fbpx
Wikipedia

Authority bias

Authority bias is the tendency to attribute greater accuracy to the opinion of an authority figure (unrelated to its content) and be more influenced by that opinion.[1] An individual is more influenced by the opinion of this authority figure, believing their views to be more credible, and hence place greater emphasis on the authority figure's viewpoint and are more likely to obey them. This concept is considered one of the social cognitive biases or collective cognitive biases.[2]

Humans generally have a deep-seated duty to authority and tend to comply when requested by an authority figure.[3] Some scholars explain that individuals are motivated to view authority as deserving of their position and this legitimacy leads people to accept and obey the decisions that it makes.[2] System justification theory articulates this phenomenon, particularly within its position that there is a psychological motivation for believing in the steadiness, stability and justness of the current social system.[4]

Authority bias can be measured concerning respect for authority, where higher respect for authority positively correlates with the increased likelihood of exhibiting authority bias.[5] Respect for authority is measured using the Respect for Authority Index (RAI), which averages responses on deference to the police. A higher score on the RAI is indicative of higher respect for authority, and hence strengthening the execution of authority bias.

Cultural differences in the strength of authority bias have been identified, in which the differences in edits made to Wikipedia articles by administrators and regular users were compared for accuracy.[6][self-published source] In Western Europe, the bias has a negligible effect. In Eastern Europe, the bias is larger and the administrator's edits are perceived as more likely to be true (despite the edits being inaccurate), indicating a cultural difference in the extent to which authority bias is experienced.

Definition

Authority bias, a term popularised by American psychologist Stanley Milgram, is defined as having an incorrectly high belief that the information verified by a person with formal authority is correct, and therefore an individual is likely to be more influenced by them.[6][self-published source] Individuals in positions of authority are seen to be treated more favourably, where people believe their views with increased certainty, though the role of authority alone is not always significant enough to directly affect decision-making without this phenomenon being used in conjunction with other heuristics and biases.

The antonym of authority bias is blanket opposition to authority, disregarding their knowledge and believing authority figures to have inherently false claims. This relates to the view of anti-authoritarianism.

Authority bias has many explanations, rooted in the human need to obey authority figures. Namely, authority bias can be explained through evolutionary and social means.

 
The majority of people accept the police as a legitimate authority, with their position in the social hierarchy giving them the right to apprehend those violating the law, such as this Extinction Rebellion protester.

Legitimacy of authority

Authority bias is greater when the authority figure in question is seen to be legitimate,[7] that is they are accepted to be in an authorized position of relative power, and have the right to demand obedience. Research has highlighted certain important characteristics that can mark one as a legitimate authority figure, strengthening authority bias, with individuals more likely to be influenced by and obey such figures. These markers can affect whether people regard an individual as an authority figure.

Relative position in the social hierarchy

All societies are structured in a hierarchical manner,[8] and often those who possess a higher social status are situated further up the hierarchy. The majority of people accept that in order to maintain a co-operative, functioning society and avoid social chaos, a certain degree of individual choice must be relinquished to authority figures, often representing government institutions, whose role means they have greater status in the social hierarchy. For example, most accept that in the criminal justice system, the police have the right to exercise power over others and apprehend wrongdoers, as their role indicates their position in the social hierarchy[9] and their authority. This is often instilled in children from a young age, with parents instructing them to defer to authority figures such as teachers and policemen and accept the opinions of authority figures as correct. There is also a tendency to attribute greater accuracy to and respect the opinions of experts such as doctors. For example, most people have complete confidence in their doctor treating them, as a trusted authority figure with a high social status.[10] This normative behavior can be a useful shortcut, but blind acceptance of expert advice can raise issues if it becomes an automatic response. This was demonstrated by a study conducted by the psychiatrist Charles Hofling, who found that 21 out of 22 nurses would have given patients a potentially fatal dose of a drug when asked to by a doctor despite this instruction contravening official guidance forbidding the administration of the drug. Similar studies in other areas of society have demonstrated the problems that can arise as a result of unquestioningly accepting the opinions of those regarded as having higher status in the social hierarchy.[11]

 
Uniform, such as that of a security guard, can be an important marker of a legitimate authority figure, and cause or enhance authority bias.

