fbpx
Wikipedia

Binocular rivalry

Binocular rivalry is a phenomenon of visual perception in which perception alternates between different images presented to each eye.[1]

An image demonstrating binocular rivalry. If you view the image with red-cyan 3D glasses, the text will alternate between Red and Blue. 3D red cyan glasses are recommended to view this image correctly.
Binocular rivalry. If you view the image with red-cyan 3D glasses, the angled Warp and weft will alternate between the Red and the Blue lines. 3D red cyan glasses are recommended to view this image correctly.

When one image is presented to one eye and a very different image is presented to the other (also known as dichoptic presentation), instead of the two images being seen superimposed, one image is seen for a few moments,[2] then the other, then the first, and so on, randomly for as long as one cares to look. For example, if a set of vertical lines is presented to one eye, and a set of horizontal lines to the same region of the retina of the other, sometimes the vertical lines are seen with no trace of the horizontal lines, and sometimes the horizontal lines are seen with no trace of the vertical lines.

At transitions, brief, unstable composites of the two images may be seen. For example, the vertical lines may appear one at a time to obscure the horizontal lines from the left or from the right, like a traveling wave, switching slowly one image for the other.[3] Binocular rivalry occurs between any stimuli that differ sufficiently,[4] including simple stimuli like lines of different orientation and complex stimuli like different alphabetic letters or different pictures such as of a face and of a house.

Very small differences between images, however, might yield singleness of vision and stereopsis. Binocular rivalry has been extensively studied in the last century.[5][page needed] In recent years[when?] neuroscientists have used neuroimaging techniques and single-cell recording techniques to identify neural events responsible for the perceptual dominance of a given image and for the perceptual alternations.

Types edit

When the images presented to the eyes differ only in their contours, rivalry is referred to as binocular contour rivalry. When the images presented to the eyes differ only in their colours, rivalry is referred to as binocular colour rivalry. When the images presented to the eyes differ only in their lightnesses, a form of rivalry called binocular lustre may be seen. When an image is presented to one eye and a blank field to the other, the image is usually seen continuously. This is referred to as contour dominance. Occasionally however, the blank field, or even the dark field of a closed eye, can become visible, making the image invisible for about as long as it would be invisible were it in rivalry with another image of equal stimulus strength. When an image is presented to one eye and a blank field to the other, introducing a different image onto the blank field usually results in that image being seen immediately. This is referred to as flash suppression.

History edit

Binocular rivalry was discovered by Porta.[6] Porta put one book in front of one eye, and another in front of the other. He reported that he could read from one book at a time and that changing from one to the other required withdrawing the "visual virtue" from one eye and moving it to the other. According to Wade (1998), binocular colour rivalry was first reported by Le Clerc (1712). Desaguiliers (1716) also recorded it when looking at different colours from spectra in the bevel of a mirror. The clearest early description of both colour and contour rivalry was made by Dutour (1760, 1763). To experience colour rivalry Dutour either crossed his eyes or overdiverged his eyes (a form of free fusion commonly used also at the end of the 20th century to view Magic Eye stereograms) to look at differently coloured pieces of cloth (Dutour 1760) or differently coloured pieces of glass (Dutour 1763). To experience contour rivalry Dutour again used free fusion of different objects or used a prism or a mirror in front of one eye to project different images into it. The first clear description of rivalry in English was by Charles Wheatstone (1838). Wheatstone invented the stereoscope, an optical device (in Wheatstone's case using mirrors) to present different images to the two eyes.

