fbpx
Wikipedia

Imaginary friend

Imaginary friends (also known as pretend friends, invisible friends or made-up friends) are a psychological and social phenomenon where a friendship or other interpersonal relationship takes place in the imagination rather than physical reality.

Caliban has a conversation with his imaginary friends in Folger Theatre's production of Shakespeare's The Tempest.

Although they may seem real to their creators, children usually understand that their imaginary friends are not real.[1]

The first studies focusing on imaginary friends are believed to have been conducted during the 1890s.[2] There is little research about the concept of imaginary friends in children's imaginations. Klausen and Passman (2007) report that imaginary companions were originally described as being supernatural creatures and spirits that were thought to connect people with their past lives.[3] Adults in history have had entities such as household gods, guardian angels, and muses that functioned as imaginary companions to provide comfort, guidance and inspiration for creative work.[3] It is possible the phenomenon appeared among children in the mid–19th century when childhood was emphasized as an important time for play and imagination.[3]

Description

In some studies, imaginary friends are defined as children impersonating a specific character (imagined by them), or objects or toys that are personified.[4] However, some psychologists will define an imaginary friend only as a separate created character.[5] Imaginary friends can be people, but they can also take the shape of other characters such as animals or other abstract ideas such as ghosts, monsters, robots, aliens or angels.[4][6] These characters can be created at any point during a lifetime, though Western culture suggests they are most acceptable in preschool- and school-age children.[4][5][6] They often function as tutelaries when played with by a child.[citation needed] They reveal, according to several theories of psychology, a child's anxieties, fears, goals and perceptions of the world through that child's conversations.[citation needed] They are, according to some children, physically indistinguishable from real people, while others say they see their imaginary friends only in their heads, and still others cannot see the friend at all but can sense his/her presence.[citation needed] Most research agrees that girls are more likely than boys to develop imaginary friends.[7] Once children reach school age, boys and girls are equally likely to have an imaginary companion.[6] Research has often reiterated that there is not a specific "type" of child that creates an imaginary friend.[4][6]

Research has shown that imaginary friends are a normative part of childhood and even adulthood.[4][5][6][8] Additionally, some psychologists[5][8] suggest that imaginary friends are much like a fictional character created by an author. As Eileen Kennedy-Moore points out, "Adult fiction writers often talk about their characters taking on a life of their own, which may be an analogous process to children’s invisible friends."[9] In addition, Marjorie Taylor and her colleagues have found that fiction writers are more likely than average to have had imaginary friends as children.[10]

There is a difference between the common imaginary friends that many children create, and the imaginary voices of psychopathology. Often when there’s a psychological disorder and any inner voices are present, they add negativity to the conversation. The person with the disorder may sometimes believe that the imagined voices are physically real, not an imagined inner dialog.[5][6]

Imaginary friends can serve various functions. Playing with imaginary friends enables children to enact behaviors and events they have not yet experienced. Imaginary play allows children to use their imagination to construct knowledge of the world. In addition, imaginary friends might also fulfill children's innate desire to connect with others before actual play among peers is common. According to psychologist Lev Vygotsky, cultural tools and interaction with people mediate psychological functioning and cognitive development. Imaginary friends, perceived as real beings, could teach children how to interact with others along with many other social skills. Vygotsky's sociocultural view of child development includes the notion of children's “zone of proximal development,” which is the difference between what children can do with and without help. Imaginary friends can aid children in learning things about the world that they could not learn without help, such as appropriate social behavior, and thus can act as a scaffold for children to achieve slightly above their social capability.[citation needed]

In addition, imaginary friends also serve as a means for children to experiment with and explore the world. In this sense, imaginary companions also relate to Piaget's theory of child development because they are completely constructed by the child. According to Piaget, children are scientific problem solvers who self-construct experiences and build internal mental structures based on experimentation. The creation of and interaction with imaginary companions helps children to build such mental structures. The relationship between a child and their imaginary friend can serve as a catalyst for the formation of real relationships in later development and thus provides a head start to practicing real-life interaction.

Research

It has been theorized that children with imaginary friends may develop language skills and retain knowledge faster than children without them, which may be because these children get more linguistic practice than their peers as a result of carrying out "conversations" with their imaginary friends.[11]

Kutner (n.d.) reported that 65% of 7-year-old children report they have had an imaginary companion at some point in their lives. He further reported:

Imaginary friends are an integral part of many children's lives. They provide comfort in times of stress, companionship when they're lonely, someone to boss around when they feel powerless, and someone to blame for the broken lamp in the living room. Most important, an imaginary companion is a tool young children use to help them make sense of the adult world.[12]

Taylor, Carlson & Gerow (c2001: p. 190) hold that:

despite some results suggesting that children with imaginary friends might be superior in intelligence, it is not true that all intelligent children create them.[13]