Uniform

Outward appearance can signify an individual's social status and relative position in the social hierarchy, and consequently whether they are perceived as a legitimate authority figure. When examining authority bias, outward appearance, manifested in clothing, in particular uniform, seems to have a profound effect on whether an individual is respected and obeyed as an authority figure. Research conducted by Bickman and colleagues found that passers-by were twice as likely to obey a confederate dressed as a security guard rather than a milkman when asked by the individual to complete tasks such as picking up litter or lending the confederate a coin for the parking meter.[12] The effects of uniform on authority bias has been a fairly consistent and reliable finding as other studies, such as those run by Milgram, have reached similar conclusions. In a variation of his original experiment, Milgram found that obedience levels dropped from 65% to 20% when the instructions to enable shocks were given by an ordinary member of the public wearing plain clothes rather than a researcher (signified by a white lab coat).[13] In the real world, attention bias may also play a role here, with people selectively paying more attention to the uniform as an important symbol of power, attributing greater accuracy to the opinions of the wearer and displaying more obedience to that figure in response. However, it is important to recognize that factors like uniform are situational and easily changed. Thus, an individual may masquerade as an authority figure and can elicit the authority bias response from others, even though their authority may not be truly legitimate. Though situational factors may point to an authority figure, the effects of authority bias may be stronger when such situational factors are combined with innate factors, such as gender.

Gender

Evidence also suggests that gender can be important in signifying an authority figure. A subset of authority bias, namely gender-authority bias explains how in particular, women may be more susceptible to authority bias than males. In addition to women being more influenced by authority figures than males, female authority figures may be less influential than their male counterparts. Research has shown that both men and women associated men with high authority roles and women with low authority roles, showing how gender can signify an authority figure and subsequently influence authority bias.[14] As a result, confirmation bias may also play a role here in what people interpret to be a characteristic of an authority figure; as many associate higher-authority roles with males, confirmation bias suggests that people will tend to look for, favor, interpret a recall information that fits with this belief, perpetuating this bias, reinforcing the idea of males being more legitimate as authority figures. Gender-authority bias has been demonstrated across a variety of real-world sectors. In politics, research into leadership using the Implicit Association Test (IAT) found that female political leaders tend to face greater resistance to their authority in comparison to their male counterparts.[15] Authority bias is therefore strengthened when an authority figure is male instead of female in politics. In finance, males are often favored as authority figures, being perceived to have greater control of resources, and able to make better decisions, demonstrated by the fact that they hold higher positions and on average earn 66% more than females in the finance industry.[16] Research also suggests that women are more susceptible to authority bias, as they are more influenced than men by hearing fake news from an authority figure.[17]

Discovery

Prior to psychological research, the most common example of this phenomenon was when people obeyed Hitler during World War II, though such effects have been evident throughout history. This relates to pluralistic ignorance, in which authority figures are obeyed regardless of immorality.

The term “authority bias” was first mentioned in literature in reference to state authority bias, in which it simply indicated a preference for being pro-state or anti-state in the US federal election.[18]

Nevertheless, the first-time authority bias was referenced in literature as a cognitive bias was a result of Milgram's experiment, in which it was used to explain obedience to authority figures.[19] Whilst Milgram did not directly use the term "authority bias" in his 1963 paper, the obedience effect identified from his study became the primary example of authority bias.  Milgram’s findings succeeded the reason why people during World War II obeyed Hitler; participants voluntarily submitted to the authority figure (the experimenter wearing a white lab coat, signifying professionalism). Authority bias is further strengthened through the use of uniforms to signify authority, initially investigated in Milgram’s situational variable (where obedience decreased when the uniform of the experimenter was changed from a lab coat to everyday clothes), but further replicated through Bickman's infamous research into obedience,[20] where security guards are more likely to be obeyed without question and thus contributing directly to authority bias.  

Real-world effects

Advertising

Authority bias is used as a marketing strategy in order to increase the legitimacy of claims made about a product. A common example in advertising is where toothpaste companies such as Sensodyne promote the validity of their claims by ensuring the dentists wear lab coats, resulting in the consumer being more trustworthy of the product and consequently more likely to buy the product.

Personalised advertising in relation to political voting attitudes (particularly in the US election) relies on authority bias.[21] Political campaigns are targeted specifically towards female voters (who are more susceptible to the bias), amplified through the use of social media, in which political leaders and other figures of authority are used in ad-campaigns to increase the effectiveness of their claims.

Medicine

The expert halo effect is synonymous with authority bias in medicine, where the expert is seen as infallible.[22] Issues arise in pharmaceutical settings, in which non-experts blindly follow expert's commands, resulting in the distribution of harmful drugs[23] and inappropriate healthcare practices.[24]

A further issue concerning the extent to which an authority figure is perceived to be providing accurate information is apparent in cases such as that of Willie Jackson. Forensic dentistry falsely proved Jackson to be guilty, yet the authority bias strengthened the doctor's standpoint in a court of law as they had expert authority bias. Consequently, the negative effect of authority bias has led to wrongful convictions.