Early theories edit

Various theories were proposed to account for binocular rivalry. Porta and Dutour took it as evidence for an ancient theory of visual perception that has come to be known as suppression theory. Its essential idea is that, despite having two eyes, we see only one of everything (known as singleness of vision) because we see with one eye at a time. According to this theory, we do not normally notice the alternations between the two eyes because their images are too similar. By making the images very different, Porta and Dutour argued, this natural alternation can be seen. Wheatstone, on the other hand, supported the alternative theory of singleness of vision, fusion theory, proposed by Aristotle. Its essential idea is that we see only one of everything because the information from the two eyes is combined or fused. Wheatstone also discovered binocular stereopsis, the perception of depth arising from the lateral placement of the eyes. Wheatstone was able to prove that stereopsis depended on the different horizontal positions (the horizontal disparity) of points in the images viewed by each eye by creating the illusion of depth from flat depictions of such images displayed in his stereoscope. Such stereopsis is impossible unless information is being combined from each eye. Although Wheatstone's discovery of stereopsis supported fusion theory, he still had to account for binocular rivalry. He regarded binocular rivalry as a special case in which fusion is impossible, saying "the mind is inattentive to impressions made on one retina when it cannot combine the impressions on the two retinae together so as to occasion a perception resembling that of some external object" (p. 264).[full citation needed]

Other theories of binocular rivalry dealt more with how it occurs than why it occurs. Dutour speculated that the alternations could be controlled by attention, a theory promoted in the nineteenth century by Hermann von Helmholtz.[full citation needed] But Dutour also speculated that the alternations could be controlled by structural properties of the images (such as by temporary fluctuations in the blur of one image, or temporary fluctuations in the luminance of one image). This theory was promoted in the nineteenth century by Helmholtz's traditional rival, Ewald Hering.[full citation needed]

Empirical studies: B. B. Breese (1899, 1909) edit

The most comprehensive early study of binocular rivalry was conducted by B. B. Breese (1899, 1909). Breese quantified the amount of rivalry by requiring his observers to press keys while observing rivalry for 100-second trials. An observer pressed one key whenever and for as long as he or she saw one rival stimulus with no trace of the other, and another key whenever and for as long as he or she saw the other rival stimulus with no trace of the first. This has come to be known as recording periods of exclusive visibility. From the key-press records (Breese's were made on a kymograph drum), Breese was able to quantify rivalry in three ways: the number of periods of exclusive visibility of each stimulus (the rate of rivalry), the total duration of exclusive visibility of each stimulus, and the average duration of each period of rivalry.

Breese first found that although observers could increase the time one rival stimulus was seen by attending to it, they could not increase the rate of that stimulus. Moreover, when he asked his observers to refrain from moving their eyes over the attended stimulus, control was abolished. When he asked observers specifically to move their eyes over one stimulus, that stimulus predominated in rivalry. He could also increase predominance of a stimulus by increasing the number of its contours, by moving it, by reducing its size, by making it brighter, and by contracting the muscles on the same side of the body as the eye viewing that stimulus. Breese also showed that rivalry occurs between afterimages. Breese also discovered the phenomenon of monocular rivalry: if the two rival stimuli are optically superimposed to the same eye and one fixates on the stimuli, then alternations in the clarity of the two stimuli are seen. Occasionally, one image disappears altogether, as in binocular rivalry, although this is much rarer than in binocular rivalry.