If imaginary friends can provide assistance to children in developing their social skills, they must function as important roles in the lives of children. Hoff (2004 – 2005) was interested in finding out the roles and functions of imaginary friends and how they impacted the lives of children.[14] The results of her study have provided some significant insight on the roles of imaginary friends. Many of the children reported their imaginary friends as being sources of comfort in times of boredom and loneliness.[14] Another interesting result was that imaginary friends served to be mentors for children in their academics.[14] They were encouraging, provided motivation, and increased the self-esteem of children when they did well in school.[14] Finally, imaginary friends were reported as being moral guides for children.[14] Many of the children reported that their imaginary friends served as a conscience and helped them to make the correct decision in times where morality was questioned.[14]

Other professionals such as Marjorie Taylor feel imaginary friends are common among school-age children and are part of normal social-cognitive development.[4] Part of the reason people believed children gave up imaginary companions earlier than has been observed is related to Piaget's stages of cognitive development. Piaget suggested that imaginary companions disappeared once children entered the concrete operational stage of development.[6] Marjorie Taylor identified middle school children with imaginary friends and followed up six years later as they were completing high school. At follow-up, those who had imaginary friends in middle school displayed better coping strategies but a "low social preference for peers." She suggested that imaginary friends may directly benefit children's resiliency and positive adjustment.[15] Because imagination play with a character involves the child often imagining how another person (or character) would act, research has been done to determine if having an imaginary companion has a positive effect on theory of mind development.[4][6] In a previous study, Taylor & Carlson (1997) found that 4-year-old children who had imaginary friends scored higher on emotional understanding measures and that having a theory of mind would predict higher emotional understanding later on in life.[16] When children develop the realization that other people have different thoughts and beliefs other than their own, they are able to grow in their development of theory of mind as they begin to have better understandings of emotions.[16]

Positive psychology

The article, "Pretend play and positive psychology: Natural companions" defined many great tools that are seen in children who engage pretend play. These five areas include creativity, coping, emotion regulation, empathy/emotional understanding and hope.[17] Hope seems to be the underlying tool children use in motivation.[17] Children become more motivated when they believe in themselves, therefore children will not be discouraged to come up with different ways of thinking because they will have confidence.[17] Imaginary companionship displays immense creativity helping them to develop their social skills and creativity is frequently discussed term amongst positive psychology.[17]

An imaginary companion can be considered the product of the child's creativity whereas the communication between the imaginary friend and the child is considered to be the process.[17]

Adolescence

"Imaginary companions in adolescence: sign of a deficient or positive development?" explores the extent to which adolescents create imaginary companions.[18] The researchers explored the prevalence of imaginary companions in adolescence by investigating the diaries of adolescents age 12-17.[18] In addition they looked at the characteristics of these imaginary companions and did a content analysis of the data obtained in the diaries.[18] There were three hypotheses tested: (1) the deficit hypothesis, (2) the giftedness hypothesis, (3) the egocentrism hypothesis.[18] The results of their study concluded that creative and socially competent adolescents with great coping skills were particularly prone to the creation of these imaginary friends.[18] These findings did not support the deficit hypothesis or egocentrism hypothesis, further suggesting that these imaginary companions were not created with the aim to replace or substitute a real-life family member or friend, but they simply created another "very special friend".[18] This is surprising because it is usually assumed that children who create imaginary companions have deficits of some sort, and in addition for an adolescent to have an imaginary companion is unheard of.[18]

Tulpa

Following the popularizing and secularizing of the concept of tulpa in the Western world, these practitioners, calling themselves "tulpamancers", report an improvement to their personal lives through the practice, and new unusual sensory experiences. Some practitioners use the tulpa for sexual and romantic interactions, though the practice is considered taboo. A survey of the community with 118 respondents on the explanation of tulpas found 8.5% support a metaphysical explanation, 76.5% support a neurological or psychological explanation, and 14% "other" explanations. Nearly all practitioners consider the tulpa a real or somewhat-real person. The number of active participants in these online communities is in the low hundreds, and few meetings in person have taken place.[19]

Birth order

To uncover the origin of imaginary companions and learn more about the children who create them, it is necessary to seek out children who have created imaginary companions.[20] Unfortunately young children cannot accurately self-report, therefore the most effective way to gather information about children and their imaginary companions is by interviewing the people who spend the most time with them. Often mothers are the primary caretakers who spend the most time with a child. Therefore, for this study 78 mothers were interviewed and asked whether their child had an imaginary friend.[20] If the mother revealed that their child did not have an imaginary companion then the researcher asked about the child's tendency to personify objects.[20]

In order to convey the meaning of personified objects the researchers explained to the mothers that it is common for children to choose a specific toy or object that they are particularly attached to or fond of.[20] For the object to qualify as a personified object the child had to treat it as animate.[20] Furthermore, it is necessary to reveal what children consider an imaginary friend or pretend play. In order to distinguish a child having or not having an imaginary companion, the friend had to be in existence for at least one month. In order to examine the developmental significance of preschool children and their imaginary companions the mothers of children were interviewed.[20] The major conclusion from the study was that there is a significant distinction between invisible companions and personified objects.[20]

