fbpx
Wikipedia

Cultural psychology

Cultural psychology is the study of how cultures reflect and shape their members' psychological processes.[1]

It is based on the premise that the mind and culture are inseparable and mutually constitutive. The concept involves two propositions;

a) people are shaped by their culture, and

b) culture is shaped by its people.[2]

Cultural psychology aims to define culture, its nature, and its function concerning psychological phenomena. Gerd Baumann argues: "Culture is not a real thing, but an abstract analytical notion. In itself, it does not cause behavior but abstracts from it. It is thus neither normative nor predictive but a heuristic means towards explaining how people understand and act upon the world."[3]

As Richard Shweder, one of the major proponents of the field, writes, "Cultural psychology is the study of how cultural traditions and social practices regulate, express, and transform the human psyche. This results less in psychic unity for humankind than in ethnic divergences in mind, self, and emotion."[4]

History edit

Yoshihisa Kashima talks about cultural psychology in two senses, as a tradition and as a movement that emerged in the late 20th century.[5][6] Cultural psychology as a tradition is traced back to Western Romanticism in the 19th century.[5] Giambatista Vico and Herder are seen as important early inspirations in thinking about the influence of culture on people.[5][7]

Its institutional origin started with the publication of the Zeitschrift für Völkerpsychologie und Sprachwissenschaft, first published in 1860. Wilhelm Wundt took this concept and his volume on Völkerpsychologie is one of the earliest accounts of a cultural perspective within the discipline of psychology.[6] He saw Völkerpsychologie as a cultural-developmental discipline that studied higher psychological processes in their social context. The proposed methods were comparative and historical analyses.[8][9][10]

Another early cultural framework is cultural-historical psychology which emerged in the 1920s. It is mostly associated with the Russian psychologists Vygotsky, Luria and Leont'ev.[6] They claimed that human activity is always embedded in a specific social and historical context and should therefore not be isolated.[6][10]

While in psychological research interest in culture had declined, in part due to the popularity of behaviorism in the US, some researchers in anthropology, like Margaret Mead, started to explore the interaction between culture and personality.[5][6] In the 1970s-1980s, there was an increasing call for an interpretive turn in anthropology and psychology. Researchers were influenced by constructivist and relativist accounts of knowledge and argued that cultural differences should be understood within their contexts.[5][11] This influence was an important factor in the emergence of the cultural psychology movement. Leading scholars of this movement were, among others, Richard Shweder and Clifford Richards.[11] The launch of a new journal and the publication of multiple major works, like Shweder's Cultural Psychology and Cole's Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline helped to shape the direction of the movement.[6]

Relationships with other branches of psychology edit

Cultural psychology is often confused with cross-cultural psychology. Even though both fields influence each other, cultural psychology is distinct from cross-cultural psychology in that cross-cultural psychologists generally use culture as a means of testing the universality of psychological processes rather than determining how local cultural practices shape psychological processes.[12] So, whereas a cross-cultural psychologist might ask whether Jean Piaget's stages of development are universal across a variety of cultures, a cultural psychologist would be interested in how the social practices of a particular set of cultures shape the development of cognitive processes in different ways.[13]

Cultural psychology research informs and is informed by several fields within psychology, including cross-cultural psychology, social psychology, cultural-historical psychology, developmental psychology, and cognitive psychology. In addition to drawing from several other fields of psychology, cultural psychology in particular utilizes anthropologists, linguists, and philosophers to help in the pursuit of understanding a wide variety of cultural facets in a society.[14] However, the constructivist perspective of cultural psychology, through which cultural psychologists study thought patterns and behaviors within and across cultures, tends to clash with the universal perspectives common in most fields of psychology, which seek to qualify fundamental psychological truths that are consistent across all of humanity.

Importance edit

Need for expanded cultural research edit

According to Richard Shweder, there has been repeated failure to replicate Western psychology laboratory findings in non-Western settings.[4] Therefore, a major goal of cultural psychology is to have many and varied cultures contribute to basic psychological theories in order to correct these theories so that they become more relevant to the predictions, descriptions, and explanations of all human behaviors, not just Western ones.[15] This goal is shared by many of the scholars who promote the indigenous psychology approach. In an attempt to show the interrelated interests of cultural and indigenous psychology, cultural psychologist Pradeep Chakkarath emphasizes that international mainstream psychology, as it has been exported to most regions of the world by the so-called West, is only one among many indigenous psychologies and therefore may not have enough intercultural expertise to claim, as it frequently does, that its theories have universal validity.[16] Accordingly, cultural groups have diverse ways of defining emotional problems, as well as distinguishing between physical and mental distress. For example, Arthur Kleinman has shown how the notion of depression in Chinese culture has been associated with physiological problems, before becoming acknowledged more recently as an emotional concern.[17] Furthermore, the type of therapy people pursue is influenced by cultural conceptions of privacy and shame, as well as the stigmas associated with specific problems.[18]

The acronym W.E.I.R.D. describes populations that are Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic. Thus far, W.E.I.R.D. populations have been vastly overrepresented in psychological research.[19][20] In an analysis of top journals in the psychology discipline, it was found that 96% of subjects who participated in those studies came from Western Industrialized countries, with 68% of them coming from the United States. This is largely because 99% of the authors of these journals were at Western Universities with 73% of them at American Universities. With this information, it is concluded that 96% of psychological findings come from W.E.I.R.D. countries.[20] Findings from psychology research utilizing primarily W.E.I.R.D. populations are often labeled as universal theories and are inaccurately applied to other cultures.[21]

Recent research is showing that cultures differ in many areas, such as logical reasoning and social values.[20][21] The evidence that basic cognitive and motivational processes vary across populations has become increasingly difficult to ignore. For example, many studies have shown that Americans, Canadians and western Europeans rely on analytical reasoning strategies, which separate objects from their contexts to explain and predict behavior. Social psychologists refer to the "fundamental attribution error" or the tendency to explain people's behavior in terms of internal, inherent personality traits rather than external, situational considerations (e.g. attributing an instance of angry behavior to an angry personality). Outside W.E.I.R.D. cultures, however, this phenomenon is less prominent, as many non-W.E.I.R.D. populations tend to pay more attention to the context in which behavior occurs. Asians tend to reason holistically, for example by considering people's behavior in terms of their situation; someone's anger might be viewed as simply a result of an irritating day.[22][23] Yet many long-standing theories of how humans think rely on the prominence of analytical thought.[21]

By studying only W.E.I.R.D. populations, psychologists fail to account for a substantial amount of diversity of the global population as W.E.I.R.D. countries only represent 12% of the world's population.[20] Applying the findings from W.E.I.R.D. populations to other populations can lead to a miscalculation of psychological theories and may hinder psychologists' abilities to isolate fundamental cultural characteristics.

Mutual constitution edit

Mutual constitution is the notion that society and the individual have an influencing effect on one another. Because a society is composed of individuals, the behavior and actions of the individuals directly impact the society. In the same manner, society directly impacts the individual living within it. The values, morals, and ways of life a society exemplifies will have an immediate impact on the way an individual is shaped as a person. The atmosphere that a society provides for the individual is a determining factor for how an individual will develop. Furthermore, mutual constitution is a cyclical model in which the society and the individual both influence one another.[24]

While cultural psychology is reliant on this model, societies often fail to recognize this. Despite the overwhelming acceptance that people affect culture and that culture affects people, societal systems tend to minimize the effect that people form on their communities. For example, mission statements of businesses, schools, and foundations attempt to make promises regarding the environment and values that their establishment holds. However, these promises cannot be made in accordance with the mutually consisting theory without being upheld by all participants. The mission statement for the employees of Southwest Airlines, for example, claims that, "...We are committed to provide our Employees a stable work environment with equal opportunity for learning and personal growth".[25] While the company can ensure the "equal opportunity for learning and personal growth", the aforementioned message cannot be promised. The work environment that Southwest provides includes paying consumers. While rules can be enforced to ensure safety on their aircraft, customers will not be removed due to attitude or a lack of courtesy. This therefore contradicts the promise of a "stable work environment". On the contrary, some establishments do ensure that their mission statements agree with the mutually consistent model. For example, Yale University promises within its mission statement that:

Yale is committed to improving the world today and for future generations through outstanding research and scholarship, education, preservation, and practice. Yale educates aspiring leaders worldwide who serve all sectors of society. We carry out this mission through the free exchange of ideas in an ethical, interdependent, and diverse community of faculty, staff, students, and alumni.[26]
 
This image is a representation derived from ideas found in the journal article "Cultures and Selves: A Cycle of Mutual Constitution" by Hazel Rose Markus and Shinoba Kitayama.

Instead of making promises that depend on all of their students and faculty, they make statements that can refer to only a part of their student/ faculty body. The statement focuses more on what they offer, and how they uphold these promises. By providing evidence they provide readers with an example as to how their school community members participate in the environment they promise, accepting the community's role in their school culture.

Past research has been conducted by middle-class North Americans analyzing culturally different societies by using comparisons mostly involving middle-class North Americans and/or aforementioned W.E.I.R.D. societies. What has been characterized as Euro-American centrism, resulted in a great volume of research for this specific selection of humans. It has also allowed us to divert from the idea that certain psychological processes can be considered basic or universal, and recognize humans' remarkable capacity to create cultures and then be shaped by them.[27] Although cultural psychology has internalized the mutually constituting model, further implementation in our society is necessary. Being aware of this model promotes taking responsibility for one's actions and the effect that their actions have on their community. Through acceptance of ones responsibilities and conscious application, communities have opportunities for improvement which in turn supports the individuals within the community. These ideas can be found in the journal article "Cultures and Selves: A Cycle of Mutual Constitution" by Hazel Rose Markus and Shinobu Kitayama which are also represented in the graphic provided.