Other senses edit

Auditory and olfactory forms of perceptual rivalry can occur when there are conflicting and so rivaling inputs into the two ears[7] or two nostrils.[8]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Blake, Randolph; Logothetis, Nikos K. (1 January 2002). "Visual competition". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3 (1): 13–21. doi:10.1038/nrn701. PMID 11823801. S2CID 8410171.
  2. ^ Wolfe, Jeremy M (1983). "Influence of spatial frequency, luminance, and duration on binocular rivalry and abnormal fusion of briefly presented dichoptic stimuli". Perception. 12 (4): 447–456. doi:10.1068/p120447. PMID 6672740. S2CID 26294790.
  3. ^ Wilson, Hugh R.; Blake, Randolph; Lee, Sang-Hun (30 August 2001). "Dynamics of travelling waves in visual perception". Nature. 412 (6850): 907–910. Bibcode:2001Natur.412..907W. doi:10.1038/35091066. PMID 11528478. S2CID 4431136.
  4. ^ Blake, Randolph (1989). "A neural theory of binocular rivalry". Psychological Review. 96 (1): 145–167. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.96.1.145. PMID 2648445.
  5. ^ Alais, David; Blake, Randolph, eds. (2005). Binocular rivalry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 9780262012126. OCLC 990669529.
  6. ^ Porta 1593, as cited in Wade 1996
  7. ^ Deutsch D. (September 1974). "An auditory illusion". Nature. 251 (5473): 307–9. Bibcode:1974Natur.251..307D. doi:10.1038/251307a0. PMID 4427654. S2CID 4273134.
  8. ^ Zhou W.; Chen D. (September 2009). "Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex". Curr. Biol. 19 (18): 1561–5. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052. PMC 2901510. PMID 19699095.
  • Breese, B.B. (1909). "Binocular rivalry". Psychological Review. 16 (6): 410–5. doi:10.1037/h0075805.
  • Breese, B.B. (1899). "On inhibition". Psychological Monographs. 3: 1–65. doi:10.1037/h0092990. S2CID 249336219.
  • Desaguiliers, J.T. (1716). "III. A plain and easy Experiment to confirm Sir Isaac Newton's Doctrine of the different Refrangibility of the Rays of Light". Philosophical Transactions. 348: 448–452.
  • Dutour, É.F. (1760). "Discussion d'une question d'optique". Mémoires de Mathématique et de Physique Présentés par Divers Savants. 3. l’Académie des Sciences: 514–530. O’Shea, R.P. (1999) Translation 2015-06-05 at the Wayback Machine
  • Dutour, É.F. (1763). "Addition au Mémoire intitulé, Discussion d'une question d'Optique, imprimé dans le troisième Volume des Mémoires des Savan[t]s Étrangers, pages 514 & suivantes" [Addition to the Memoir entitled, Discussion on a question of Optics printed in the third Volume of Memoirs of Foreign Scientists, pages 514 and following]. Mémoires de Mathématique et de Physique Préséntes par Divers Savants. 4. Académie des Sciences: 499–511. O’Shea, R.P. (1999) Translation 2015-06-05 at the Wayback Machine
  • Le Clerc, S. (1712). Système de la vision. Paris: Delaulne.
  • Porta, J. B. (1593). De refractione. Optices parte. Libri novem. Naples: Salviani.
  • Wade, N.J. (1996). "Descriptions of visual phenomena from Aristotle to Wheatstone". Perception. 25 (10): 1137–75. doi:10.1068/p251137. PMID 9027920. S2CID 21480863.
  • Wade, N.J. (1998). "Early studies of eye dominances". Laterality. 3 (2): 97–108. doi:10.1080/713754296. PMID 15513077.
  • Wheatstone, Charles (1838). "Contributions to the physiology of vision.—Part the First. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phænomena of binocular vision". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. 128: 371–394. doi:10.1098/rstl.1838.0019. S2CID 36512205.

Further reading edit

  • Wikibooks: Consciousness Studies
  • Alais, D.; Blake, R. (2005). Binocular Rivalry. MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-01212-X.
  • Carter O.L.; Pettigrew J.D.; Hasler F.; et al. (June 2005). "Modulating the rate and rhythmicity of perceptual rivalry alternations with the mixed 5-HT2A and 5-HT1A agonist psilocybin". Neuropsychopharmacology. 30 (6): 1154–62. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300621. PMID 15688092. — Effects of psilocybin on binocular rivalry.
  • Blake, R. (2001). "A primer on binocular rivalry, including current controversies". Brain and Mind. 2: 5–38. doi:10.1023/A:1017925416289. S2CID 2336275.
  • Blake R., Logothetis N.K. (January 2002). "Visual competition". Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 (1): 13–21. doi:10.1038/nrn701. PMID 11823801. S2CID 8410171.

