which they have found to be ineffective. To Brown—as to Horton—magical and scientific thinking differ merely in idiom.[35] These theories blur the boundaries between magic, science, and religion, and focus on the similarities in magical, technical, and spiritual practices. Brown even ironically writes that he is tempted to disclaim the existence of 'magic.'[36]

One theory of substantive difference is that of the open versus closed society. Horton describes this as one of the key dissimilarities between traditional thought and Western science. He suggests that the scientific worldview is distinguished from a magical one by the scientific method and by skepticism, requiring the falsifiability of any scientific hypothesis. He notes that for native peoples "there is no developed awareness of alternatives to the established body of theoretical texts."[37] He notes that all further differences between traditional and Western thought can be understood as a result of this factor. He says that because there are no alternatives in societies based on magical thought, a theory does not need to be objectively judged to be valid.

In children

According to Jean Piaget's Theory of Cognitive Development,[38] magical thinking is most prominent in children between ages 2 and 7. Due to examinations of grieving children, it is said that during this age, children strongly believe that their personal thoughts have a direct effect on the rest of the world. It is posited that their minds will create a reason to feel responsible if they experience something tragic that they do not understand, e.g. a death. Jean Piaget, a developmental psychologist, came up with a theory of four developmental stages. Children between ages 2 and 7 would be classified under his preoperational stage of development. During this stage children are still developing their use of logical thinking. A child's thinking is dominated by perceptions of physical features, meaning that if the child is told that a family pet has "gone away to a farm" when it has in fact died, then the child will have difficulty comprehending the transformation of the dog not being around anymore. Magical thinking would be evident here, since the child may believe that the family pet being gone is just temporary. Their young minds in this stage do not understand the finality of death and magical thinking may bridge the gap.

Grief

It was discovered that children often feel that they are responsible for an event or events occurring or are capable of reversing an event simply by thinking about it and wishing for a change: namely, "magical thinking".[39] Make-believe and fantasy are an integral part of life at this age and are often used to explain the inexplicable.[40][41]

According to Piaget, children within this age group are often "egocentric", believing that what they feel and experience is the same as everyone else's feelings and experiences.[42] Also at this age, there is often a lack of ability to understand that there may be other explanations for events outside of the realm of things they have already comprehended. What happens outside their understanding needs to be explained using what they already know, because of an inability to fully comprehend abstract concepts.[42]

Magical thinking is found particularly in children's explanations of experiences about death, whether the death of a family member or pet, or their own illness or impending death. These experiences are often new for a young child, who at that point has no experience to give understanding of the ramifications of the event.[43] A child may feel that they are responsible for what has happened, simply because they were upset with the person who died, or perhaps played with the pet too roughly. There may also be the idea that if the child wishes it hard enough, or performs just the right act, the person or pet may choose to come back, and not be dead any longer.[44] When considering their own illness or impending death, some children may feel that they are being punished for doing something wrong, or not doing something they should have, and therefore have become ill.[45] If a child's ideas about an event are incorrect because of their magical thinking, there is a possibility that the conclusions the child makes could result in long-term beliefs and behaviours that create difficulty for the child as they mature.[46]

Related terms

"Quasi-magical thinking" describes "cases in which people act as if they erroneously believe that their action influences the outcome, even though they do not really hold that belief".[47] People may realize that a superstitious intuition is logically false, but act as if it were true because they do not exert an effort to correct the intuition.[48]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ The Azande practice of curing epilepsy by eating the burnt skull of a red bush monkey, based on the apparent similarity of epileptic movements and those of the monkeys, was discussed in Evans-Pritchard 1937, p. 487.