Criticisms edit

Stereotyping edit

One of the most significant themes in recent years has been cultural differences between East Asians and North Americans in attention,[28] perception,[29] cognition,[30] and social psychological phenomena such as the self.[31] Some psychologists, such as Turiel, have argued that this research is based on cultural stereotyping.[32] Psychologist Per Gjerde states that cultural psychology tends to "generalize about human development across nations and continents" and assigning characteristics to a culture promotes a disregard for heterogeneity and minimizes the role of the individual.[33] Gjerde argues that individuals develop multiple perspectives about their culture, sometimes act in accord with their culture without sharing cultural beliefs, and sometimes outright oppose their culture. Stereotyping thus views individuals as homogeneous products of culture.[34]

Faulty methodology edit

Self-reporting data is one of the easiest and most accessible methods of mass data collection, especially in cultural psychology.[28][35] However, overemphasizing cross-cultural comparisons of self-reported attitudes and values can lead to relatively unstable and ultimately misleading data.[36][37]

Methods edit

Cultural psychologist, Richard Shweder argues that the psyche and culture are mutually constructed and inseparable.[4] The failure of replicating many psychology findings in other regions of the world supported the idea that the mind and environment are interdependent and different throughout the world. Some criticisms state that using self-report may be a relatively unreliable method, and could be misleading especially in different cultural contexts. Regardless of the fact that self-report is an important way to obtain mass data, it is not the only way.

In fact, cultural psychologists utilized multiple measurements and resources no different from other scientific researches – observation, experiment, data analysis etc. For example, Nisbett & Cohen (1996) investigated the relation between historical cultural background and regional aggression difference in the U.S. In this study, researchers designed laboratory experiment to observe participants' aggression, and crime rate, demographic statistics were analyzed. The experiment results supported the culture of honor theory that the aggression is a defense mechanism which is rooted in the herding cultural origin for most of the southerners.[31] In laboratory observations, Heine and his colleagues found that Japanese students spend more time than American students on tasks that they did poorly on, and the finding presents a self-improvement motivation often seen in East Asian that failure and success is interconvertible with effort.[38] In terms of cognition styles, Chinese tend to perceive image using a holistic view compared to American.[39]

Quantitative statistics of cultural products revealed that public media in western countries promote more individualistic components than East-Asian countries.[40] These statistics are objective because it does not involve having people fill out questionnaire, instead, psychologists use physical measurements to quantitatively collect data about culture products, such as painting and photos. These statistics data can also be national records, for example, Chiao & Blizinsky (2010) revealed that cultures of high collectivism is associated with lower prevalence of mood/anxiety disorders in study involving 29 countries.[41] In addition to the experimental and statistics data, evidence from neuro-imaging studies, also help strengthen the reliability of cultural psychology research. For example, when thinking of mother, the brain region related to self-concept showed significant activation in Chinese, whereas no activation observed in Westerners.[42]

Cultural models edit

To understand the social world, people may use cultural models, which "consist of culturally derived ideas and practices that are embodied, enacted, or instituted in everyday life." Cultural psychologists develop models to categorize cultural phenomena.[43]

4 I's culture cycle edit

The 4 I's cultural model was developed by Hazel Rose Markus and Alana Conner in their book Clash! 8 Cultural Conflicts That Make Us Who We Are. In it, they refer to the mutually constitutive nature of culture and individual as a "culture cycle". The culture cycle consists of four layers (Individuals, Interactions, Institutions, Ideas) of cultural influence that help to explain the interaction between self and culture.[44]

Individuals edit

The first "I" concerns how an individual thinks about and expresses itself. Studies show that in the United States, individuals are more likely think of themselves as "independent", "equal", and "individualistic". Individuals have characteristics that are consistent across time and situation. When asked to describe themselves, Americans are likely to use adjectives to describe their personalities, such as "energetic", "friendly", or "hard-working". In Japan, studies show that individuals are more likely to think of themselves as "obligated to society", "interdependent", and "considerate". The self is adaptable to the situation. Japanese individuals are therefore more likely to describe themselves in relation to others, such as "I try not to upset anyone," or "I am a father, a son, and a brother."[45]

Interactions edit

Interactions with other people and products reinforce cultural behaviors on a daily basis. Stories, songs, architecture, and advertisements are all methods of interaction that guide individuals in a culture to promote certain values and teach them how to behave.[35] For example, in Japan, no-smoking signs emphasize the impact that smoke has on others by illustrating the path of smoke as it affects surrounding people. In the US, no-smoking signs focus on individual action by simply saying "No Smoking". These signs reflect underlying cultural norms and values, and when people see them they are encouraged to behave in accordance with the greater cultural values.

Institutions edit

The next layer of culture is made up of the institutions in which everyday interactions take place. These determine and enforce the rules for a society and include legal, government, economic, scientific, philosophical, and religious bodies. Institutions encourage certain practices and products while discouraging others. In Japanese kindergartens, children learn about important cultural values such as teamwork, group harmony, and cooperation. During "birthday month celebration," for example, the class celebrates all the children who have birthdays that month. This institutional practice underscores the importance of a group over an individual. In US kindergartens, children learn their personal value when they celebrate their birthdays one by one, enforcing the cultural value of uniqueness and individualism. Everyday institutional practices such as classroom birthday celebrations propagate prominent cultural themes.[35][45]

Ideas

The final cycle, which houses the highest and most abstract idea level of the cycle, focuses on the big ideas that each culture has which answers the big questions of life, such as Why are we here, where did we come from, and where are we going. The culture around the ideas is what gives structure to the answers and allows for a greater understanding of what is believed. In their book Hazel and Alana say, "In charting the course of your self, your postal code is just as important as your genetic code".[44] The culture of the idea is just as important as the idea itself.

Whiting model edit

John and Beatrice Whiting, along with their research students at Harvard University, developed the "Whiting model" for child development during the 1970s and 1980s, which specifically focused on how culture influences development.[46]

The Whitings coined the term "cultural learning environment", to describe the surroundings that influence a child during development.[47] Beatrice Whiting defined a child's environmental contexts as being "characterized by an activity in progress, a physically defined space, a characteristic group of people, and norms of behavior".[47] This environment is composed of several layers. A child's geographical context influences the history/anthropology of their greater community. This results in maintenance systems (i.e., sociological characteristics) that form a cultural learning environment. These factors inform learned behavior, or progressive expressive systems that take the form of religion, magic beliefs, ritual and ceremony, art, recreation, games and play, or crime rates.[48]

Many researchers have expanded upon the Whiting model,[46] and the Whiting model's influence is clear in both modern psychology and anthropology. According to an article by Thomas Weisner in the Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, "All these [more recent] approaches share a common intellectual project: to take culture and context deeply and seriously into account in studies of human development."[49]

Culture and motivation edit

Self-enhancement vs. self-improvement edit

While self-enhancement is a person's motivation to view themselves positively, self-improvement is a person's motivation to have others view themselves positively. The distinction between the two modes of life is most evident between independent and collectivistic cultures.[50] Cultures with independent self-views (the premise that people see themselves as self-contained entities) often emphasize self-esteem, confidence in one's own worth and abilities.[51] With self-esteem seen as a main source of happiness in Western cultures,[52] the motivation to self-enhance generally follows as a way to maintain one's positive view about oneself. Some strategies employed when self-enhancing often include downward social comparison, compensatory self-enhancement, discounting, external attributions and basking in reflected glory.[53] In contrast, collectivistic cultures often emphasize self-improvement as a leading motivating factor in their lives. This motivation is often derived from a desire to not lose face and to appear positively among social groups.[54]

Culture and empathy edit

Cultural orientation: collectivistic and individualistic edit

A main distinction to understand when looking at psychology and culture is the difference between individualistic and collectivistic cultures. People from an individualistic culture typically demonstrate an independent view of the self; the focus is usually on personal achievement.[55] Members of a collectivistic society have more of a focus on the group (interdependent view of self), usually focusing on things that will benefit the group.[56] Research has shown such differences of the self when comparing collectivistic and individualistic cultures: The Fundamental Attribution Error has been shown to be more common in America (individualistic) as compared to in India (collectivistic).[57] Along these same lines, the self-serving bias was again shown as more common among Americans than Japanese individuals.[58] This can be seen in a study involving an animation of fish, wherein Western viewers interpreted the scene of a fish swimming away from a school as an expression of individualism and independence, while Eastern individuals wondered what was wrong with the singular fish and concluded that the school had kicked it out.[59] Another study showed that in coverage of the same instance of violent crime, Western news focused on innate character flaws and the failings of the individual while Chinese news pointed out the lack of relationships of the perpetrator in a foreign environment and the failings of society.[59] This is not to imply that collectivism and individualism are completely dichotomous, but these two cultural orientations are to be understood more so as a spectrum. Each representation is at either end; thus, some members of individualistic cultures may hold collectivistic values, and some collectivistic individual may hold some individualist values. The concepts of collectivism and individualism show a general idea of the values of a specific ethnic culture but should not be juxtaposed in competition.[60]

Empathy across cultures edit

These differences in values across cultures suggests that understanding and expressing empathy may be manifested differently throughout varying cultures. Duan and Hill[61] first discussed empathy in subcategories of intellectual empathy: taking on someone's thoughts/perspective, also known as cognitive empathy[62] and emotional empathy: taking on someone's feeling/experience. Duan, Wei, and Wang[63] furthered this idea to include empathy in terms of being either dispositional (capacity for noticing/understanding empathy) or experiential (specific to a certain context or situation, observing the person and empathizing). This created four types of empathy to further examine: 1) dispositional intellectual empathy; 2) dispositional empathic emotion; 3) experienced intellectual empathy; and 4) experienced empathic emotion. These four branches allowed researchers to examine empathic proclivities among individuals of different cultures. While individualism was not shown to correlate with either types of dispositional empathy, collectivism was shown to have a direct correlation with both types of dispositional empathy, possibly suggesting that by having less focus on the self, there is more capacity towards noticing the needs of others. More so, individualism predicted experienced intellectual empathy, and collectivism predicted experienced empathic emotion. These results are congruent with the values of collectivistic and individualistic societies. The self-centered identity and egoistic motives prevalent in individualistic cultures perhaps acts as a hindrance in being open to (fully) experiencing empathy.[64] Many individuals tend to harbor dislike towards those from different cultural backgrounds, often fixating on these differences. Failing to comprehend the diversity of others significantly impedes our understanding of their lives. This may happen as a result of unawareness when an individual is behaving in such way.