External links edit

  • Blake, Randolph; Tong, Frank (2008). "Binocular rivalry". Scholarpedia. 3 (12): 1578. Bibcode:2008SchpJ...3.1578B. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1578.
  • Blake, Randolph. "Binocular Rivalry Demonstrations". Binocular Rivalry.
  • O'Shea, Robert P. . Robert P O'Shea. Archived from the original on 2020-10-11. Retrieved 2013-04-08.

binocular, rivalry, phenomenon, visual, perception, which, perception, alternates, between, different, images, presented, each, image, demonstrating, binocular, rivalry, view, image, with, cyan, glasses, text, will, alternate, between, blue, cyan, glasses, rec. Binocular rivalry is a phenomenon of visual perception in which perception alternates between different images presented to each eye 1 An image demonstrating binocular rivalry If you view the image with red cyan 3D glasses the text will alternate between Red and Blue 3D red cyan glasses are recommended to view this image correctly Binocular rivalry If you view the image with red cyan 3D glasses the angled Warp and weft will alternate between the Red and the Blue lines 3D red cyan glasses are recommended to view this image correctly When one image is presented to one eye and a very different image is presented to the other also known as dichoptic presentation instead of the two images being seen superimposed one image is seen for a few moments 2 then the other then the first and so on randomly for as long as one cares to look For example if a set of vertical lines is presented to one eye and a set of horizontal lines to the same region of the retina of the other sometimes the vertical lines are seen with no trace of the horizontal lines and sometimes the horizontal lines are seen with no trace of the vertical lines At transitions brief unstable composites of the two images may be seen For example the vertical lines may appear one at a time to obscure the horizontal lines from the left or from the right like a traveling wave switching slowly one image for the other 3 Binocular rivalry occurs between any stimuli that differ sufficiently 4 including simple stimuli like lines of different orientation and complex stimuli like different alphabetic letters or different pictures such as of a face and of a house Very small differences between images however might yield singleness of vision and stereopsis Binocular rivalry has been extensively studied in the last century 5 page needed In recent years when neuroscientists have used neuroimaging techniques and single cell recording techniques to identify neural events responsible for the perceptual dominance of a given image and for the perceptual alternations Contents 1 Types 2 History 3 Early theories 4 Empirical studies B B Breese 1899 1909 5 Other senses 6 See also 7 References 8 Further reading 9 External linksTypes editWhen the images presented to the eyes differ only in their contours rivalry is referred to as binocular contour rivalry When the images presented to the eyes differ only in their colours rivalry is referred to as binocular colour rivalry When the images presented to the eyes differ only in their lightnesses a form of rivalry called binocular lustre may be seen When an image is presented to one eye and a blank field to the other the image is usually seen continuously This is referred to as contour dominance Occasionally however the blank field or even the dark field of a closed eye can become visible making the image invisible for about as long as it would be invisible were it in rivalry with another image of equal stimulus strength When an image is presented to one eye and a blank field to the other introducing a different image onto the blank field usually results in that image being seen immediately This is referred to as flash suppression History editBinocular rivalry was discovered by Porta 6 Porta put one book in front of one eye and another in front of the other He reported that he could read from one book at a time and that changing from one to the other required withdrawing the visual virtue from one eye and moving it to the other According to Wade 1998 binocular colour rivalry was first reported by Le Clerc 1712 Desaguiliers 1716 also recorded it when looking at different colours from spectra in the bevel of a mirror The clearest early description of both colour and contour rivalry was made by Dutour 1760 1763 To experience colour rivalry Dutour either crossed his eyes or overdiverged his eyes a form of free fusion commonly used also at the end of the 20th century to view Magic