References

  1. ^ a b c d Bennett, Bo. "Magical Thinking". Logically Fallacious. Retrieved 20 May 2020.
  2. ^ a b Carroll RT (12 Sep 2014). "Magical thinking". The Skeptic's Dictionary. Retrieved 20 May 2020.
  3. ^ a b c Robert J. Sternberg; Henry L. Roediger III; Diane F. Halpern (2007). Critical Thinking in Psychology. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-60834-3.
  4. ^ Vamos, Marina (2010). "Organ transplantation and magical thinking". Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 44 (10): 883–887. doi:10.3109/00048674.2010.498786. ISSN 0004-8674. PMID 20932201. S2CID 25440192.
  5. ^ Carhart-Harris, R. (2013). "Psychedelic drugs, magical thinking and psychosis". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 84 (9): e1. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2013-306103.17. ISSN 0022-3050.
  6. ^ Colman, Andrew M. (2012). A Dictionary of Psychology (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
  7. ^ American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. pp. 655, 824. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596. ISBN 978-0-89042-554-1.
  8. ^ Sadock, B. J.; Sadock, V. A.; Ruiz, P. (2017). Kaplan and Sadock's Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry (10th ed.). Wolters Kluwer. ISBN 978-1-4511-0047-1.
  9. ^ Fonseca-Pedrero E, Ortuno J, Debbané M, Chan E, Cicero D, Zhang L, Brenner C, Barkus E, Linscott E, Kwapil T, Barrantes-Vidal N, Cohen A, Raine A, Compton M, Tone E, Suhr J, Inchausti F, Bobes J, Fumero A, Giakoumaki S, Tsaousis I, Preti A, Chmielewski M, Laloyaux J, Mechri A, Lahmar M, Wuthrich V, Laroi F, Badcock J, Jablensky A, Isvoranu A, Epskamp S, Fried E (2018). "The network structure of schizotypal personality traits". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 44 (2): 468–479. doi:10.1093/schbul/sby044. PMC 6188518. PMID 29684178.
  10. ^ Barkataki B (2019). Explaining obsessive-compulsive symptoms? A transcultural exploration of magical thinking and OCD in India and Australia (PhD). Curtin university.
  11. ^ Hamerman, Eric J.; Morewedge, Carey K. (2015-03-01). "Reliance on luck identifying which achievement goals elicit superstitious behavior". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 41 (3): 323–335. doi:10.1177/0146167214565055. PMID 25617118. S2CID 1160061.
  12. ^ Nisbett, D.; Ross, L. (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. pp. 115–8.
  13. ^ Glucklich, Ariel (1997). The End of Magic. Oxford University Press. pp. 32–3.
  14. ^ Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1977). Theories of Primitive Religion. Oxford University Press. pp. 26–7.
  15. ^ Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, Magic, and Oracles Among the Azande. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  16. ^ Frazer, James (1915) [1911]. The Golden Bough: A Study in Magic and Religion (3rd ed.). London: Macmillan.
  17. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1925). How Natives Think. Knopf. p. 36.
  18. ^ Lévy-Bruhl 1925, p. 76
  19. ^ Shweder, Richard A. (1977). "Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: Magical thinking in judgments about personality". Current Anthropology. 18 (4): 637–58 (637). doi:10.1086/201974. JSTOR 2741505. S2CID 29780746.
  20. ^ Glucklich 1997, pp. 59–61, 205–12
  21. ^ Glucklich 1997, pp. 53–5
  22. ^ Brown, Michael F. (1993). Thinking About Magic. Greenwood Press. pp. 5–7.
  23. ^ Glucklich 1997, pp. 60–2
  24. ^ Glucklich 1997, pp. 49–53