A significant finding in this study was the role of the child's birth order in the family in terms of having an imaginary companion or not. The results of the interviews with mothers indicated that children with imaginary friends were more likely to be a first-born child when compared to children who did not have an imaginary companion at all.[20] This study further supports that children may create imaginary friends to work on social development. The findings that a first-born child is more likely to have an imaginary friend sheds some light on the idea that the child needs to socialize therefore they create the imaginary friend to develop their social skills. This is an extremely creative way for children to develop their social skills and creativity is frequently discussed term amongst positive psychology.[17] An imaginary companion can be considered the product of creativity whereas the communication between the imaginary friend and the child is the process.[17]

In regards to birth order there is also research on children who do not have any siblings at all. The research in this area further investigates the notion that children create imaginary companions due to the absence of peer relationships.[21] A study that examined the differences in self-talk frequency as a function of age, only-child, and imaginary childhood companion status provides a lot of insight to the commonalties of children with imaginary companions.[22] The researchers collected information from college students who were asked if they ever had an imaginary friend as a child (Brinthaupt & Dove, 2012). There were three studies within the one study and they found that there were significant differences in self-talk between different age groupings.[22]

Their first study indicated that only children who create imaginary companions actually engage in high levels of positive self-talk had more positive social development.[22] They also found a gender difference within their study that women were more likely than men to have an imaginary companion.[22] Their findings were consistent with other research supporting that it is more common for females to have imaginary companions.[22] One possible explanation the researchers suggested that women may be more likely to have imaginary companions is because they are more likely to rely on feedback from other than themselves supporting the conclusions that men were found to have more self reinforcing self-talk.[22]

Furthermore, other research has concluded that women seek more social support than men, which could be another possibility for creating these imaginary companions.[22] The second study found that children without siblings reported more self-talk than children with siblings and the third study found that the students who reported having an imaginary friend also reported more self talk than the other students who did not have imaginary friends.[22] Self-talk is often associated with negative effects such as increased anxiety and depression when the self-talk is specifically negative.[22] The researchers found that "Individuals with higher levels of social-assessment and critical self-talk reported lower self-esteem and more frequent automatic negative self-statements".[22]

However, there is also a positive side to positive self-talk and in this study they found that, "people with higher levels of self-reinforcing self-talk reported more positive self-esteem and more frequent automatic positive self-statements".[22] They also found that men had a more frequent self-reinforcing self-talk than females.[22] This particular finding is important because there are not many general findings comparing men and women in adult self-talk in today's research. Self-talk and imaginary companionship contain many similarities therefore it is possible that they can be related. Through positive self-talk children can increase their self-esteem, which leads to the possibility that a positive relationship with an imaginary companion could predict a similar outcome.