Intercultural and ethnocultural empathy edit

Cultural empathy became broadly understood as concurrent understanding and acceptance of a culture different from one's own.[65] This idea has been further developed with the concept of ethnocultural empathy.[66] This moves beyond merely accepting and understanding another culture, and also includes acknowledging how the values of a culture may affect empathy. This idea is meant to foster cultural empathy as well as engender cultural competence.[66][67] One of the greatest barriers of empathy between cultures is people's tendency to operate from an ethnocentric point of view. Eysenck[68] conceptualized ethnocentrism as using one's own culture to understand the rest of the world, while holding one's own values as correct. Concomitant with this barrier to intercultural empathy, Rasoal, Eklund, and Hansen[69] posit five hindrances of intercultural empathy; these include:

Paucity of:

  • (general) knowledge outside one's own culture
  • (general) experience with other cultures outside one's own
  • (specific) knowledge regarding other people's cultures
  • (specific) experiences regarding other people's cultures

and:

  • inability to bridge different cultures by understanding the commonalities and dissimilarities

These five points elucidate lack of both depth and breadth as hindrances in developing and practicing intercultural empathy.

Another barrier to intercultural empathy is that there is often a power dynamic between different cultures. Bridging an oppressed culture with their (upper-echelon) oppressor is a goal of intercultural empathy. One approach to this barrier is to attempt to acknowledge one's personal oppression.[70] While this may be minimal in comparison to other people's oppression, it will still help with realizing that other people have been oppressed.[70] The goal of bridging the gap should focus on building an alliance by finding the core commonalities of the human experience; this shows empathy to be a relational experience, not an independent one. Through this, the goal is that intercultural empathy can lend toward broader intercultural understanding across cultures and societies.

Four important facets of cultural empathy are:[66]

  • Taking the perspective of someone from a different culture
  • Understanding the verbal/behavioral expression that occurs during ethnocultural empathy
  • Being cognizant of how different cultures are treated by larger entities such as the job market and the media
  • Accepting differences in cultural choices regarding language, clothing preference, food choice, etc.

These four aspects may be especially helpful for practicing cultural competence in a clinical setting. Given that most psychological practices were founded on the parochial ideals of Euro-American psychologists, cultural competence was not considered much of a necessity until said psychologists increasingly began seeing clients with different ethnic backgrounds.[67] Many of the problems that contribute to therapy not being beneficial for people of color include; therapy having an individual focus, an emphasis on expressiveness, and an emphasis on openness.[71] For more on intercultural competence, see intercultural competence.

Cultural Influences in the mental health treatment edit

In some studies, there has been a correlation between client comfort and their therapists sharing a similar ethnicity. This occurs because the client may feel more at ease or feel a stronger sense of connection with their therapists. From 2010 through 2015 there was a research study that concluded how important it is to have a variety of mental health care professionals in the work setting.[72] However, it is also true that the primary demographic receiving more mental health services compromises the majority population. This reflects the lack of universal accessibility to mental health care.[72] In the past years, we have observed an increase in validation and understanding of cultural psychology in the many aspects of life.

Nijmegen school edit

Already in 1956 the department of cultural psychology and psychology of religion was founded at the Radboud University of Nijmegen, the Netherlands.[73] One of its aims was to study culture and religion as psychological phenomena. In 1986 the department was split up in a Psychology of Religion section and a Cultural Psychology section. The research aim of the latter was to study culture as a behavior regulating system, which in fact implied that culture was no longer seen as an explanatory concept, but as something to be explained. Instead of viewing culture as a domain in its own right, as something separate from individual human beings, culture was seen as the product of human interaction leading to patterned behavior characteristic of human groups. It looks so self-evident, but this shift has wide-reaching implications. The expression: "culture of...." – and one can fill in whatever nation or group – can no longer be used to explain behaviors. One has to look for other determinants of behavior than the ones associated with 'culture'. Expressions like: 'it is our culture to put women in a dependent position and men above them' can no longer be used. Such a way of reasoning obscures the real determinants of the behavioral patterning that causes this sex and gender related state of affairs. The main publication in the department in which this view is elaborated is the book Culture as Embodiment.[74] In this book a tool kit is presented, which can be helpful in replacing the idea of culture as an explanatory variable with concepts and research instruments by means of which the behavioral patterning can be understood much better.

In 2020 an empirical program was launched by Ernst Graamans in his book Beyond the Idea of Culture: Understanding and Changing Cultural Practices in Business and Life Matters.[75] This dissertation at the Amsterdam Free University Business School of Economics explores so called 'cultural change' and related practices in business boardrooms, institutions of care, but also in the customs of female sexual mutilation in African communities. The defence of these practices in terms of: "it is our culture" is cogently criticized. In cases of communal female circumcision practices this empirical program makes the replacement of these practices by alternative rituals more viable.