  25. ^ Keinan, Giora (2002). "The effects of stress and desire for control on superstitious behavior". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (1): 102–108. doi:10.1177/0146167202281009. S2CID 145223253.
  26. ^ Keinan, Giora (1994). "The effects of stress and tolerance of ambiguity on magical thinking". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (1): 48–55. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.48.
  27. ^ a b Boyer, Pascal; Liénard, Pierre (2008). "Ritual behavior in obsessive and normal individuals". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (4): 291–94. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.503.1537. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00592.x. S2CID 145218875.
  28. ^ "Why Rituals Work". Scientific American. Retrieved 2015-12-17.
  29. ^ Glucklich 1997, pp. 50–68
  30. ^ Horton, Robin (1967). "African traditional thought and western science: Part I. From tradition to science". Africa: Journal of the International African Institute. 37 (1): 50–71. doi:10.2307/1157195. JSTOR 1157195. S2CID 145507695.
  31. ^ Horton, Robin (1967). "African traditional thought and western science: Part II. The 'closed' and 'open' predicaments". Africa: Journal of the International African Institute. 37 (2): 155–87. doi:10.2307/1158253. JSTOR 1158253. S2CID 245911255.
  32. ^ Glucklich 1997, p. 12
  33. ^ Lewis, Gilbert. The Look of Magic. University of Cambridge.
  34. ^ Horton 1967b, p. 171
  35. ^ Brown, Michael F. (1986). Tsewa's Gift: Magic and Meaning in an Amazonian Society. University of Alabama Press.
  36. ^ Brown 1993, p. 2
  37. ^ Horton 1967b, p. 155
  38. ^ Piaget, Jean (1929). The child's conception of the world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  39. ^ Nielson, D. (2012). "Discussing death with pediatric patients: Implications for nurses". Journal of Pediatric Nursing. 27 (5): e59–e64. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2011.11.006. PMID 22198004.
  40. ^ Samide, L.; Stockton, R. (2002). "Letting go of grief: Bereavement groups for children in the school setting". Journal for Specialists in Group Work. 27 (2): 192–204. doi:10.1177/0193392202027002006.
  41. ^ Webb, N. (2010). "The child and death". In Webb, N.B. (ed.). Helping Bereaved Children: A Handbook for Practitioners. New York: Guildford. pp. 5–6.
  42. ^ a b Biank, N.; Werner-Lin, A. (2011). "Growing up with grief: Revisiting the death of a parent over the life course". Omega. 63 (3): 271–290. doi:10.2190/om.63.3.e. PMID 21928600. S2CID 37763796.
  43. ^ Webb 2010, p. 51
  44. ^ Schoen, A.; Burgoyen, M.; Schoen, S. (2004). "Are the developmental needs of children in America adequately addressed during the grief process?". Journal of Instructional Psychology. 31: 143–8. EBSCOhost 13719052[dead link].
  45. ^ Schonfeld, D. (1993). "Talking with children about death". Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 7 (6): 269–74. doi:10.1016/s0891-5245(06)80008-8. PMID 8106926.
  46. ^ Sossin, K.; Cohen, P. (2011). "Children's play in the wake of loss and trauma". Journal of Infant, Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy. 10 (2–3): 255–72. doi:10.1080/15289168.2011.600137. S2CID 146429165.
  47. ^ Shafir, E.; Tversky, A. (1992). "Thinking through uncertainty: Nonconsequential reasoning and choice". Cognitive Psychology. 24 (4): 449–74. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90015-T. PMID 1473331. S2CID 29570235.
  48. ^ Risen, Jane L. (2016). "Believing what we do not believe: Acquiescence to superstitious beliefs and other powerful intuitions". Psychological Review. 123 (2): 182–207. doi:10.1037/rev0000017. PMID 26479707. S2CID 14384232.