Eye stereograms to look at differently coloured pieces of cloth Dutour 1760 or differently coloured pieces of glass Dutour 1763 To experience contour rivalry Dutour again used free fusion of different objects or used a prism or a mirror in front of one eye to project different images into it The first clear description of rivalry in English was by Charles Wheatstone 1838 Wheatstone invented the stereoscope an optical device in Wheatstone s case using mirrors to present different images to the two eyes Early theories editVarious theories were proposed to account for binocular rivalry Porta and Dutour took it as evidence for an ancient theory of visual perception that has come to be known as suppression theory Its essential idea is that despite having two eyes we see only one of everything known as singleness of vision because we see with one eye at a time According to this theory we do not normally notice the alternations between the two eyes because their images are too similar By making the images very different Porta and Dutour argued this natural alternation can be seen Wheatstone on the other hand supported the alternative theory of singleness of vision fusion theory proposed by Aristotle Its essential idea is that we see only one of everything because the information from the two eyes is combined or fused Wheatstone also discovered binocular stereopsis the perception of depth arising from the lateral placement of the eyes Wheatstone was able to prove that stereopsis depended on the different horizontal positions the horizontal disparity of points in the images viewed by each eye by creating the illusion of depth from flat depictions of such images displayed in his stereoscope Such stereopsis is impossible unless information is being combined from each eye Although Wheatstone s discovery of stereopsis supported fusion theory he still had to account for binocular rivalry He regarded binocular rivalry as a special case in which fusion is impossible saying the mind is inattentive to impressions made on one retina when it cannot combine the impressions on the two retinae together so as to occasion a perception resembling that of some external object p 264 full citation needed Other theories of binocular rivalry dealt more with how it occurs than why it occurs Dutour speculated that the alternations could be controlled by attention a theory promoted in the nineteenth century by Hermann von Helmholtz full citation needed But Dutour also speculated that the alternations could be controlled by structural properties of the images such as by temporary fluctuations in the blur of one image or temporary fluctuations in the luminance of one image This theory was promoted in the nineteenth century by Helmholtz s traditional rival Ewald Hering full citation needed Empirical studies B B Breese 1899 1909 editThe most comprehensive early study of binocular rivalry was conducted by B B Breese 1899 1909 Breese quantified the amount of rivalry by requiring his observers to press keys while observing rivalry for 100 second trials An observer pressed one key whenever and for as long as he or she saw one rival stimulus with no trace of the other and another key whenever and for as long as he or she saw the other rival stimulus with no trace of the first This has come to be known as recording periods of exclusive visibility From the key press records Breese s were made on a kymograph drum Breese was able to quantify rivalry in three ways the number of periods of exclusive visibility of each stimulus the rate of rivalry the total duration of exclusive visibility of each stimulus and the average duration of each period of rivalry Breese first found that although observers could increase the time one rival stimulus was seen by attending to it they could not increase the rate of that stimulus Moreover when he asked his observers to refrain from moving their eyes over the attended stimulus control was abolished When he asked observers specifically to move their eyes over one stimulus that stimulus predominated in rivalry He could also increase predominance of a stimulus by increasing the number of its contours by moving it by reducing its size by making it brighter and by contracting the muscles on the same side of the body as the eye viewing that stimulus Breese also showed that rivalry occurs between afterimages Breese also discovered the phenomenon of monocular rivalry if the two rival stimuli are optically superimposed to the same eye and one fixates on the stimuli then alternations in the clarity of