Research institutions edit

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Heine, S. J. (2011). Cultural Psychology. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
  2. ^ Fiske, A.; Kitayama, S.; Markus, H.R.; & Nisbett, R.E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert & S. Fiske & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology (4th ed., pp. 915–81). San Francisco: McGraw-Hill.
  3. ^ Baumann, Gerd (1997). Dominant and demiotic discourses of culture. Their Relevance to Multi-Ethnic Alliances. In: P. Werbner & T. Modood (Eds.), Debating cultural hybridity. Multicultural Identities and the Politics of Anti-Racism. London & New Jersey: Zed Books.
  4. ^ a b c Shweder, Richard (1991). Thinking Through Cultures. Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-88415-9.
  5. ^ a b c d e Bock, Philip K. (2020-09-01). "Handbook of Cultural Psychology, second edition. Dov Cohen and Shinobo Kitayama, eds. New York: Guilford Press, 2019, 930 pp. $93.50, cloth. ISBN 9781462536238". Journal of Anthropological Research. 76 (3): 369–371. doi:10.1086/708409. ISSN 0091-7710. S2CID 243495819.
  6. ^ a b c d e f Slunecko, Thomas; Wieser, Martin (2014), "Cultural Psychology", in Teo, Thomas (ed.), Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology, New York, NY: Springer New York, pp. 347–352, doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-5583-7_64, ISBN 978-1-4614-5582-0, retrieved 2022-08-09
  7. ^ Holmes, Robyn M. (2020-03-02). "History of Cultural Psychology". Cultural Psychology. pp. 38–69. doi:10.1093/oso/9780199343805.003.0002. ISBN 978-0-19-934380-5.
  8. ^ Jahoda, Gustav (2012-05-02). Culture and Psychology: Words and Ideas in History. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396430.013.0002.
  9. ^ Danziger, Kurt (1983). "Origins and basic principles of Wundt's Völkerpsychologie". British Journal of Social Psychology. 22 (4): 303–313. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1983.tb00597.x.
  10. ^ a b Diriwächter, Rainer (2012-05-02). Völkerpsychologie. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396430.013.0003.
  11. ^ a b Jovanović, Gordana; Allolio-Näcke, Lars; Ratner, Carl (2018-09-28). Jovanović, Gordana; Allolio-Näcke, Lars; Ratner, Carl (eds.). The Challenges Of Cultural Psychology: Historical Legacies and Future Responsibilities (1 ed.). Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781315559667. ISBN 978-1-315-55966-7. S2CID 158949622.
  12. ^ Heine, S.; Ruby, M. B. (2010). "Cultural Psychology". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 1 (2): 254–266. doi:10.1002/wcs.7. PMID 26271239.
  13. ^ Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. (2003). "Culture, Self, and the Reality of the Social". Psychological Inquiry. 14 (3): 277–83. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1403&4_17.
  14. ^ Snibbe, Alana Conner (2003-12-10). "Cultural Psychology: Studying More Than the 'Exotic Other'". APS Observer. 16.
  15. ^ Shweder, R.A. & Levine, R.A., eds. (1984). Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  16. ^ Chakkarath, P. (2012). "The role of indigenous psychologies in the building of basic cultural psychology". In J. Valsiner (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 71–95. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396430.001.0001. ISBN 9780195396430.
  17. ^ Kleinman, Arthur (1982). "Neurasthenia and depression: A study of somatization and culture in China". Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry. 6 (2): 117–190. doi:10.1007/BF00051427. PMID 7116909. S2CID 23591895.
  18. ^ Hizi, Gil (2016). "Evading chronicity: Paradoxes in counseling psychology in contemporary China". Asian Anthropology. 15: 68–81. doi:10.1080/1683478X.2016.1164353. S2CID 147094764.
  19. ^ Arnett, J. J. (2008). (PDF). American Psychologist. 63 (7): 602–614. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.455.5296. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602. PMID 18855491. S2CID 21072349. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-08-08. Retrieved 2013-09-28.
  20. ^ a b c d Henrich, J.; Heine, S. J.; Norenzayan, A. (2010). "The weirdest people in the world?" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 33 (2–3): 61–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-26A1-6. PMID 20550733. S2CID 220918842.
  21. ^ a b c Henrich, Joseph (2010). "Most people are not WEIRD". Nature. 466 (5): 29. Bibcode:2010Natur.466...29H. doi:10.1038/466029a. PMID 20595995.
  22. ^ Jones, D. (2010). "A WEIRD View of Human Nature" (PDF). Science. 328 (25): 1627. doi:10.1126/science.328.5986.1627. PMID 20576866.
  23. ^ Nisbett, R.; Miyamoto, Y. (2005). "The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (10): 467–473. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.87.43. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.004. PMID 16129648. S2CID 231366.
  24. ^ Rogoff, Barbara (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development (Reprint ed.). Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-513133-8.
  25. ^ "About Southwest". Southwest Airlines.
  26. ^ "Mission Statement". Yale University. 2015-09-03.
  27. ^ Markus, Hazel Rose; Kitayama, Shinobu (2010). "Cultures and Selves: A Cycle of Mutual Constitution". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 5 (4): 420–430. doi:10.1177/1745691610375557. PMID 26162188. S2CID 7533754.
  28. ^ a b Masuda, T.; Nisbett, R.A. (2001). (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (5): 922–34. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922. PMID 11708567. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-12-24.
  29. ^ Kitayama, S.; Duffy, S.; Kawamura, T.; Larsen, J.T. (2003). (PDF). Psychological Science. 14 (3): 201–06. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02432. PMID 12741741. S2CID 13528749. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-12-24. Retrieved 2013-09-29.
  30. ^ Cole, M. (1998). Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  31. ^ a b Nisbett, R.E.; & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of Honor: The Psychology of Violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview Press.
  32. ^ Turiel, Elliott (2002). The Culture of Morality: Social Development, Context, and Conflict. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  33. ^ McNulty, Jennifer (2004, July 26). "Emphasis on 'culture' in psychology fuels stereotypes, scholar says." University of California: Santa Cruz.
  34. ^ Wainryb, C (2004). (PDF). Human Development. 47 (3): 131–137. doi:10.1159/000077986. S2CID 143136441. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-11-10.
  35. ^ a b c Kitayama, S.; et al. (2002). "Culture and basic psychological processes—Toward a system view of culture: Comment on Oyserman et al" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 128 (1): 89–96. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.89. PMID 11843550.
  36. ^ Heine, S.J.; Lehman, D.R.; Peng, K.; Greenholtz, J. (2002). "What's wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales: The reference-group problem" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (6): 903–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.903. PMID 12051579. S2CID 10077606.
  37. ^ Peng, K.; Nisbett, R.E.; Wong, N. (1997). "Validity problems of cross-cultural value comparison and possible solutions" (PDF). Psychological Methods. 2 (4): 329–41. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.2.4.329.
  38. ^ Heine, Steven (2012). Cultural psychology. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. pp. 253–254. ISBN 978-0-393-91283-8.
  39. ^ Jenkins, Lucas J.; Yang, Yung-Jui; Goh, Joshua; Hong, Ying-Yi; Park, Denise C. (2010-06-01). "Cultural differences in the lateral occipital complex while viewing incongruent scenes". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 5 (2–3): 236–241. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp056. ISSN 1749-5016. PMC 2894688. PMID 20083532.
  40. ^ Morling, Beth; Lamoreaux, Marika (2008-08-01). "Measuring Culture Outside the Head: A Meta-Analysis of Individualism—Collectivism in Cultural Products". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 12 (3): 199–221. doi:10.1177/1088868308318260. ISSN 1088-8683. PMID 18544712. S2CID 6673527.
  41. ^ Chiao, Joan Y.; Blizinsky, Katherine D. (2010-02-22). "Culture–gene coevolution of individualism–collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 277 (1681): 529–537. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1650. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 2842692. PMID 19864286.
  42. ^ Zhu, Ying; Zhang, Li; Fan, Jin; Han, Shihui (2007-02-01). "Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation". NeuroImage. 34 (3): 1310–1316. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.125.9234. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047. PMID 17134915. S2CID 11613104.
  43. ^ Fryberg, S.A.; Markus, H.R. (2007). "Cultural models of education in American Indian, Asian America, and European American contexts". Social Psychology of Education. 10 (2): 1381–2890. doi:10.1007/s11218-007-9017-z. S2CID 143689413.
  44. ^ a b Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. C. (2013). Clash! Eight Cultural Conflicts that Make Us Who We Are. New York: Penguin (Hudson Street Press).
  45. ^ a b Heine, S. (2011). Cultural Psychology. San Francisco: W. W. Norton & Co.
  46. ^ a b Worthman, C. M. (2010). "The Ecology of Human Development: Evolving Models for Cultural Psychology". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 41 (4): 546–562. doi:10.1177/0022022110362627. S2CID 4942595.
  47. ^ a b Edwards, Carolyn P. and Bloch, M. (2010). "The Whitings' Concepts of Culture and How They Have Fared in Contemporary Psychology and Anthropology." Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology.Paper 501.
  48. ^ John W. Berry, Ype H. Poortinga, Marshall H. Segall, Pierre R. Dasen, Cambridge University Press, 1992, Cross-Cultural Psychology: Research and Applications: Second Edition
  49. ^ Weisner, T.S. (2010). "John and Beatrice Whiting's Contributions to the Cross-Cultural Study of Human Development: Their Values, Goals, Norms, and Practices". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 41 (4): 499–509. doi:10.1177/0022022110362720. S2CID 145703685.
  50. ^ Heine, Steven (1999). "Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?". 2017 American Psychological Association.
  51. ^ "High self-esteem may be culturally universal, international study shows".
  52. ^ Kim, Hyunji (February 2016). "The Role of Positive Self-Evaluation on Cross-Cultural Differences in Well-Being". Cross-Cultural Research. 50: 85–99. doi:10.1177/1069397115617902. S2CID 146919675.
  53. ^ Heine, Steven (2010). "Cultural Psychology". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science. 1 (2): 254–266. doi:10.1002/wcs.7. PMID 26271239.
  54. ^ Heine, Steven J.; Raineri, Andres (January 2009). "Self-Improving Motivations and Collectivism". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 40 (1): 158–163. doi:10.1177/0022022108326193. S2CID 35773418.
  55. ^ Prooijen, J. (2013). "Individualistic and social motives for justice judgments". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1299 (1): 60–67. Bibcode:2013NYASA1299...60V. doi:10.1111/nyas.12143. PMID 25708080. S2CID 196583933.
  56. ^ Hui, C.H. (1988). "Measurement of individualism-collectivism". Journal of Research in Personality. 22 (1): 17–36. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(88)90022-0.
  57. ^ Ross (1977). "The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process". In Berkowitz, L. (ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (4th ed.). New York: Academic Press.
  58. ^ Kashima, Y.; Triandis, H.C. (1986). "The self-serving bias in attributions as a coping strategy: A cross-cultural study". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 17 (1): 83–97. doi:10.1177/0022002186017001006. S2CID 145222207.
  59. ^ a b Zimbardo, Philip. "Discovering Psychology: Cultural Psychology" (PDF). Learner.org. Retrieved 26 January 2018.
  60. ^ Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
  61. ^ Duan, C.; Hill, C.E. (1996). "The current state of empathy research". Journal of Counseling Psychology. 43 (3): 261–74. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.43.3.261.
  62. ^ Soto, J.A.; Levenson, R.W. (2009). "Emotion recognition across culture: The influence of ethnicity on empathic accuracy and physiological linkage". Emotion. 9 (6): 874–884. doi:10.1037/a0017399. PMC 2877627. PMID 20001130.
  63. ^ Duan, C.; Wei, M.; Wang, L. (2008). "The role of individualism-collectivism". Asian Journal of Counseling. 29 (3): 57–81.
  64. ^ Kitayama, S.; Markus, H.R. (1994). Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  65. ^ Ridely, C.R.; Lingle, D.W. (1996). "Cultural empathy in multicultural counseling: A multidimensional process model.". In Pedersen, P.B.; Draguns, J.G. (eds.). Counseling Across Culture. Thousands Oaks: CA: Sage.
  66. ^ a b c Wang, Y.W.; Blier, J.; Davidson, M.; Savoy, H.; Tan, J.; Tan, J.; Yakushka, O. (2003). "The scale of ethnocultural empathy: Development, validation, and reliability". Journal of Counseling Psychology. 50 (2): 221–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.221.
  67. ^ a b Dyche, L.; Zayas, L.H. (2001). "Cross-cultural empathy and training the contemporary psychotherapist". Clinical Social Work Journal. 29 (3): 245–258. doi:10.1023/A:1010407728614. S2CID 34783510.
  68. ^ Eysenck, M. (2000). Psychology: A student's handbook. Psychology Press LTD.
  69. ^ Raosal, C.; Eklund, J.; Hansen, E.M. (2011). "Toward a conceptualization of ethnocultural empathy". Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology. 5 (1): 1–13. doi:10.1037/h0099278.
  70. ^ a b DeTurk, S. (2001). "Intercultural empathy: Myth, competency, or possibility for alliance building?". Communication Building. 50 (4): 374–384. doi:10.1080/03634520109379262. S2CID 144949477.
  71. ^ Sue, D.W.; Sue, D. (1977). "Barriers to effective cross-cultural counseling". Journal of Counseling Psychology. 24 (5): 420–429. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.24.5.420.
  72. ^ a b Huang, Cindy Y; Zane, Nolan (April 2016). "Cultural influences in mental health treatment". Current Opinion in Psychology. 8: 131–136. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.009. ISSN 2352-250X. PMC 9528809. PMID 29506788.
  73. ^ Kempen, H., Paul Voestermans & V.J. Welten. De Nijmeegse cultuurpsychologie. Psychologisch Laboratorium, Universiteit Nijmegen, 1991.
  74. ^ The social tuning of behavior, written by Paul Voestermans and Theo Verheggen. Oxford, Blackwell, 2014.
  75. ^ Graamans, E. P. (2020). Beyond the idea of culture: Understanding and changing cultural practices in business and life matters (PhD-Thesis - Research and graduation internal).