See also

References

  1. ^ Taylor, M. (1999) Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them. New York: Oxford University Press.
  2. ^ Klausen, Espen; Richard H. Passman (December 2006). "Pretend companions (imaginary playmates): the emergence of a field". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 167 (4): 349–364. doi:10.3200/gntp.167.4.349-364. PMID 17645227. S2CID 35306762. Gale Document Number: GALE|A166239640. Retrieved 9 November 2011.
  3. ^ a b c Klausen, E.; Passman, R. H. (2007). "Pretend companions (imaginary playmates): The emergence of a field". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 167 (4): 349–364. doi:10.3200/gntp.167.4.349-364. PMID 17645227. S2CID 35306762.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g Taylor, Marjorie; Carlson, Stephanie; Maring, Bayta; Gerow, Lynn; Charley, Carolyn (2004). "The characteristics and correlates of fantasy in school-age children: Imaginary friends, impersonation, and social understanding". Developmental Psychology. 40 (6): 1173–1187. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1173. PMID 15535765. S2CID 16472272.
  5. ^ a b c d e Gleason, Tracy (2013). The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-539576-1.
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h Taylor, Marjorie; Mannering, Anne (2006). "Of Hobbes and Harvey: The imaginary friends created by children and adults.". In Göncü, Artin; Gaskins, Suzanne (eds.). Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives. pp. 227–245.
  7. ^ Carlson, S.M.; Taylor, M. (2005). "Imaginary companions and impersonated characters: Sex differences in children's fantasy play". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 51: 93–118. doi:10.1353/mpq.2005.0003. S2CID 14359259.
  8. ^ a b Gopnik, Alison (2010). The philosophical baby : what children's minds tell us about truth, love, and the meaning of life (1st Picador ed.). New York: Picador/Farrar, Straus and Giroux. ISBN 978-0-312-42984-3.
  9. ^ Kennedy-Moore, Eileen (2013) "Imaginary Friends: Are invisible friends a sign of social problems?" Psychology Today; Growing Friendships blog. 31 January 2013. [1] (accessed: 24 May 2013)
  10. ^ Taylor, M.; Hodges, S. D.; Kohányi, A. (2002). "The Illusion of Independent Agency: Do adult fiction writers experience their characters as having minds of their own?". Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 22 (4): 361–380. doi:10.2190/ftg3-q9t0-7u26-5q5x. S2CID 14988767.
  11. ^ University of Manchester (8 March 2005). "Imaginary Friendships Could Boost Child Development". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 9 November 2011.
  12. ^ Kutner, Lawrence (n.d.). Insights for Parents: Midnight Monsters and Imaginary Companions. Source: [2] 2015-04-30 at the Wayback Machine (accessed: Monday May 18, 2009)
  13. ^ Taylor, Marjorie; Carlson, Stephanie M.; Gerow, Lynn (c. 2001). "Imaginary Companions: Characteristics and Correlates" in Reifel, Robert Stuart (2001). Theory in Context and Out. Greenwood Publishing Group. Edition: illustrated. ISBN 1-56750-486-8. Source: [3] (accessed: Monday May 18, 2009)
  14. ^ a b c d e f Hoff, E.V. (2004). "A friend living inside me - The forms and functions of imaginary companions". Imagination, Cognition, and Personality. 24 (2): 151–189. doi:10.2190/4m9j-76m2-4q4q-8kyt. S2CID 143609822.
  15. ^ Taylor, M.; Hulette, A.C.; Dishion, T. J. (2010). "Longitudinal Outcomes of Young High-Risk Adolescents With Imaginary Companions". Developmental Psychology. 46 (6): 1632–1636. doi:10.1037/a0019815. PMC 3353747. PMID 20677857.
  16. ^ a b Taylor, M.; Carlson, S. (1997). "The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind". Child Development. 68 (3): 436–458. doi:10.2307/1131670. JSTOR 1131670. PMID 9249959.
  17. ^ a b c d e f g Pearson, Beth L.; Russ, Sandra W.; Spannagel, Sarah A. Cain (April 2008). "Pretend play and positive psychology: Natural companions". Journal of Positive Psychology. 3 (2): 110–119. doi:10.1080/17439760701760617. S2CID 143987001.
  18. ^ a b c d e f g Siffge-Krenke, I. (April 1997). "Imaginary companions in adolescence: sign of a deficient or positive development". Journal of Early Adolescence. 20 (2): 137–154. doi:10.1006/jado.1996.0072. PMID 9104650.
  19. ^ Samuel Veissière (April 3, 2015), "Varieties of Tulpa Experiences: Sentient Imaginary Friends, Embodied Joint Attention, and Hypnotic Sociality in a Wired World", in Amir Raz; Michael Lifshitz (eds.), Hypnosis and Meditation, Oxford University Press, ISBN 9780198759102, retrieved July 14, 2016
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h Gleason, T.; Sebanc, A.; Hartup, W. (2000). "Imaginary companions of preschool children". Developmental Psychology. 36 (4): 419–428. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.4.419. PMID 10902694.
  21. ^ Gleason, T (2004). "Imaginary companions: An evaluation of parents as reporters". Infant and Child Development. 13 (3): 199–215. doi:10.1002/icd.349.
  22. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Brinthaupt, Thomas M.; Dove, Christian T. (June 2012). "Differences in self-talk frequency as a function of age, only-child, and imaginary childhood companion status". Journal of Research in Personality. 46 (3): 326–333. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.03.003.

Further reading

  • Dierker, L. C.; Davis, K. F.; Sanders, B. (1995). "The imaginary companion phenomenon: An analysis of personality correlates and developmental antecedents". Dissociation. 8: 220–228.
  • Gleason, T (2002). "Social provisions of real and imaginary relationships in early childhood". Developmental Psychology. 38 (6): 979–992. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.979. PMID 12428709.
  • Gleason, T. (2009). 'Imaginary companions.' In Harry T. Reis & Susan Sprecher (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human Relationships (pp. 833–834). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Hall, E. (1982). 'The fearful child's hidden talents [Interview with Jerome Kagan].' Psychology Today, 16 (July), 50–59.
  • Hurlock, E.; Burstein, M. (1932). "The imaginary playmate: A questionnaire study". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 41 (2): 380–392. doi:10.1080/08856559.1932.10533102.
  • Manosevitz, M.; Fling, S.; Prentice, N. (1977). "Imaginary companions in young children: Relationships with intelligence, creativity and waiting ability". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 18 (1): 73–78. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1977.tb00418.x. PMID 838788.
  • Manosevitz, M.; Prentice, N.; Wilson, F. (1973). "Individual and family correlates of imaginary companions in preschool children". Developmental Psychology. 8: 72–79. doi:10.1037/h0033834.
  • Mauro, J (1991). "'The friend that only I can see: A longitudinal investigation of children's imaginary companions' (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene, 1991)". Dissertation Abstracts International. 52: 4995.
  • Meyer, J.; Tuber, S. (1989). "Intrapsychic and behavioral correlates of the phenomenon of imaginary companions in young children". Psychoanalytic Psychology. 6 (2): 151–168. doi:10.1037/0736-9735.6.2.151.
  • Nagera, H (1969). "The imaginary companion: Its significance for ego development and conflict solution". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child. 24: 165–195. doi:10.1080/00797308.1969.11822691. PMID 5353362.
  • Partington, J., & Grant, C. (1984). 'Imaginary playmates and other useful fantasies.' In P. Smith (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 217–240). New York: Basil Blackwell.
  • Imaginary Friends with Dr Evan Kidd 2020-06-23 at the Wayback Machine podcast interview with Dr Evan Kidd of La Trobe University