Further reading

External links

  • Hutson, Matthew (2008). "Magical thinking". Psychology Today. Vol. March–April. pp. 89–95.
  • Stevens, Phillips Jr. (November–December 2001). . Skeptical Inquirer. 25 (6). Archived from the original on 2010-06-03. Retrieved 2010-09-22.

magical, thinking, other, uses, disambiguation, superstitious, thinking, belief, that, unrelated, events, causally, connected, despite, absence, plausible, causal, link, between, them, particularly, result, supernatural, effects, examples, include, idea, that,. For other uses see Magical thinking disambiguation Magical thinking or superstitious thinking 1 is the belief that unrelated events are causally connected despite the absence of any plausible causal link between them particularly as a result of supernatural effects 1 2 3 Examples include the idea that personal thoughts can influence the external world without acting on them or that objects must be causally connected if they resemble each other or have come into contact with each other in the past 1 2 4 Magical thinking is a type of fallacious thinking and is a common source of invalid causal inferences 3 5 Unlike the confusion of correlation with causation magical thinking does not require the events to be correlated 3 The precise definition of magical thinking may vary subtly when used by different theorists or among different fields of study In anthropology the posited causality is between religious ritual prayer sacrifice or the observance of a taboo and an expected benefit or recompense In psychology magical thinking is the belief that one s thoughts by themselves can bring about effects in the world or that thinking something corresponds with doing it 6 These beliefs can cause a person to experience an irrational fear of performing certain acts or having certain thoughts because of an assumed correlation between doing so and threatening calamities 1 In psychiatry magical thinking defines false beliefs about the capability of thoughts actions or words to cause or prevent undesirable events 7 It is a commonly observed symptom in thought disorder schizotypal personality disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder 8 9 10 Contents 1 Anthropology 2 Types 2 1 Direct effect 2 2 Symbolic approaches 3 Causes 3 1 Phenomenological approach 4 Cultural differences 5 In children 5 1 Grief 6 Related terms 7 See also 8 Notes 9 References 10 Further reading 11 External linksAnthropology EditIn religion folk religion and superstitious beliefs the posited causality is between religious ritual prayer meditation trances sacrifice incantation curses benediction faith healing or the observance of a taboo and an expected benefit or recompense The use of a lucky charm or ritual for example is assumed to increase the probability that one will perform at a level so that one can achieve a desired goal or outcome 11 Researchers have identified two possible principles as the formal causes of the attribution of false causal relationships the temporal contiguity of two events associative thinking the association of entities based upon their resemblance to one anotherProminent Victorian theorists identified associative thinking a common feature of practitioners of magic as a characteristic form of irrationality As with all forms of magical thinking association based and similarities based notions of causality are not always said to be the practice of magic by a magician For example the doctrine of signatures held that similarities between plant parts and body parts indicated their efficacy in treating diseases of those body parts and was a part of Western medicine during the Middle Ages This association based thinking is a vivid example of the general human application of the representativeness heuristic 12 Edward Burnett Tylor coined the term associative thinking 13 characterizing it as pre logical citation needed in which the magician s folly is in mistaking an imagined connection with a real one The magician believes that thematically linked items can influence one another by virtue of their similarity 14 For example in E E Evans Pritchard s account members of the Azande tribe 15 believe that rubbing crocodile teeth on banana plants can invoke a fruitful crop Because crocodile teeth are curved like bananas and grow back if they fall out the Azande observe this similarity and want to impart this capacity of regeneration to their bananas To them the rubbing constitutes a means of transference Sir James Frazer 1854 1941 elaborated upon Tylor s principle by dividing magic into the categories of sympathetic and contagious magic The latter is based upon the law of contagion or contact in which two things that were once connected retain this link and have the ability to affect their supposedly related objects such as harming a person by harming a lock of his hair Sympathetic magic and homeopathy operate upon the premise that like affects like or that one can impart characteristics of one object to a similar object Frazer believed that