Business

The authority bias is demonstrated in the case of the highest-paid persons' opinion (HIPPO) impact, which describes how employees and other stakeholders in the solution environment tend to go with the opinions and impressions of the highly paid people in an organization.[25]

Explanations

Evolution

Evolution has established a dominance hierarchy in which it is an evolutionary advantage to obey authority figures, as figures of authority have a greater allocation of resources and other means of survival. The logical fallacy of ad verecundiam is evolutionary, highlighting that experts are more reliable due to a lack of opposing information, increasing trustworthiness.[26]

Societal benefits

In any society, a diverse and widely accepted system of authority allows the development of sophisticated structures for the production of resources, trade, expansion and social control. Notions of obedience and loyalty to the legitimate rule of others are generally accorded values in schools, the law, the military and in political systems. The strength of the bias to obey a legitimate authority figure comes from systemic socialization practices designed to instill the perception that such obedience constitutes correct behavior, and that genuine authority figures usually possess higher degrees of knowledge, wisdom and power. Different societies vary the terms of this dimension.[6][self-published source] As a result, authority bias can be rooted in the underlying social norms of society. Consequently, deference to authority can occur mindlessly as a kind of decision-making short cut.[27]

The role of other heuristics and biases

Research support for the strength of authority bias is evident, however, the effect is not significant in some instances.[28] Research is merely correlational, and hence other behavioural effects experienced in conjunction with authority bias strengthen its effects.

Confirmation bias

An individual exhibiting authority bias may also be subject to experiencing confirmation bias, which is the tendency to search for information that confirms your own existing beliefs. Research suggests that greater authority is given to financial advisors who confirm one’s existing opinions, implying that authority bias is strengthened when it coincides with confirmation bias.[29]

Bandwagon effect

The bandwagon effect is where people adopt the ideologies of those surrounding them. Society favours the opinions of authority figures, hence it is a majority view which others support.[30]