Further reading edit

  • Kitayama, Shinobu, & Cohen, Dov (2010). Handbook of Cultural Psychology. Guilford.
  • Turiel, Elliot (2002). The Culture of Morality. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.
  • Cole, Michael (1996). Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press: Cambridge.
  • Matsumoto, D (Ed) (2001). The Handbook of Culture & Psychology. Oxford University Press: New York.
  • Shweder, R.A.; & Levine, R.A. (Eds., 1984). Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Triandis, H.C. (1989). (PDF). Psychological Review. 96 (3): 506–20. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506. S2CID 18670762. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-05-13.
  • Bruner, Jerome (1990). Acts of Meaning. Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-00360-8.
  • Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. (1991). "Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation" (PDF). Psychological Review. 98 (2): 224–53. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.1159. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.
  • Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture and the problem of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Nisbett, R.E.; Peng, K.; Choi, I.; Norenzayan, A. (2001). "Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition" (PDF). Psychological Review. 108 (2): 291–310. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291. PMID 11381831. S2CID 17739645.
  • Nisbett, R.E. (2003). The Geography of Thought. New York: Free Press.

cultural, psychology, this, section, tone, style, reflect, encyclopedic, tone, used, wikipedia, wikipedia, guide, writing, better, articles, suggestions, april, 2022, learn, when, remove, this, message, study, cultures, reflect, shape, their, members, psycholo. This section s tone or style may not reflect the encyclopedic tone used on Wikipedia See Wikipedia s guide to writing better articles for suggestions April 2022 Learn how and when to remove this message Cultural psychologyis the study of how cultures reflect and shape their members psychological processes 1 It is based on the premise that the mind and culture are inseparable and mutually constitutive The concept involves two propositions a people are shaped by their culture andb culture is shaped by its people 2 Cultural psychology aims to define culture its nature and its function concerning psychological phenomena Gerd Baumann argues Culture is not a real thing but an abstract analytical notion In itself it does not cause behavior but abstracts from it It is thus neither normative nor predictive but a heuristic means towards explaining how people understand and act upon the world 3 As Richard Shweder one of the major proponents of the field writes Cultural psychology is the study of how cultural traditions and social practices regulate express and transform the human psyche This results less in psychic unity for humankind than in ethnic divergences in mind self and emotion 4 Contents 1 History 2 Relationships with other branches of psychology 3 Importance 3 1 Need for expanded cultural research 4 Mutual constitution 5 Criticisms 5 1 Stereotyping 5 2 Faulty methodology 6 Methods 7 Cultural models 7 1 4 I s culture cycle 7 1 1 Individuals 7 1 2 Interactions 7 1 3 Institutions 7 2 Whiting model 8 Culture and motivation 8 1 Self enhancement vs self improvement 9 Culture and empathy 9 1 Cultural orientation collectivistic and individualistic 9 2 Empathy across cultures 9 3 Intercultural and ethnocultural empathy 9 4 Cultural Influences in the mental health treatment 10 Nijmegen school 11 Research institutions 12 See also 13 References 14 Further readingHistory editYoshihisa Kashima talks about cultural psychology in two senses as a tradition and as a movement that emerged in the late 20th century 5 6 Cultural psychology as a tradition is traced back to Western Romanticism in the 19th century 5 Giambatista Vico and Herder are seen as important early inspirations in thinking about the influence of culture on people 5 7 Its institutional origin started with the publication of the Zeitschrift fur Volkerpsychologie und Sprachwissenschaft first published in 1860 Wilhelm Wundt took this concept and his volume on Volkerpsychologie is one of the earliest accounts of a cultural perspective within the discipline of psychology 6 He saw Volkerpsychologie as a cultural developmental discipline that studied higher psychological processes in their social context The proposed methods were comparative and historical analyses 8 9 10 Another early cultural framework is cultural historical psychology which emerged in the 1920s It is mostly associated with the Russian psychologists Vygotsky Luria and Leont ev 6 They claimed that human activity is always embedded in a specific social and historical context and should therefore not be isolated 6 10 While in psychological research interest in culture had declined in part due to the popularity of behaviorism in the US some researchers in anthropology like Margaret Mead started to explore the interaction between culture and personality 5 6 In the 1970s 1980s there was an increasing call for an interpretive turn in anthropology and psychology Researchers were influenced by constructivist and relativist accounts of knowledge and argued that cultural differences should be understood within their contexts 5 11 This influence was an important factor in the emergence of the cultural psychology movement Leading scholars of this movement were among others Richard Shweder and Clifford Richards 11 The launch of a new journal and the publication of multiple major works like Shweder s Cultural Psychology and Cole s Cultural Psychology A Once and Future Discipline helped to shape the direction of the movement 6 Relationships with other branches of psychology editCultural psychology is often confused with cross cultural psychology Even though both fields influence each other cultural psychology is distinct from cross cultural psychology in that cross cultural psychologists generally use culture as a means of testing the universality of psychological processes rather than determining how local cultural practices shape psychological processes 12 So whereas a cross cultural psychologist might ask whether Jean Piaget s stages of development are universal across a variety of cultures a cultural psychologist would be interested in how the social practices of a particular set of cultures shape the development of cognitive processes in different ways 13 Cultural psychology research informs and is informed by several fields within psychology including cross cultural psychology social psychology cultural historical psychology developmental psychology and cognitive psychology In addition to drawing from several other fields of psychology cultural psychology in particular utilizes anthropologists linguists and philosophers to help in the pursuit of understanding a wide variety of cultural facets in a society 14 However the constructivist perspective of cultural psychology through which cultural psychologists study thought patterns and behaviors within and across cultures tends to clash with the universal perspectives common in most fields of psychology which seek to qualify fundamental psychological truths that are consistent across all of humanity Importance editNeed for expanded cultural research edit According to Richard Shweder there has been repeated failure to replicate Western psychology laboratory findings in non Western settings 4 Therefore a major goal of cultural psychology is to have many and varied cultures contribute to basic psychological theories in order to correct these theories so that they become more relevant to the predictions descriptions and explanations of all human behaviors not just Western ones 15 This goal is shared by many of the scholars who promote the indigenous psychology approach In an attempt to show the interrelated interests of cultural and indigenous psychology cultural psychologist Pradeep Chakkarath emphasizes that international mainstream psychology as it has been exported to most regions of the world by the so called West is only one among many indigenous psychologies and therefore may not have enough intercultural expertise to claim as it frequently does that its theories have universal validity 16 Accordingly cultural groups have diverse ways of defining emotional problems as well as distinguishing between physical and mental distress For example Arthur Kleinman has shown how the notion of depression in Chinese culture has been associated with physiological problems before becoming acknowledged more recently as an emotional concern 17 Furthermore the type of therapy people pursue is influenced by cultural conceptions of privacy and shame as well as the stigmas associated with specific problems 18 The acronym W E I R D describes populations that are Western Educated Industrialized Rich and Democratic Thus far W E I R D populations have been vastly overrepresented in psychological research 19 20 In an analysis of top journals in the psychology discipline it was found that 96 of subjects who participated in those studies came from Western Industrialized countries with 68 of them coming from the United States This is largely because 99 of the authors of these journals were at Western Universities with 73 of them at American Universities With this information it is concluded that 96 of psychological findings come from W E I R D countries 20 Findings from psychology research utilizing primarily W E I R D populations are often labeled as universal theories and are inaccurately applied to other cultures 21 Recent research is showing that cultures differ in many areas such as logical reasoning and social values 20 21 The evidence that basic cognitive and motivational processes vary across populations has become increasingly difficult to ignore For example many studies have shown that Americans Canadians and western Europeans rely on analytical reasoning strategies which separate objects from their contexts to explain and predict behavior Social psychologists refer to the fundamental attribution error or the tendency to explain people s behavior in terms of internal inherent personality traits rather than external situational considerations e g attributing an instance of angry behavior to an angry personality Outside W E I R D cultures however this phenomenon is less prominent as many non W E I R D populations tend to pay more attention to the context in which behavior occurs Asians tend to reason holistically for example by considering people s behavior in terms of their situation someone s anger might be viewed as simply a result of an irritating day 22 23 Yet many long standing theories of how humans think rely on the prominence of analytical thought 21 By studying only W E I R D populations psychologists fail to account for a substantial amount of diversity of the global population as W E I R D countries only represent 12 of the world s population 20 Applying the findings from W E I R D populations to other populations can lead to a miscalculation of psychological theories and may hinder psychologists abilities to isolate fundamental cultural characteristics Mutual constitution editMutual constitution is the notion that society and the individual have an influencing effect on one another Because a society is composed of individuals the behavior and actions of the individuals directly impact the society In the same manner society directly impacts the individual living within it The values morals and ways of life a society exemplifies will have an immediate impact on the way an individual is shaped as a person The atmosphere that a society provides for the individual is a determining factor for how an individual will develop Furthermore mutual constitution is a cyclical model in which the society and the individual both influence one another 24 While cultural psychology is reliant on this model societies often fail to recognize this Despite the overwhelming acceptance that people affect culture and that culture affects people societal systems tend to minimize the effect that people form on their communities For example mission statements of businesses schools and foundations attempt to make promises regarding the environment and values that their establishment holds However these promises cannot be made in accordance with the mutually consisting theory without being upheld by all participants The mission statement for the employees of Southwest Airlines for example claims that We are committed to provide our Employees a stable work environment with equal opportunity for learning and personal growth 25 While the company can ensure