imaginary, friend, other, uses, imaginary, friend, disambiguation, also, known, pretend, friends, invisible, friends, made, friends, psychological, social, phenomenon, where, friendship, other, interpersonal, relationship, takes, place, imagination, rather, th. For other uses see Imaginary Friend disambiguation Imaginary friends also known as pretend friends invisible friends or made up friends are a psychological and social phenomenon where a friendship or other interpersonal relationship takes place in the imagination rather than physical reality Caliban has a conversation with his imaginary friends in Folger Theatre s production of Shakespeare s The Tempest Although they may seem real to their creators children usually understand that their imaginary friends are not real 1 The first studies focusing on imaginary friends are believed to have been conducted during the 1890s 2 There is little research about the concept of imaginary friends in children s imaginations Klausen and Passman 2007 report that imaginary companions were originally described as being supernatural creatures and spirits that were thought to connect people with their past lives 3 Adults in history have had entities such as household gods guardian angels and muses that functioned as imaginary companions to provide comfort guidance and inspiration for creative work 3 It is possible the phenomenon appeared among children in the mid 19th century when childhood was emphasized as an important time for play and imagination 3 Contents 1 Description 2 Research 2 1 Positive psychology 2 2 Adolescence 2 2 1 Tulpa 2 3 Birth order 3 See also 4 References 5 Further readingDescription EditIn some studies imaginary friends are defined as children impersonating a specific character imagined by them or objects or toys that are personified 4 However some psychologists will define an imaginary friend only as a separate created character 5 Imaginary friends can be people but they can also take the shape of other characters such as animals or other abstract ideas such as ghosts monsters robots aliens or angels 4 6 These characters can be created at any point during a lifetime though Western culture suggests they are most acceptable in preschool and school age children 4 5 6 They often function as tutelaries when played with by a child citation needed They reveal according to several theories of psychology a child s anxieties fears goals and perceptions of the world through that child s conversations citation needed They are according to some children physically indistinguishable from real people while others say they see their imaginary friends only in their heads and still others cannot see the friend at all but can sense his her presence citation needed Most research agrees that girls are more likely than boys to develop imaginary friends 7 Once children reach school age boys and girls are equally likely to have an imaginary companion 6 Research has often reiterated that there is not a specific type of child that creates an imaginary friend 4 6 Research has shown that imaginary friends are a normative part of childhood and even adulthood 4 5 6 8 Additionally some psychologists 5 8 suggest that imaginary friends are much like a fictional character created by an author As Eileen Kennedy Moore points out Adult fiction writers often talk about their characters taking on a life of their own which may be an analogous process to children s invisible friends 9 In addition Marjorie Taylor and her colleagues have found that fiction writers are more likely than average to have had imaginary friends as children 10 There is a difference between the common imaginary friends that many children create and the imaginary voices of psychopathology Often when there s a psychological disorder and any inner voices are present they add negativity to the conversation The person with the disorder may sometimes believe that the imagined voices are physically real not an imagined inner dialog 5 6 Imaginary friends can serve various functions Playing with imaginary friends enables children to enact behaviors and events they have not yet experienced Imaginary play allows children to use their imagination to construct knowledge of the world In addition imaginary friends might also fulfill children s innate desire to connect with others before actual play among peers is common According to psychologist Lev Vygotsky cultural tools and interaction with people mediate psychological functioning and cognitive development Imaginary friends perceived as real beings could teach children how to interact with others along with many other social skills Vygotsky s sociocultural view of child development includes the notion of children s zone of proximal development which is the difference between what children can do with and without help Imaginary friends can aid children in learning things about the world that they could not learn without help such as appropriate social behavior and thus can act as a scaffold for children to achieve slightly above their social capability citation needed In addition imaginary friends also serve as a means for children to experiment with and explore the world In this sense imaginary companions also relate to Piaget s theory of child development because they are completely constructed by the child According to Piaget children are scientific problem solvers who self construct experiences and build internal mental structures based on experimentation The creation of and interaction with imaginary companions helps children to build such mental structures The relationship between a child and their imaginary friend can serve as a catalyst for the formation of real relationships in later development and thus provides a head start to practicing real life interaction Research EditIt has been theorized that children with imaginary friends may develop language skills and retain knowledge faster than children without them which may be because these children get more linguistic practice than their peers as a result of carrying out conversations with their imaginary friends 11 Kutner n d reported that 65 of 7 year old children report they have had an