some individuals think the entire world functions according to these mimetic or homeopathic principles 16 In How Natives Think 1925 Lucien Levy Bruhl describes a similar notion of mystical collective representations He too sees magical thinking as fundamentally different from a Western style of thought He asserts that in these representations primitive people s mental activity is too little differentiated for it to be possible to consider ideas or images of objects by themselves apart from the emotions and passions which evoke those ideas or are evoked by them 17 Levy Bruhl explains that the indigenous people commit the post hoc ergo propter hoc fallacy in which people observe that x is followed by y and conclude that x has caused y 18 He believes that this fallacy is institutionalized in native culture and is committed regularly and repeatedly Despite the view that magic is less than rational and entails an inferior concept of causality in The Savage Mind 1966 Claude Levi Strauss suggested that magical procedures are relatively effective in exerting control over the environment This outlook has generated alternative theories of magical thinking such as the symbolic and psychological approaches and softened the contrast between educated and primitive thinking Magical thinking is no less characteristic of our own mundane intellectual activity than it is of Zande curing practices 19 n 1 Types EditDirect effect Edit Bronislaw Malinowski s Magic Science and Religion 1954 discusses another type of magical thinking in which words and sounds are thought to have the ability to directly affect the world 20 This type of wish fulfillment thinking can result in the avoidance of talking about certain subjects speak of the devil and he ll appear the use of euphemisms instead of certain words or the belief that to know the true name of something gives one power over it or that certain chants prayers or mystical phrases will bring about physical changes in the world More generally it is magical thinking to take a symbol to be its referent or an analogy to represent an identity Sigmund Freud believed that magical thinking was produced by cognitive developmental factors He described practitioners of magic as projecting their mental states onto the world around them similar to a common phase in child development 21 From toddlerhood to early school age children will often link the outside world with their internal consciousness e g It is raining because I am sad Symbolic approaches Edit Another theory of magical thinking is the symbolic approach Leading thinkers of this category including Stanley J Tambiah believe that magic is meant to be expressive rather than instrumental As opposed to the direct mimetic thinking of Frazer Tambiah asserts that magic utilizes abstract analogies to express a desired state along the lines of metonymy or metaphor 22 An important question raised by this interpretation is how mere symbols could exert material effects One possible answer lies in John L Austin s concept of performativity in which the act of saying something makes it true such as in an inaugural or marital rite 23 Other theories propose that magic is effective because symbols are able to affect internal psycho physical states They claim that the act of expressing a certain anxiety or desire can be reparative in itself 24 Causes Edit A healing ritual the laying on of hands According to theories of anxiety relief and control people turn to magical beliefs when there exists a sense of uncertainty and potential danger and with little access to logical or scientific responses to such danger Magic is used to restore a sense of control over circumstance In support of this theory research indicates that superstitious behavior is invoked more often in high stress situations especially by people with a greater desire for control 25 26 Another potential reason for the persistence of magic rituals is that the rituals prompt their own use by creating a feeling of insecurity and then proposing themselves as precautions 27 Boyer and Lienard propose that in obsessive compulsive rituals a possible clinical model for certain forms of magical thinking focus shifts to the lowest level of gestures resulting in goal demotion For example an obsessive compulsive cleaning ritual may overemphasize the order direction and number of wipes used to clean the surface The goal becomes less important than the actions used to achieve the goal with the implication that magic rituals can persist without efficacy because the intent is lost within the act 27 Alternatively some cases of harmless rituals may have positive effects in bolstering intent as may be the case with certain pre game exercises in sports 28 Some scholars believe that magic is effective psychologically They cite the placebo effect and psychosomatic disease as prime examples of how our mental functions exert power over our bodies 29 Similarly Robin Horton suggests that engaging in magical practices surrounding healing can relieve anxiety