the equal opportunity for learning and personal growth the aforementioned message cannot be promised The work environment that Southwest provides includes paying consumers While rules can be enforced to ensure safety on their aircraft customers will not be removed due to attitude or a lack of courtesy This therefore contradicts the promise of a stable work environment On the contrary some establishments do ensure that their mission statements agree with the mutually consistent model For example Yale University promises within its mission statement that Yale is committed to improving the world today and for future generations through outstanding research and scholarship education preservation and practice Yale educates aspiring leaders worldwide who serve all sectors of society We carry out this mission through the free exchange of ideas in an ethical interdependent and diverse community of faculty staff students and alumni 26 dd nbsp This image is a representation derived from ideas found in the journal article Cultures and Selves A Cycle of Mutual Constitution by Hazel Rose Markus and Shinoba Kitayama Instead of making promises that depend on all of their students and faculty they make statements that can refer to only a part of their student faculty body The statement focuses more on what they offer and how they uphold these promises By providing evidence they provide readers with an example as to how their school community members participate in the environment they promise accepting the community s role in their school culture Past research has been conducted by middle class North Americans analyzing culturally different societies by using comparisons mostly involving middle class North Americans and or aforementioned W E I R D societies What has been characterized as Euro American centrism resulted in a great volume of research for this specific selection of humans It has also allowed us to divert from the idea that certain psychological processes can be considered basic or universal and recognize humans remarkable capacity to create cultures and then be shaped by them 27 Although cultural psychology has internalized the mutually constituting model further implementation in our society is necessary Being aware of this model promotes taking responsibility for one s actions and the effect that their actions have on their community Through acceptance of ones responsibilities and conscious application communities have opportunities for improvement which in turn supports the individuals within the community These ideas can be found in the journal article Cultures and Selves A Cycle of Mutual Constitution by Hazel Rose Markus and Shinobu Kitayama which are also represented in the graphic provided Criticisms editStereotyping edit One of the most significant themes in recent years has been cultural differences between East Asians and North Americans in attention 28 perception 29 cognition 30 and social psychological phenomena such as the self 31 Some psychologists such as Turiel have argued that this research is based on cultural stereotyping 32 Psychologist Per Gjerde states that cultural psychology tends to generalize about human development across nations and continents and assigning characteristics to a culture promotes a disregard for heterogeneity and minimizes the role of the individual 33 Gjerde argues that individuals develop multiple perspectives about their culture sometimes act in accord with their culture without sharing cultural beliefs and sometimes outright oppose their culture Stereotyping thus views individuals as homogeneous products of culture 34 Faulty methodology edit Self reporting data is one of the easiest and most accessible methods of mass data collection especially in cultural psychology 28 35 However overemphasizing cross cultural comparisons of self reported attitudes and values can lead to relatively unstable and ultimately misleading data 36 37 Methods editCultural psychologist Richard Shweder argues that the psyche and culture are mutually constructed and inseparable 4 The failure of replicating many psychology findings in other regions of the world supported the idea that the mind and environment are interdependent and different throughout the world Some criticisms state that using self report may be a relatively unreliable method and could be misleading especially in different cultural contexts Regardless of the fact that self report is an important way to obtain mass data it is not the only way In fact cultural psychologists utilized multiple measurements and resources no different from other scientific researches observation experiment data analysis etc For example Nisbett amp Cohen 1996 investigated the relation between historical cultural background and regional aggression difference in the U S In this study researchers designed laboratory experiment to observe participants aggression and crime rate demographic statistics were analyzed The experiment results supported the culture of honor theory that the aggression is a defense mechanism which is rooted in the herding cultural origin for most of the southerners 31 In laboratory observations Heine and his colleagues found that Japanese students spend more time than American students on tasks that they did poorly on and the finding presents a self improvement motivation often seen in East Asian that failure and success is interconvertible with effort 38 In terms of cognition styles Chinese tend to perceive image using a holistic view compared to American 39 Quantitative statistics of cultural products revealed that public media in western countries promote more individualistic components than East Asian countries 40 These statistics are objective because it does not involve having people fill out questionnaire instead psychologists use physical measurements to quantitatively collect data about culture products such as painting and photos These statistics data can also be national records for example Chiao amp Blizinsky 2010 revealed that cultures of high collectivism is associated with lower prevalence of mood anxiety disorders in study involving 29 countries 41 In addition to the experimental and statistics data evidence from neuro imaging studies also help strengthen the reliability of cultural psychology research For example when thinking of mother the brain region related to self concept showed significant activation in Chinese whereas no activation observed in Westerners 42 Cultural models editTo understand the social world people may use cultural models which consist of culturally derived ideas and practices that are embodied enacted or instituted in everyday life Cultural psychologists develop models to categorize cultural phenomena 43 4 I s culture cycle edit The 4 I s cultural model was developed by Hazel Rose Markus and Alana Conner in their book Clash 8 Cultural Conflicts That Make Us Who We Are In it they refer to the mutually constitutive nature of culture and individual as a culture cycle The culture cycle consists of four layers Individuals Interactions Institutions Ideas of cultural influence that help to explain the interaction between self and culture 44 Individuals edit The first I concerns how an individual thinks about and expresses itself Studies show that in the United States individuals are more likely think of themselves as independent equal and individualistic Individuals have characteristics that are consistent across time and situation When asked to describe themselves Americans are likely to use adjectives to describe their personalities such as energetic friendly or hard working In Japan studies show that individuals are more likely to think of themselves as obligated to society interdependent and considerate The self is adaptable to the situation Japanese individuals are therefore more likely to describe themselves in relation to others such as I try not to upset anyone or I am a father a son and a brother 45 Interactions edit Interactions with other people and products reinforce cultural behaviors on a daily basis Stories songs architecture and advertisements are all methods of interaction that guide individuals in a culture to promote certain values and teach them how to behave 35 For example in Japan no smoking signs emphasize the impact that smoke has on others by illustrating the path of smoke as it affects surrounding people In the US no smoking signs focus on individual action by simply saying No Smoking These signs reflect underlying cultural norms and values and when people see them they are encouraged to behave in accordance with the greater cultural values Institutions edit The next layer of culture is made up of the institutions in which everyday interactions take place These determine and enforce the rules for a society and include legal government economic scientific philosophical and religious bodies Institutions encourage certain practices and products while discouraging others In Japanese kindergartens children learn about important cultural values such as teamwork group harmony and cooperation During birthday month celebration for example the class celebrates all the children who have birthdays that month This institutional practice underscores the importance of a group over an individual In US kindergartens children learn their personal value when they celebrate their birthdays one by one enforcing the cultural value of uniqueness and individualism Everyday institutional practices such as classroom birthday celebrations propagate prominent cultural themes 35 45 IdeasThe final cycle which houses the highest and most abstract idea level of the cycle focuses on the big ideas that each culture has which answers the big questions of life such as Why are we here where did we come from and where are we going The culture around the ideas is what gives structure to the answers and allows for a greater understanding of what is believed In their book Hazel and Alana say In charting the course of your self your postal code is just as important as your genetic code 44 The culture of the idea is just as important as the idea itself Whiting model edit John and Beatrice Whiting along with their research students at Harvard University developed the Whiting model for child development during the 1970s and 1980s which specifically focused on how culture influences development 46 The Whitings coined the term cultural learning environment to describe the surroundings that influence a child during development 47 Beatrice Whiting defined a child s environmental contexts as being characterized by an activity in progress a physically defined space a characteristic group of people and norms of behavior 47 This environment is composed of several layers A child s geographical context influences the history anthropology of their greater community This results in maintenance systems i e sociological characteristics that form a cultural learning environment These factors inform learned behavior or progressive expressive systems that take the form of religion magic beliefs ritual and ceremony art recreation games and play or crime rates 48 Many researchers have expanded upon the Whiting model 46 and the Whiting model s influence is clear in both modern psychology and anthropology According to an article by Thomas Weisner in the Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology All these more recent approaches share a common intellectual project to take culture and context deeply and seriously into account in studies of human development 49 Culture and motivation editSelf enhancement vs self improvement edit While self enhancement is a person s motivation to view themselves positively self improvement is a person s motivation to have others view themselves positively The distinction between the two modes of life is most evident between independent and collectivistic cultures 50 Cultures with independent self views the premise that people see themselves as self contained entities often emphasize self esteem confidence in one s own worth and abilities 51 With self esteem seen as a main source of happiness in Western cultures 52 the motivation to self enhance generally follows as a way to maintain one s positive view about oneself Some strategies employed when self enhancing often include downward social comparison compensatory self enhancement discounting external attributions and basking in reflected glory 53 In contrast collectivistic cultures often emphasize self improvement as a leading motivating factor in their lives This motivation is often derived from a desire to not lose face and to appear positively among social groups 54 Culture and empathy editCultural orientation collectivistic and individualistic edit A main distinction to understand when