imaginary companion at some point in their lives He further reported Imaginary friends are an integral part of many children s lives They provide comfort in times of stress companionship when they re lonely someone to boss around when they feel powerless and someone to blame for the broken lamp in the living room Most important an imaginary companion is a tool young children use to help them make sense of the adult world 12 Taylor Carlson amp Gerow c2001 p 190 hold that despite some results suggesting that children with imaginary friends might be superior in intelligence it is not true that all intelligent children create them 13 If imaginary friends can provide assistance to children in developing their social skills they must function as important roles in the lives of children Hoff 2004 2005 was interested in finding out the roles and functions of imaginary friends and how they impacted the lives of children 14 The results of her study have provided some significant insight on the roles of imaginary friends Many of the children reported their imaginary friends as being sources of comfort in times of boredom and loneliness 14 Another interesting result was that imaginary friends served to be mentors for children in their academics 14 They were encouraging provided motivation and increased the self esteem of children when they did well in school 14 Finally imaginary friends were reported as being moral guides for children 14 Many of the children reported that their imaginary friends served as a conscience and helped them to make the correct decision in times where morality was questioned 14 Other professionals such as Marjorie Taylor feel imaginary friends are common among school age children and are part of normal social cognitive development 4 Part of the reason people believed children gave up imaginary companions earlier than has been observed is related to Piaget s stages of cognitive development Piaget suggested that imaginary companions disappeared once children entered the concrete operational stage of development 6 Marjorie Taylor identified middle school children with imaginary friends and followed up six years later as they were completing high school At follow up those who had imaginary friends in middle school displayed better coping strategies but a low social preference for peers She suggested that imaginary friends may directly benefit children s resiliency and positive adjustment 15 Because imagination play with a character involves the child often imagining how another person or character would act research has been done to determine if having an imaginary companion has a positive effect on theory of mind development 4 6 In a previous study Taylor amp Carlson 1997 found that 4 year old children who had imaginary friends scored higher on emotional understanding measures and that having a theory of mind would predict higher emotional understanding later on in life 16 When children develop the realization that other people have different thoughts and beliefs other than their own they are able to grow in their development of theory of mind as they begin to have better understandings of emotions 16 Positive psychology Edit The article Pretend play and positive psychology Natural companions defined many great tools that are seen in children who engage pretend play These five areas include creativity coping emotion regulation empathy emotional understanding and hope 17 Hope seems to be the underlying tool children use in motivation 17 Children become more motivated when they believe in themselves therefore children will not be discouraged to come up with different ways of thinking because they will have confidence 17 Imaginary companionship displays immense creativity helping them to develop their social skills and creativity is frequently discussed term amongst positive psychology 17 An imaginary companion can be considered the product of the child s creativity whereas the communication between the imaginary friend and the child is considered to be the process 17 Adolescence Edit Imaginary companions in adolescence sign of a deficient or positive development explores the extent to which adolescents create imaginary companions 18 The researchers explored the prevalence of imaginary companions in adolescence by investigating the diaries of adolescents age 12 17 18 In addition they looked at the characteristics of these imaginary companions and did a content analysis of the data obtained in the diaries 18 There were three hypotheses tested 1 the deficit hypothesis 2 the giftedness hypothesis 3 the egocentrism hypothesis 18 The results of their study concluded that creative and socially competent adolescents with great coping skills were particularly prone to the creation of these imaginary friends 18 These findings did not support the deficit hypothesis or egocentrism hypothesis further suggesting that these imaginary companions were not created with the aim to replace or substitute a real life family member or friend but they simply created another very special friend 18 This is surprising because it is usually assumed that children who create imaginary companions have deficits of some sort and in addition for an adolescent to have an imaginary companion is unheard of 18 Tulpa Edit Following the popularizing and secularizing of the concept of tulpa in the Western world these practitioners calling themselves tulpamancers report an improvement to their personal lives through the practice and new unusual sensory experiences Some practitioners use the tulpa for sexual and romantic interactions though the practice is considered taboo A survey of the community with 118 respondents on the explanation of tulpas found 8 5 support a metaphysical explanation 76 5 support a neurological or psychological explanation and 14 other explanations Nearly all practitioners consider the tulpa a real or somewhat real person The number of active participants in these online communities is in the low hundreds and few meetings in person have taken place 19 Birth order Edit To uncover the origin of imaginary companions and learn more about the children who create them it is necessary to seek