which could have a significant positive physical effect In the absence of advanced health care such effects would play a relatively major role thereby helping to explain the persistence and popularity of such practices 30 31 Phenomenological approach Edit Ariel Glucklich tries to understand magic from a subjective perspective attempting to comprehend magic on a phenomenological experientially based level Glucklich seeks to describe the attitude that magical practitioners feel what he calls magical consciousness or the magical experience He explains that it is based upon the awareness of the interrelatedness of all things in the world by means of simple but refined sense perception 32 Another phenomenological model is that of Gilbert Lewis who argues that habit is unthinking He believes that those practicing magic do not think of an explanatory theory behind their actions any more than the average person tries to grasp the pharmaceutical workings of aspirin 33 When the average person takes an aspirin he does not know how the medicine chemically functions He takes the pill with the premise that there is proof of efficacy Similarly many who avail themselves of magic do so without feeling the need to understand a causal theory behind it Cultural differences EditRobin Horton maintains that the difference between the thinking of Western and of non Western peoples is predominantly idiomatic He says that the members of both cultures use the same practical common sense and that both science and magic are ways beyond basic logic by which people formulate theories to explain whatever occurs However non Western cultures use the idiom of magic and have community spiritual figures and therefore non Westerners turn to magical practices or to a specialist in that idiom Horton sees the same logic and common sense in all cultures but notes that their contrasting ontological idioms lead to cultural practices which seem illogical to observers whose own culture has correspondingly contrasting norms He explains T he layman s grounds for accepting the models propounded by the scientist are often no different from the young African villager s ground for accepting the models propounded by one of his elders 34 Along similar lines Michael F Brown argues that the Aguaruna of Peru see magic as a type of technology no more supernatural than their physical tools Brown says that the Aguaruna utilize magic in an empirical manner for example they discard any magical stones which they have found to be ineffective To Brown as to Horton magical and scientific thinking differ merely in idiom 35 These theories blur the boundaries between magic science and religion and focus on the similarities in magical technical and spiritual practices Brown even ironically writes that he is tempted to disclaim the existence of magic 36 One theory of substantive difference is that of the open versus closed society Horton describes this as one of the key dissimilarities between traditional thought and Western science He suggests that the scientific worldview is distinguished from a magical one by the scientific method and by skepticism requiring the falsifiability of any scientific hypothesis He notes that for native peoples there is no developed awareness of alternatives to the established body of theoretical texts 37 He notes that all further differences between traditional and Western thought can be understood as a result of this factor He says that because there are no alternatives in societies based on magical thought a theory does not need to be objectively judged to be valid In children EditAccording to Jean Piaget s Theory of Cognitive Development 38 magical thinking is most prominent in children between ages 2 and 7 Due to examinations of grieving children it is said that during this age children strongly believe that their personal thoughts have a direct effect on the rest of the world It is posited that their minds will create a reason to feel responsible if they experience something tragic that they do not understand e g a death Jean Piaget a developmental psychologist came up with a theory of four developmental stages Children between ages 2 and 7 would be classified under his preoperational stage of development During this stage children are still developing their use of logical thinking A child s thinking is dominated by perceptions of physical features meaning that if the child is told that a family pet has gone away to a farm when it has in fact died then the child will have difficulty comprehending the transformation of the dog not being around anymore Magical thinking would be evident here since the child may believe that the family pet being gone is just temporary Their young minds in this stage do not understand the finality of death and magical thinking may bridge the gap Grief Edit It was discovered that children often feel that they are responsible for an event or events occurring or are capable of reversing an event simply by thinking about it and wishing for a change