looking at psychology and culture is the difference between individualistic and collectivistic cultures People from an individualistic culture typically demonstrate an independent view of the self the focus is usually on personal achievement 55 Members of a collectivistic society have more of a focus on the group interdependent view of self usually focusing on things that will benefit the group 56 Research has shown such differences of the self when comparing collectivistic and individualistic cultures The Fundamental Attribution Error has been shown to be more common in America individualistic as compared to in India collectivistic 57 Along these same lines the self serving bias was again shown as more common among Americans than Japanese individuals 58 This can be seen in a study involving an animation of fish wherein Western viewers interpreted the scene of a fish swimming away from a school as an expression of individualism and independence while Eastern individuals wondered what was wrong with the singular fish and concluded that the school had kicked it out 59 Another study showed that in coverage of the same instance of violent crime Western news focused on innate character flaws and the failings of the individual while Chinese news pointed out the lack of relationships of the perpetrator in a foreign environment and the failings of society 59 This is not to imply that collectivism and individualism are completely dichotomous but these two cultural orientations are to be understood more so as a spectrum Each representation is at either end thus some members of individualistic cultures may hold collectivistic values and some collectivistic individual may hold some individualist values The concepts of collectivism and individualism show a general idea of the values of a specific ethnic culture but should not be juxtaposed in competition 60 Empathy across cultures edit These differences in values across cultures suggests that understanding and expressing empathy may be manifested differently throughout varying cultures Duan and Hill 61 first discussed empathy in subcategories of intellectual empathy taking on someone s thoughts perspective also known as cognitive empathy 62 and emotional empathy taking on someone s feeling experience Duan Wei and Wang 63 furthered this idea to include empathy in terms of being either dispositional capacity for noticing understanding empathy or experiential specific to a certain context or situation observing the person and empathizing This created four types of empathy to further examine 1 dispositional intellectual empathy 2 dispositional empathic emotion 3 experienced intellectual empathy and 4 experienced empathic emotion These four branches allowed researchers to examine empathic proclivities among individuals of different cultures While individualism was not shown to correlate with either types of dispositional empathy collectivism was shown to have a direct correlation with both types of dispositional empathy possibly suggesting that by having less focus on the self there is more capacity towards noticing the needs of others More so individualism predicted experienced intellectual empathy and collectivism predicted experienced empathic emotion These results are congruent with the values of collectivistic and individualistic societies The self centered identity and egoistic motives prevalent in individualistic cultures perhaps acts as a hindrance in being open to fully experiencing empathy 64 Many individuals tend to harbor dislike towards those from different cultural backgrounds often fixating on these differences Failing to comprehend the diversity of others significantly impedes our understanding of their lives This may happen as a result of unawareness when an individual is behaving in such way Intercultural and ethnocultural empathy edit Cultural empathy became broadly understood as concurrent understanding and acceptance of a culture different from one s own 65 This idea has been further developed with the concept of ethnocultural empathy 66 This moves beyond merely accepting and understanding another culture and also includes acknowledging how the values of a culture may affect empathy This idea is meant to foster cultural empathy as well as engender cultural competence 66 67 One of the greatest barriers of empathy between cultures is people s tendency to operate from an ethnocentric point of view Eysenck 68 conceptualized ethnocentrism as using one s own culture to understand the rest of the world while holding one s own values as correct Concomitant with this barrier to intercultural empathy Rasoal Eklund and Hansen 69 posit five hindrances of intercultural empathy these include Paucity of general knowledge outside one s own culture general experience with other cultures outside one s own specific knowledge regarding other people s cultures specific experiences regarding other people s cultures and inability to bridge different cultures by understanding the commonalities and dissimilarities These five points elucidate lack of both depth and breadth as hindrances in developing and practicing intercultural empathy Another barrier to intercultural empathy is that there is often a power dynamic between different cultures Bridging an oppressed culture with their upper echelon oppressor is a goal of intercultural empathy One approach to this barrier is to attempt to acknowledge one s personal oppression 70 While this may be minimal in comparison to other people s oppression it will still help with realizing that other people have been oppressed 70 The goal of bridging the gap should focus on building an alliance by finding the core commonalities of the human experience this shows empathy to be a relational experience not an independent one Through this the goal is that intercultural empathy can lend toward broader intercultural understanding across cultures and societies Four important facets of cultural empathy are 66 Taking the perspective of someone from a different culture Understanding the verbal behavioral expression that occurs during ethnocultural empathy Being cognizant of how different cultures are treated by larger entities such as the job market and the media Accepting differences in cultural choices regarding language clothing preference food choice etc These four aspects may be especially helpful for practicing cultural competence in a clinical setting Given that most psychological practices were founded on the parochial ideals of Euro American psychologists cultural competence was not considered much of a necessity until said psychologists increasingly began seeing clients with different ethnic backgrounds 67 Many of the problems that contribute to therapy not being beneficial for people of color include therapy having an individual focus an emphasis on expressiveness and an emphasis on openness 71 For more on intercultural competence see intercultural competence Cultural Influences in the mental health treatment edit In some studies there has been a correlation between client comfort and their therapists sharing a similar ethnicity This occurs because the client may feel more at ease or feel a stronger sense of connection with their therapists From 2010 through 2015 there was a research study that concluded how important it is to have a variety of mental health care professionals in the work setting 72 However it is also true that the primary demographic receiving more mental health services compromises the majority population This reflects the lack of universal accessibility to mental health care 72 In the past years we have observed an increase in validation and understanding of cultural psychology in the many aspects of life Nijmegen school editAlready in 1956 the department of cultural psychology and psychology of religion was founded at the Radboud University of Nijmegen the Netherlands 73 One of its aims was to study culture and religion as psychological phenomena In 1986 the department was split up in a Psychology of Religion section and a Cultural Psychology section The research aim of the latter was to study culture as a behavior regulating system which in fact implied that culture was no longer seen as an explanatory concept but as something to be explained Instead of viewing culture as a domain in its own right as something separate from individual human beings culture was seen as the product of human interaction leading to patterned behavior characteristic of human groups It looks so self evident but this shift has wide reaching implications The expression culture of and one can fill in whatever nation or group can no longer be used to explain behaviors One has to look for other determinants of behavior than the ones associated with culture Expressions like it is our culture to put women in a dependent position and men above them can no longer be used Such a way of reasoning obscures the real determinants of the behavioral patterning that causes this sex and gender related state of affairs The main publication in the department in which this view is elaborated is the book Culture as Embodiment 74 In this book a tool kit is presented which can be helpful in replacing the idea of culture as an explanatory variable with concepts and research instruments by means of which the behavioral patterning can be understood much better In 2020 an empirical program was launched by Ernst Graamans in his book Beyond the Idea of Culture Understanding and Changing Cultural Practices in Business and Life Matters 75 This dissertation at the Amsterdam Free University Business School of Economics explores so called cultural change and related practices in business boardrooms institutions of care but also in the customs of female sexual mutilation in African communities The defence of these practices in terms of it is our culture is cogently criticized In cases of communal female circumcision practices this empirical program makes the replacement of these practices by alternative rituals more viable Research institutions editInstitute of Cultural Psychology and Qualitative Social Research ikus Institute of Psychology Sigmund Freud University Vienna Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition LCHC Culture and Cognition University of Michigan Centre for Cultural Psychology Aalborg University Hans Kilian and Lotte Kohler Center for Cultural Psychology and Historical Anthropology KKC Culture and Self Lab University of British ColumbiaSee also editCultural historical activity theory Indian psychologyReferences edit Heine S J 2011 Cultural Psychology New York W W Norton amp Company Fiske A Kitayama S Markus H R amp Nisbett R E 1998 The cultural matrix of social psychology In D Gilbert amp S Fiske amp G Lindzey Eds The Handbook of Social Psychology 4th ed pp 915 81 San Francisco McGraw Hill Baumann Gerd 1997 Dominant and demiotic discourses of culture Their Relevance to Multi Ethnic Alliances In P Werbner amp T Modood Eds Debating cultural hybridity Multicultural Identities and the Politics of Anti Racism London amp New Jersey Zed Books a b c Shweder Richard 1991 Thinking Through Cultures Harvard University Press ISBN 0 674 88415 9 a b c d e Bock Philip K 2020 09 01 Handbook of Cultural Psychology second edition Dov Cohen and Shinobo Kitayama eds New York Guilford Press 2019 930 pp 93 50 cloth ISBN 9781462536238 Journal of Anthropological Research 76 3 369 371 doi 10 1086 708409 ISSN 0091 7710 S2CID 243495819 a b c d e f Slunecko Thomas Wieser Martin 2014 Cultural Psychology in Teo Thomas ed Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology New York NY Springer New York pp 347 352 doi 10 1007 978 1 4614 5583 7 64 ISBN 978 1 4614 5582 0 retrieved 2022 08 09 Holmes Robyn M 2020 03 02 History of Cultural Psychology Cultural Psychology pp 38 69 doi 10 1093 oso 9780199343805 003 0002 ISBN 978 0 19 934380 5 Jahoda Gustav 2012 05 02 Culture and Psychology Words and Ideas in History Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780195396430 013 0002 Danziger Kurt 1983 Origins and basic principles of Wundt s Volkerpsychologie British Journal of Social Psychology 22 4 303 313 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8309 1983 tb00597 x a b Diriwachter Rainer 2012 05 02 Volkerpsychologie Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780195396430 013 0003 a b Jovanovic Gordana Allolio Nacke Lars Ratner Carl 2018 09 28 Jovanovic Gordana Allolio Nacke Lars Ratner Carl eds The Challenges Of Cultural Psychology Historical Legacies and Future Responsibilities 1 ed Routledge doi 10 4324 9781315559667 ISBN 978 1 315 55966 7 S2CID 158949622 Heine S Ruby M B 2010 Cultural Psychology Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 1 2 254 266 doi 10 1002 wcs 7 PMID 26271239 Markus H