out children who have created imaginary companions 20 Unfortunately young children cannot accurately self report therefore the most effective way to gather information about children and their imaginary companions is by interviewing the people who spend the most time with them Often mothers are the primary caretakers who spend the most time with a child Therefore for this study 78 mothers were interviewed and asked whether their child had an imaginary friend 20 If the mother revealed that their child did not have an imaginary companion then the researcher asked about the child s tendency to personify objects 20 In order to convey the meaning of personified objects the researchers explained to the mothers that it is common for children to choose a specific toy or object that they are particularly attached to or fond of 20 For the object to qualify as a personified object the child had to treat it as animate 20 Furthermore it is necessary to reveal what children consider an imaginary friend or pretend play In order to distinguish a child having or not having an imaginary companion the friend had to be in existence for at least one month In order to examine the developmental significance of preschool children and their imaginary companions the mothers of children were interviewed 20 The major conclusion from the study was that there is a significant distinction between invisible companions and personified objects 20 A significant finding in this study was the role of the child s birth order in the family in terms of having an imaginary companion or not The results of the interviews with mothers indicated that children with imaginary friends were more likely to be a first born child when compared to children who did not have an imaginary companion at all 20 This study further supports that children may create imaginary friends to work on social development The findings that a first born child is more likely to have an imaginary friend sheds some light on the idea that the child needs to socialize therefore they create the imaginary friend to develop their social skills This is an extremely creative way for children to develop their social skills and creativity is frequently discussed term amongst positive psychology 17 An imaginary companion can be considered the product of creativity whereas the communication between the imaginary friend and the child is the process 17 In regards to birth order there is also research on children who do not have any siblings at all The research in this area further investigates the notion that children create imaginary companions due to the absence of peer relationships 21 A study that examined the differences in self talk frequency as a function of age only child and imaginary childhood companion status provides a lot of insight to the commonalties of children with imaginary companions 22 The researchers collected information from college students who were asked if they ever had an imaginary friend as a child Brinthaupt amp Dove 2012 There were three studies within the one study and they found that there were significant differences in self talk between different age groupings 22 Their first study indicated that only children who create imaginary companions actually engage in high levels of positive self talk had more positive social development 22 They also found a gender difference within their study that women were more likely than men to have an imaginary companion 22 Their findings were consistent with other research supporting that it is more common for females to have imaginary companions 22 One possible explanation the researchers suggested that women may be more likely to have imaginary companions is because they are more likely to rely on feedback from other than themselves supporting the conclusions that men were found to have more self reinforcing self talk 22 Furthermore other research has concluded that women seek more social support than men which could be another possibility for creating these imaginary companions 22 The second study found that children without siblings reported more self talk than children with siblings and the third study found that the students who reported having an imaginary friend also reported more self talk than the other students who did not have imaginary friends 22 Self talk is often associated with negative effects such as increased anxiety and depression when the self talk is specifically negative 22 The researchers found that Individuals with higher levels of social assessment and critical self talk reported lower self esteem and more frequent automatic negative self statements 22 However there is also a positive side to positive self talk and in this study they found that people with higher levels of self reinforcing self talk reported more positive self esteem and more frequent automatic positive self statements 22 They also found that men had a more frequent self reinforcing self talk than females 22 This particular finding is important because there are not many general findings comparing men and women in adult self talk in today s research Self talk and imaginary companionship contain many similarities therefore it is possible that they can be related Through positive self talk children can increase their self esteem which leads to the possibility that a positive relationship with an imaginary companion could predict a similar outcome See also EditBicameral mentality List of imaginary characters in fiction Paracosm Superstition TulpaReferences Edit Taylor M 1999 Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them New York Oxford University Press Klausen Espen Richard H Passman December 2006 Pretend companions imaginary playmates the emergence of a field Journal of Genetic Psychology 167 4 349 364 doi 10 3200 gntp 167 4 349 364 PMID 17645227 S2CID 35306762 Gale Document Number GALE A166239640 Retrieved 9 November 2011 a b c Klausen E Passman R H 2007 Pretend companions imaginary playmates The emergence of a field The Journal of Genetic Psychology 167 4 349 364 doi 10 3200 gntp 167 4 349 364 PMID 17645227 S2CID 35306762 a b c d e f g Taylor Marjorie Carlson Stephanie Maring Bayta Gerow Lynn