namely magical thinking 39 Make believe and fantasy are an integral part of life at this age and are often used to explain the inexplicable 40 41 According to Piaget children within this age group are often egocentric believing that what they feel and experience is the same as everyone else s feelings and experiences 42 Also at this age there is often a lack of ability to understand that there may be other explanations for events outside of the realm of things they have already comprehended What happens outside their understanding needs to be explained using what they already know because of an inability to fully comprehend abstract concepts 42 Magical thinking is found particularly in children s explanations of experiences about death whether the death of a family member or pet or their own illness or impending death These experiences are often new for a young child who at that point has no experience to give understanding of the ramifications of the event 43 A child may feel that they are responsible for what has happened simply because they were upset with the person who died or perhaps played with the pet too roughly There may also be the idea that if the child wishes it hard enough or performs just the right act the person or pet may choose to come back and not be dead any longer 44 When considering their own illness or impending death some children may feel that they are being punished for doing something wrong or not doing something they should have and therefore have become ill 45 If a child s ideas about an event are incorrect because of their magical thinking there is a possibility that the conclusions the child makes could result in long term beliefs and behaviours that create difficulty for the child as they mature 46 Related terms Edit Quasi magical thinking describes cases in which people act as if they erroneously believe that their action influences the outcome even though they do not really hold that belief 47 People may realize that a superstitious intuition is logically false but act as if it were true because they do not exert an effort to correct the intuition 48 See also EditCognitive bias Faith Illusion of control Law of attraction New Thought Mythopoeic thought Synchronicity Tinkerbell effect The Year of Magical Thinking an account of how mourning the death of a spouse led to magical thinking Notes Edit The Azande practice of curing epilepsy by eating the burnt skull of a red bush monkey based on the apparent similarity of epileptic movements and those of the monkeys was discussed in Evans Pritchard 1937 p 487 References Edit a b c d Bennett Bo Magical Thinking Logically Fallacious Retrieved 20 May 2020 a b Carroll RT 12 Sep 2014 Magical thinking The Skeptic s Dictionary Retrieved 20 May 2020 a b c Robert J Sternberg Henry L Roediger III Diane F Halpern 2007 Critical Thinking in Psychology Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 60834 3 Vamos Marina 2010 Organ transplantation and magical thinking Australian amp New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 44 10 883 887 doi 10 3109 00048674 2010 498786 ISSN 0004 8674 PMID 20932201 S2CID 25440192 Carhart Harris R 2013 Psychedelic drugs magical thinking and psychosis Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery amp Psychiatry 84 9 e1 doi 10 1136 jnnp 2013 306103 17 ISSN 0022 3050 Colman Andrew M 2012 A Dictionary of Psychology 3rd ed Oxford University Press American Psychiatric Association 2013 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition DSM 5 Arlington VA American Psychiatric Publishing pp 655 824 doi 10 1176 appi books 9780890425596 ISBN 978 0 89042 554 1 Sadock B J Sadock V A Ruiz P 2017 Kaplan and Sadock s Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry 10th ed Wolters Kluwer ISBN 978 1 4511 0047 1 Fonseca Pedrero E Ortuno J Debbane M Chan E Cicero D Zhang L Brenner C Barkus E Linscott E Kwapil T Barrantes Vidal N Cohen A Raine A Compton M Tone E Suhr J Inchausti F Bobes J Fumero A Giakoumaki S Tsaousis I Preti A Chmielewski M Laloyaux J Mechri A Lahmar M Wuthrich V Laroi F Badcock J Jablensky A Isvoranu A Epskamp S Fried E 2018 The network structure of schizotypal personality traits Schizophrenia Bulletin 44 2 468 479 doi 10 1093 schbul sby044 PMC 6188518 PMID 29684178 Barkataki B 2019 Explaining obsessive compulsive symptoms A transcultural exploration of magical thinking and OCD in India and Australia PhD Curtin university Hamerman Eric J Morewedge Carey K 2015 03 01 Reliance on luck identifying which achievement goals elicit superstitious behavior Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41 3 323 335 doi 10 1177 0146167214565055 PMID 25617118 S2CID 1160061 Nisbett D Ross L 1980 Human Inference Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment Englewood Cliffs NJ Prentice Hall pp 115 8 Glucklich Ariel 1997 The End of Magic Oxford University Press pp 32 3 Evans Pritchard E E 1977 Theories of Primitive Religion Oxford University Press pp 26 7 Evans Pritchard E E 1937 Witchcraft Magic and Oracles