R Kitayama S 2003 Culture Self and the Reality of the Social Psychological Inquiry 14 3 277 83 doi 10 1207 S15327965PLI1403 amp 4 17 Snibbe Alana Conner 2003 12 10 Cultural Psychology Studying More Than the Exotic Other APS Observer 16 Shweder R A amp Levine R A eds 1984 Culture theory Essays on mind self and emotion New York Cambridge University Press Chakkarath P 2012 The role of indigenous psychologies in the building of basic cultural psychology In J Valsiner ed The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology New York Oxford University Press pp 71 95 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780195396430 001 0001 ISBN 9780195396430 Kleinman Arthur 1982 Neurasthenia and depression A study of somatization and culture in China Culture Medicine and Psychiatry 6 2 117 190 doi 10 1007 BF00051427 PMID 7116909 S2CID 23591895 Hizi Gil 2016 Evading chronicity Paradoxes in counseling psychology in contemporary China Asian Anthropology 15 68 81 doi 10 1080 1683478X 2016 1164353 S2CID 147094764 Arnett J J 2008 The neglected 95 Why American psychology needs to become less American PDF American Psychologist 63 7 602 614 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 455 5296 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 63 7 602 PMID 18855491 S2CID 21072349 Archived from the original PDF on 2016 08 08 Retrieved 2013 09 28 a b c d Henrich J Heine S J Norenzayan A 2010 The weirdest people in the world PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 2 3 61 135 doi 10 1017 S0140525X0999152X hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0013 26A1 6 PMID 20550733 S2CID 220918842 a b c Henrich Joseph 2010 Most people are not WEIRD Nature 466 5 29 Bibcode 2010Natur 466 29H doi 10 1038 466029a PMID 20595995 Jones D 2010 A WEIRD View of Human Nature PDF Science 328 25 1627 doi 10 1126 science 328 5986 1627 PMID 20576866 Nisbett R Miyamoto Y 2005 The influence of culture holistic versus analytic perception Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 10 467 473 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 87 43 doi 10 1016 j tics 2005 08 004 PMID 16129648 S2CID 231366 Rogoff Barbara 2003 The Cultural Nature of Human Development Reprint ed Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 513133 8 About Southwest Southwest Airlines Mission Statement Yale University 2015 09 03 Markus Hazel Rose Kitayama Shinobu 2010 Cultures and Selves A Cycle of Mutual Constitution Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 4 420 430 doi 10 1177 1745691610375557 PMID 26162188 S2CID 7533754 a b Masuda T Nisbett R A 2001 Attending holistically versus analytically Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 5 922 34 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 5 922 PMID 11708567 Archived from the original PDF on 2013 12 24 Kitayama S Duffy S Kawamura T Larsen J T 2003 Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures A cultural look at new look PDF Psychological Science 14 3 201 06 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 02432 PMID 12741741 S2CID 13528749 Archived from the original PDF on 2013 12 24 Retrieved 2013 09 29 Cole M 1998 Cultural Psychology A Once and Future Discipline Cambridge Harvard University Press a b Nisbett R E amp Cohen D 1996 Culture of Honor The Psychology of Violence in the South Denver CO Westview Press Turiel Elliott 2002 The Culture of Morality Social Development Context and Conflict Cambridge Cambridge University Press McNulty Jennifer 2004 July 26 Emphasis on culture in psychology fuels stereotypes scholar says University of California Santa Cruz Wainryb C 2004 The Study of Diversity in Human Development Culture Urgencies and Perils PDF Human Development 47 3 131 137 doi 10 1159 000077986 S2CID 143136441 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 11 10 a b c Kitayama S et al 2002 Culture and basic psychological processes Toward a system view of culture Comment on Oyserman et al PDF Psychological Bulletin 128 1 89 96 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 128 1 89 PMID 11843550 Heine S J Lehman D R Peng K Greenholtz J 2002 What s wrong with cross cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales The reference group problem PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82 6 903 18 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 82 6 903 PMID 12051579 S2CID 10077606 Peng K Nisbett R E Wong N 1997 Validity problems of cross cultural value comparison and possible solutions PDF Psychological Methods 2 4 329 41 doi 10 1037 1082 989X 2 4 329 Heine Steven 2012 Cultural psychology New York W W Norton amp Company Inc pp 253 254 ISBN 978 0 393 91283 8 Jenkins Lucas J Yang Yung Jui Goh Joshua Hong Ying Yi Park Denise C 2010 06 01 Cultural differences in the lateral occipital complex while viewing incongruent scenes Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 2 3 236 241 doi 10 1093 scan nsp056 ISSN 1749 5016 PMC 2894688 PMID 20083532 Morling Beth Lamoreaux Marika 2008 08 01 Measuring Culture Outside the Head A Meta Analysis of Individualism Collectivism in Cultural Products Personality and Social Psychology Review 12 3 199 221 doi 10 1177 1088868308318260 ISSN 1088 8683 PMID 18544712 S2CID 6673527 Chiao Joan Y Blizinsky Katherine D 2010 02 22 Culture gene coevolution of individualism collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences 277 1681 529 537 doi 10 1098 rspb 2009 1650 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 2842692 PMID 19864286 Zhu Ying Zhang Li Fan Jin Han Shihui 2007 02 01 Neural basis of cultural influence on self representation NeuroImage 34 3 1310 1316 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 125 9234 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2006 08 047 PMID 17134915 S2CID 11613104 Fryberg S A Markus H R 2007 Cultural models of education in American Indian Asian America and European American contexts Social Psychology of Education 10 2 1381 2890 doi 10 1007 s11218 007 9017 z S2CID 143689413 a b Markus H R amp Conner A C 2013 Clash Eight Cultural Conflicts that Make Us Who We Are New York Penguin Hudson Street Press a b Heine S 2011 Cultural Psychology San Francisco W W Norton amp Co a b Worthman C M 2010 The Ecology of Human Development Evolving Models for Cultural Psychology Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 41 4 546 562 doi 10 1177 0022022110362627 S2CID 4942595 a b Edwards Carolyn P and Bloch M 2010 The Whitings Concepts of Culture and How They Have Fared in Contemporary Psychology and Anthropology Faculty Publications Department of Psychology Paper 501 John W Berry Ype H Poortinga Marshall H Segall Pierre R Dasen Cambridge University Press 1992 Cross Cultural Psychology Research and Applications Second Edition Weisner T S 2010 John and Beatrice Whiting s Contributions to the Cross Cultural Study of Human Development Their Values Goals Norms and Practices Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 41 4 499 509 doi 10 1177 0022022110362720 S2CID 145703685 Heine Steven 1999 Is there a universal need for positive self regard 2017 American Psychological Association High self esteem may be culturally universal international study shows Kim Hyunji February 2016 The Role of Positive Self Evaluation on Cross Cultural Differences in Well Being Cross Cultural Research 50 85 99 doi 10 1177 1069397115617902 S2CID 146919675 Heine Steven 2010 Cultural Psychology Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 1 2 254 266 doi 10 1002 wcs 7 PMID 26271239 Heine Steven J Raineri Andres January 2009 Self Improving Motivations and Collectivism Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 40 1 158 163 doi 10 1177 0022022108326193 S2CID 35773418 Prooijen J 2013 Individualistic and social motives for justice judgments Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1299 1 60 67 Bibcode 2013NYASA1299 60V doi 10 1111 nyas 12143 PMID 25708080 S2CID 196583933 Hui C H 1988 Measurement of individualism collectivism Journal of Research in Personality 22 1 17 36 doi 10 1016 0092 6566 88 90022 0 Ross 1977 The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings Distortions in the attribution process In Berkowitz L ed Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 4th ed New York Academic Press Kashima Y Triandis H C 1986 The self serving bias in attributions as a coping strategy A cross cultural study Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 17 1 83 97 doi 10 1177 0022002186017001006 S2CID 145222207 a b Zimbardo Philip Discovering Psychology Cultural Psychology PDF Learner org Retrieved 26 January 2018 Hofstede G 1980 Culture s consequences International differences in work related values Beverly Hills CA Sage Duan C Hill C E 1996 The current state of empathy research Journal of Counseling Psychology 43 3 261 74 doi 10 1037 0022 0167 43 3 261 Soto J A Levenson R W 2009 Emotion recognition across culture The influence of ethnicity on empathic accuracy and physiological linkage Emotion 9 6 874 884 doi 10 1037 a0017399 PMC 2877627 PMID 20001130 Duan C Wei M Wang L 2008 The role of individualism collectivism Asian Journal of Counseling 29 3 57 81 Kitayama S Markus H R 1994 Emotion and culture Empirical studies of mutual influence Washington DC American Psychological Association Ridely C R Lingle D W 1996 Cultural empathy in multicultural counseling A multidimensional process model In Pedersen P B Draguns J G eds Counseling Across Culture Thousands Oaks CA Sage a b c Wang Y W Blier J Davidson M Savoy H Tan J Tan J Yakushka O 2003 The scale of ethnocultural empathy Development validation and reliability Journal of Counseling Psychology 50 2 221 234 doi 10 1037 0022 0167 50 2 221 a b Dyche L Zayas L H 2001 Cross cultural empathy and training the contemporary psychotherapist Clinical Social Work Journal 29 3 245 258 doi 10 1023 A 1010407728614 S2CID 34783510 Eysenck M 2000 Psychology A student s handbook Psychology Press LTD Raosal C Eklund J Hansen E M 2011 Toward a conceptualization of ethnocultural empathy Journal of Social Evolutionary and Cultural Psychology 5 1 1 13 doi 10 1037 h0099278 a b DeTurk S 2001 Intercultural empathy Myth competency or possibility for alliance building Communication Building 50 4 374 384 doi 10 1080 03634520109379262 S2CID 144949477 Sue D W Sue D 1977 Barriers to effective cross cultural counseling Journal of Counseling Psychology 24 5 420 429 doi 10 1037 0022 0167 24 5 420 a b Huang Cindy Y Zane Nolan April 2016 Cultural influences in mental health treatment Current Opinion in Psychology 8 131 136 doi 10 1016 j copsyc 2015 10 009 ISSN 2352 250X PMC 9528809 PMID 29506788 Kempen H Paul Voestermans amp V J Welten De Nijmeegse cultuurpsychologie Psychologisch Laboratorium Universiteit Nijmegen 1991 The social tuning of behavior written by Paul Voestermans and Theo Verheggen Oxford Blackwell 2014 Graamans E P 2020 Beyond the idea of culture Understanding and changing cultural practices in business and life matters PhD Thesis Research and graduation internal Further reading editLibrary resources about Cultural psychology Resources in your library Resources in other libraries Kitayama Shinobu amp Cohen Dov 2010 Handbook of Cultural Psychology Guilford Turiel Elliot 2002 The Culture of Morality Cambridge University Press Cambridge Cole Michael 1996 Cultural Psychology A Once and Future Discipline The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press Cambridge Matsumoto D Ed 2001 The Handbook of Culture amp Psychology Oxford University Press New York Shweder R A amp Levine R A Eds 1984 Culture theory Essays on mind self and emotion New York Cambridge University Press Triandis H C 1989 The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts PDF Psychological Review 96 3 506 20 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 96 3 506 S2CID 18670762 Archived from the original PDF on 2013 05 13 Bruner Jerome 1990 Acts of Meaning Harvard University Press ISBN 0 674 00360 8 Markus H R Kitayama S 1991 Culture and the self Implications for cognition emotion and motivation PDF Psychological Review 98 2 224 53 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 320 1159 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 98 2 224 Shore B 1996 Culture in mind Cognition culture and the problem of meaning New York Oxford University Press Nisbett R E Peng K Choi I Norenzayan A 2001 Culture and systems of thought Holistic vs analytic cognition PDF Psychological Review 108 2 291 310 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 108 2 291 PMID 11381831 S2CID 17739645 Nisbett R E 2003 The Geography of Thought New York Free Press Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Cultural psychology amp oldid 1216440855, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.