Charley Carolyn 2004 The characteristics and correlates of fantasy in school age children Imaginary friends impersonation and social understanding Developmental Psychology 40 6 1173 1187 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 40 6 1173 PMID 15535765 S2CID 16472272 a b c d e Gleason Tracy 2013 The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 539576 1 a b c d e f g h Taylor Marjorie Mannering Anne 2006 Of Hobbes and Harvey The imaginary friends created by children and adults In Goncu Artin Gaskins Suzanne eds Play and development Evolutionary sociocultural and functional perspectives pp 227 245 Carlson S M Taylor M 2005 Imaginary companions and impersonated characters Sex differences in children s fantasy play Merrill Palmer Quarterly 51 93 118 doi 10 1353 mpq 2005 0003 S2CID 14359259 a b Gopnik Alison 2010 The philosophical baby what children s minds tell us about truth love and the meaning of life 1st Picador ed New York Picador Farrar Straus and Giroux ISBN 978 0 312 42984 3 Kennedy Moore Eileen 2013 Imaginary Friends Are invisible friends a sign of social problems Psychology Today Growing Friendships blog 31 January 2013 1 accessed 24 May 2013 Taylor M Hodges S D Kohanyi A 2002 The Illusion of Independent Agency Do adult fiction writers experience their characters as having minds of their own Imagination Cognition and Personality 22 4 361 380 doi 10 2190 ftg3 q9t0 7u26 5q5x S2CID 14988767 University of Manchester 8 March 2005 Imaginary Friendships Could Boost Child Development ScienceDaily Retrieved 9 November 2011 Kutner Lawrence n d Insights for Parents Midnight Monsters and Imaginary Companions Source 2 Archived 2015 04 30 at the Wayback Machine accessed Monday May 18 2009 Taylor Marjorie Carlson Stephanie M Gerow Lynn c 2001 Imaginary Companions Characteristics and Correlates in Reifel Robert Stuart 2001 Theory in Context and Out Greenwood Publishing Group Edition illustrated ISBN 1 56750 486 8 Source 3 accessed Monday May 18 2009 a b c d e f Hoff E V 2004 A friend living inside me The forms and functions of imaginary companions Imagination Cognition and Personality 24 2 151 189 doi 10 2190 4m9j 76m2 4q4q 8kyt S2CID 143609822 Taylor M Hulette A C Dishion T J 2010 Longitudinal Outcomes of Young High Risk Adolescents With Imaginary Companions Developmental Psychology 46 6 1632 1636 doi 10 1037 a0019815 PMC 3353747 PMID 20677857 a b Taylor M Carlson S 1997 The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind Child Development 68 3 436 458 doi 10 2307 1131670 JSTOR 1131670 PMID 9249959 a b c d e f g Pearson Beth L Russ Sandra W Spannagel Sarah A Cain April 2008 Pretend play and positive psychology Natural companions Journal of Positive Psychology 3 2 110 119 doi 10 1080 17439760701760617 S2CID 143987001 a b c d e f g Siffge Krenke I April 1997 Imaginary companions in adolescence sign of a deficient or positive development Journal of Early Adolescence 20 2 137 154 doi 10 1006 jado 1996 0072 PMID 9104650 Samuel Veissiere April 3 2015 Varieties of Tulpa Experiences Sentient Imaginary Friends Embodied Joint Attention and Hypnotic Sociality in a Wired World in Amir Raz Michael Lifshitz eds Hypnosis and Meditation Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198759102 retrieved July 14 2016 a b c d e f g h Gleason T Sebanc A Hartup W 2000 Imaginary companions of preschool children Developmental Psychology 36 4 419 428 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 36 4 419 PMID 10902694 Gleason T 2004 Imaginary companions An evaluation of parents as reporters Infant and Child Development 13 3 199 215 doi 10 1002 icd 349 a b c d e f g h i j k l Brinthaupt Thomas M Dove Christian T June 2012 Differences in self talk frequency as a function of age only child and imaginary childhood companion status Journal of Research in Personality 46 3 326 333 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2012 03 003 Further reading EditDierker L C Davis K F Sanders B 1995 The imaginary companion phenomenon An analysis of personality correlates and developmental antecedents Dissociation 8 220 228 Gleason T 2002 Social provisions of real and imaginary relationships in early childhood Developmental Psychology 38 6 979 992 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 38 6 979 PMID 12428709 Gleason T 2009 Imaginary companions In Harry T Reis amp Susan Sprecher Eds Encyclopedia of Human Relationships pp 833 834 Thousand Oaks CA Sage Hall E 1982 The fearful child s hidden talents Interview with Jerome Kagan Psychology Today 16 July 50 59 Hurlock E Burstein M 1932 The imaginary playmate A questionnaire study Journal of Genetic Psychology 41 2 380 392 doi 10 1080 08856559 1932 10533102 Manosevitz M Fling S Prentice N 1977 Imaginary companions in young children Relationships with intelligence creativity and waiting ability Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 18 1 73 78 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 1977 tb00418 x PMID 838788 Manosevitz M Prentice N Wilson F 1973 Individual and family correlates of imaginary companions in preschool children Developmental Psychology 8 72 79 doi 10 1037 h0033834 Mauro J 1991 The friend that only I can see A longitudinal investigation of children s imaginary companions Doctoral dissertation University of Oregon Eugene 1991 Dissertation Abstracts International 52 4995 Meyer J Tuber S 1989 Intrapsychic and behavioral correlates of the phenomenon of imaginary companions in young children Psychoanalytic Psychology 6 2 151 168 doi 10 1037 0736 9735 6 2 151 Nagera H 1969 The imaginary companion Its significance for ego development and conflict solution Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 24 165 195 doi 10 1080 00797308 1969 11822691 PMID 5353362 Partington J amp Grant C 1984 Imaginary playmates and other useful fantasies In P Smith Ed Play in animals and humans pp 217 240 New York Basil Blackwell Imaginary Friends with Dr Evan Kidd Archived 2020 06 23 at the Wayback Machine podcast interview with Dr Evan Kidd of La Trobe University Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Imaginary friend amp oldid 1134589211, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.