Among the Azande Oxford Clarendon Press Frazer James 1915 1911 The Golden Bough A Study in Magic and Religion 3rd ed London Macmillan Levy Bruhl Lucien 1925 How Natives Think Knopf p 36 Levy Bruhl 1925 p 76 Shweder Richard A 1977 Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought Magical thinking in judgments about personality Current Anthropology 18 4 637 58 637 doi 10 1086 201974 JSTOR 2741505 S2CID 29780746 Glucklich 1997 pp 59 61 205 12 Glucklich 1997 pp 53 5 Brown Michael F 1993 Thinking About Magic Greenwood Press pp 5 7 Glucklich 1997 pp 60 2 Glucklich 1997 pp 49 53 Keinan Giora 2002 The effects of stress and desire for control on superstitious behavior Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 1 102 108 doi 10 1177 0146167202281009 S2CID 145223253 Keinan Giora 1994 The effects of stress and tolerance of ambiguity on magical thinking Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 1 48 55 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 67 1 48 a b Boyer Pascal Lienard Pierre 2008 Ritual behavior in obsessive and normal individuals Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 4 291 94 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 503 1537 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2008 00592 x S2CID 145218875 Why Rituals Work Scientific American Retrieved 2015 12 17 Glucklich 1997 pp 50 68 Horton Robin 1967 African traditional thought and western science Part I From tradition to science Africa Journal of the International African Institute 37 1 50 71 doi 10 2307 1157195 JSTOR 1157195 S2CID 145507695 Horton Robin 1967 African traditional thought and western science Part II The closed and open predicaments Africa Journal of the International African Institute 37 2 155 87 doi 10 2307 1158253 JSTOR 1158253 S2CID 245911255 Glucklich 1997 p 12 Lewis Gilbert The Look of Magic University of Cambridge Horton 1967b p 171 Brown Michael F 1986 Tsewa s Gift Magic and Meaning in an Amazonian Society University of Alabama Press Brown 1993 p 2 Horton 1967b p 155 Piaget Jean 1929 The child s conception of the world London Routledge amp Kegan Paul Nielson D 2012 Discussing death with pediatric patients Implications for nurses Journal of Pediatric Nursing 27 5 e59 e64 doi 10 1016 j pedn 2011 11 006 PMID 22198004 Samide L Stockton R 2002 Letting go of grief Bereavement groups for children in the school setting Journal for Specialists in Group Work 27 2 192 204 doi 10 1177 0193392202027002006 Webb N 2010 The child and death In Webb N B ed Helping Bereaved Children A Handbook for Practitioners New York Guildford pp 5 6 a b Biank N Werner Lin A 2011 Growing up with grief Revisiting the death of a parent over the life course Omega 63 3 271 290 doi 10 2190 om 63 3 e PMID 21928600 S2CID 37763796 Webb 2010 p 51 Schoen A Burgoyen M Schoen S 2004 Are the developmental needs of children in America adequately addressed during the grief process Journal of Instructional Psychology 31 143 8 EBSCOhost 13719052 dead link Schonfeld D 1993 Talking with children about death Journal of Pediatric Health Care 7 6 269 74 doi 10 1016 s0891 5245 06 80008 8 PMID 8106926 Sossin K Cohen P 2011 Children s play in the wake of loss and trauma Journal of Infant Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy 10 2 3 255 72 doi 10 1080 15289168 2011 600137 S2CID 146429165 Shafir E Tversky A 1992 Thinking through uncertainty Nonconsequential reasoning and choice Cognitive Psychology 24 4 449 74 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 92 90015 T PMID 1473331 S2CID 29570235 Risen Jane L 2016 Believing what we do not believe Acquiescence to superstitious beliefs and other powerful intuitions Psychological Review 123 2 182 207 doi 10 1037 rev0000017 PMID 26479707 S2CID 14384232 Further reading EditHood Bruce 2009 SuperSense Why We Believe in the Unbelievable HarperOne ISBN 9780061452642 Horton Robin 1970 African traditional thought and western science In Wilson Bryan R ed Rationality Key Concepts in the Social Sciences Oxford Basil Blackwell pp 131 171 ISBN 9780631119302 Abridged version of Horton 1967a and Horton 1967b Hutson Matthew 2012 The 7 Laws of Magical Thinking How Irrational Beliefs Keep Us Happy Healthy and Sane Hudson Street Press ISBN 9781594630873 Serban George 1982 The Tyranny of Magical Thinking New York E P Dutton ISBN 978 0525241409 This work discusses how and why the magical thinking of childhood can carry into adulthood causing various maladaptions and psychopathologies Vyse Stuart 1997 Believing in Magic The Psychology of Superstition Oxford University Press ISBN 9780195136340 External links EditHutson Matthew 2008 Magical thinking Psychology Today Vol March April pp 89 95 Stevens Phillips Jr November December 2001 Magical thinking in complementary and alternative medicine Skeptical Inquirer 25 6 Archived from the original on 2010 06 03 Retrieved 2010 09 22 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Magical thinking amp oldid 1150140140, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.