the two stimuli are seen Occasionally one image disappears altogether as in binocular rivalry although this is much rarer than in binocular rivalry Other senses editAuditory and olfactory forms of perceptual rivalry can occur when there are conflicting and so rivaling inputs into the two ears 7 or two nostrils 8 See also editBinocular summation Binocular vision DiplopiaReferences edit Blake Randolph Logothetis Nikos K 1 January 2002 Visual competition Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3 1 13 21 doi 10 1038 nrn701 PMID 11823801 S2CID 8410171 Wolfe Jeremy M 1983 Influence of spatial frequency luminance and duration on binocular rivalry and abnormal fusion of briefly presented dichoptic stimuli Perception 12 4 447 456 doi 10 1068 p120447 PMID 6672740 S2CID 26294790 Wilson Hugh R Blake Randolph Lee Sang Hun 30 August 2001 Dynamics of travelling waves in visual perception Nature 412 6850 907 910 Bibcode 2001Natur 412 907W doi 10 1038 35091066 PMID 11528478 S2CID 4431136 Blake Randolph 1989 A neural theory of binocular rivalry Psychological Review 96 1 145 167 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 96 1 145 PMID 2648445 Alais David Blake Randolph eds 2005 Binocular rivalry Cambridge MA MIT Press ISBN 9780262012126 OCLC 990669529 Porta 1593 as cited in Wade 1996 Deutsch D September 1974 An auditory illusion Nature 251 5473 307 9 Bibcode 1974Natur 251 307D doi 10 1038 251307a0 PMID 4427654 S2CID 4273134 Zhou W Chen D September 2009 Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex Curr Biol 19 18 1561 5 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 07 052 PMC 2901510 PMID 19699095 Breese B B 1909 Binocular rivalry Psychological Review 16 6 410 5 doi 10 1037 h0075805 Breese B B 1899 On inhibition Psychological Monographs 3 1 65 doi 10 1037 h0092990 S2CID 249336219 Desaguiliers J T 1716 III A plain and easy Experiment to confirm Sir Isaac Newton s Doctrine of the different Refrangibility of the Rays of Light Philosophical Transactions 348 448 452 Dutour E F 1760 Discussion d une question d optique Memoires de Mathematique et de Physique Presentes par Divers Savants 3 l Academie des Sciences 514 530 O Shea R P 1999 Translation Archived 2015 06 05 at the Wayback Machine Dutour E F 1763 Addition au Memoire intitule Discussion d une question d Optique imprime dans le troisieme Volume des Memoires des Savan t s Etrangers pages 514 amp suivantes Addition to the Memoir entitled Discussion on a question of Optics printed in the third Volume of Memoirs of Foreign Scientists pages 514 and following Memoires de Mathematique et de Physique Presentes par Divers Savants 4 Academie des Sciences 499 511 O Shea R P 1999 Translation Archived 2015 06 05 at the Wayback Machine Le Clerc S 1712 Systeme de la vision Paris Delaulne Porta J B 1593 De refractione Optices parte Libri novem Naples Salviani Wade N J 1996 Descriptions of visual phenomena from Aristotle to Wheatstone Perception 25 10 1137 75 doi 10 1068 p251137 PMID 9027920 S2CID 21480863 Wade N J 1998 Early studies of eye dominances Laterality 3 2 97 108 doi 10 1080 713754296 PMID 15513077 Wheatstone Charles 1838 Contributions to the physiology of vision Part the First On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phaenomena of binocular vision Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 128 371 394 doi 10 1098 rstl 1838 0019 S2CID 36512205 Further reading editWikibooks Consciousness Studies Alais D Blake R 2005 Binocular Rivalry MIT Press ISBN 0 262 01212 X Carter O L Pettigrew J D Hasler F et al June 2005 Modulating the rate and rhythmicity of perceptual rivalry alternations with the mixed 5 HT2A and 5 HT1A agonist psilocybin Neuropsychopharmacology 30 6 1154 62 doi 10 1038 sj npp 1300621 PMID 15688092 Effects of psilocybin on binocular rivalry Blake R 2001 A primer on binocular rivalry including current controversies Brain and Mind 2 5 38 doi 10 1023 A 1017925416289 S2CID 2336275 Blake R Logothetis N K January 2002 Visual competition Nat Rev Neurosci 3 1 13 21 doi 10 1038 nrn701 PMID 11823801 S2CID 8410171 External links editBlake Randolph Tong Frank 2008 Binocular rivalry Scholarpedia 3 12 1578 Bibcode 2008SchpJ 3 1578B doi 10 4249 scholarpedia 1578 Blake Randolph Binocular Rivalry Demonstrations Binocular Rivalry O Shea Robert P Binocular rivalry bibliography Robert P O Shea Archived from the original on 2020 10 11 Retrieved 2013 04 08 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Binocular rivalry amp oldid 1219014610, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.