See also

References

  1. ^ Milgram, Stanley (1963). "Behavioral study of obedience". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 67 (4): 371–378. doi:10.1037/h0040525. PMID 14049516.
  2. ^ a b Juárez Ramos, Veronica (2019). Analyzing the Role of Cognitive Biases in the Decision-Making Process. Hershey, PA: IGI Global. p. 113. ISBN 9781522529798.
  3. ^ Milgram, Stanley (1974). Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View.
  4. ^ Browstein, Michael (2016). Implicit Bias and Philosophy, Volume 1: Metaphysics and Epistemology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 240. ISBN 9780198766186.
  5. ^ "Respect for Authority". Cato Institute. Retrieved 2021-02-16.
  6. ^ a b c Hinnosaar, Marit; Hinnosaar, Toomas (2012). "Authority Bias". marit.hinnosaar.net.
  7. ^ van der Toorn, Jojanneke; Tyler, Tom R.; Jost, John T. (2011-01-01). "More than fair: Outcome dependence, system justification, and the perceived legitimacy of authority figures". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 47 (1): 127–138. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.09.003. ISSN 0022-1031.
  8. ^ Cloutier, Jasmin; Cardenas-Iniguez, Carlos; Gyurovski, Ivo; Barakzai, Anam; Li, Tianyi (2016-01-01), Absher, John R.; Cloutier, Jasmin (eds.), "Chapter 9 - Neuroimaging Investigations of Social Status and Social Hierarchies", Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character, San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 187–203, doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-800935-2.00009-9, ISBN 978-0-12-800935-2, retrieved 2022-02-09
  9. ^ Jackson, Jonathan; Bradford, Ben; Hough, Mike; Myhill, Andy; Quinton, Paul King; Tyler, Tom (2012). "Why Do People Comply with the Law? Legitimacy and the Influence of Legal Institutions". SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.1994490. ISSN 1556-5068.
  10. ^ "Confidence and trust in clinicians". The Nuffield Trust. 2018-10-15. Retrieved 2022-02-10.
  11. ^ Tarnow, Eugen (2000-01-01). "Towards the Zero Accident Goal: Assisting the First Officer: Monitor and Challenge Captain Errors". Journal of Aviation/Aerospace Education & Research. 10 (1). doi:10.15394/jaaer.2000.1269. ISSN 2329-258X.
  12. ^ Bickman, Leonard (1974). "The Social Power of a Uniform1". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 4 (1): 47–61. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1974.tb02599.x. ISSN 1559-1816.
  13. ^ "Milgram Experiment | Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 2022-02-14.
  14. ^ Rudman, Laurie A.; Kilianski, Stephen E. (2000). "Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Toward Female Authority". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 26 (11): 1315–1328. doi:10.1177/0146167200263001. ISSN 0146-1672.
  15. ^ Yang, Ke; Zhu, John Jianjun; Santoro, Michael D. (2017-08-01). "Inter-Firm Managerial Social Ties and Strategic Alliances Formation: A Multiplexity Perspective". Academy of Management Proceedings. 2017 (1): 10151. doi:10.5465/AMBPP.2017.27. ISSN 0065-0668.
  16. ^ "Women finance firm directors earn 66% less than men in UK, study finds". Reuters. 2021-03-01. Retrieved 2022-02-09.
  17. ^ Jost, Peter J.; Pünder, Johanna; Schulze-Lohoff, Isabell (2020-04-01). "Fake news - Does perception matter more than the truth?". Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics. 85: 101513. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2020.101513. ISSN 2214-8043.
  18. ^ Sprague, John D.; Incorporated, Macmillan Publishing Company (1968). Voting Patterns of the United States Supreme Court: Cases in Federalism, 1889-1959. Ardent Media.
  19. ^ Ellis RM (2015). Middle Way Philosophy: Omnibus Edition. Lulu Press. ISBN 9781326351892.
  20. ^ Bickman, Leonard (1974). "The Social Power of a Uniform1". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 4 (1): 47–61. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1974.tb02599.x. ISSN 1559-1816.
  21. ^ Kruikemeier, Sanne; Sezgin, Minem; Boerman, Sophie C. (June 2016). "Political Microtargeting: Relationship Between Personalized Advertising on Facebook and Voters' Responses". Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 19 (6): 367–372. doi:10.1089/cyber.2015.0652. ISSN 2152-2715. PMID 27327063.
  22. ^ Austin, Jared P.; Foster, Byron A. (2019-07-01). "How Pediatric Hospitalists Must Contend With the Expert Halo Effect". Hospital Pediatrics. 9 (7): 560–562. doi:10.1542/hpeds.2019-0053. ISSN 2154-1663. PMID 31175143.
  23. ^ Austin, Jared P.; Halvorson, Stephanie A. C. (2019-02-05). "Reducing the Expert Halo Effect on Pharmacy and Therapeutics Committees". JAMA. 321 (5): 453–454. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.20789. ISSN 1538-3598. PMID 30657521.
  24. ^ Seshia, Shashi; Makhinson, Michael; Young, G Bryan (2015). "'Cognitive biases plus': covert subverters of healthcare evidence". Evidence-Based Medicine. 21 (2): 41–5. doi:10.1136/ebmed-2015-110302. PMID 26612071. S2CID 353523.
  25. ^ Garrett, John (2018-10-24). Data Analytics for IT Networks: Developing Innovative Use Cases. Cisco Press. ISBN 9780135183441.
  26. ^ Oswald, Steve; Hart, Christopher (2013-05-22). "Trust based on bias: Cognitive constraints on source-related fallacies". OSSA Conference Archive.
  27. ^ Huczynski, Andrzej (2004). Influencing within organizations. Routledge.
  28. ^ Scruggs, Meredith (2020-01-01). "Impact of the CSI Effect and Authority Bias on Juror Decisions". Undergraduate Honors Theses.
  29. ^ Zaleskiewicz, Tomasz; Gasiorowska, Agata (2020). "Evaluating experts may serve psychological needs: Self-esteem, bias blind spot, and processing fluency explain confirmation effect in assessing financial advisors' authority". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 27 (1): 27–45. doi:10.1037/xap0000308. PMID 32597675.
  30. ^ Howard, Jonathan (2019), Howard, Jonathan (ed.), "Bandwagon Effect and Authority Bias", Cognitive Errors and Diagnostic Mistakes: A Case-Based Guide to Critical Thinking in Medicine, Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 21–56, doi:10.1007/978-3-319-93224-8_3, ISBN 978-3-319-93224-8, retrieved 2021-02-17

External links

  • Shatz, I. Authority Bias: What We Can Learn from the Milgram Obedience Experiments, Effectiviology
  • Reijman M Don’t fall for the authority bias The Star Online 9 Jul 2016

authority, bias, tendency, attribute, greater, accuracy, opinion, authority, figure, unrelated, content, more, influenced, that, opinion, individual, more, influenced, opinion, this, authority, figure, believing, their, views, more, credible, hence, place, gre. Authority bias is the tendency to attribute greater accuracy to the opinion of an authority figure unrelated to its content and be more influenced by that opinion 1 An individual is more influenced by the opinion of this authority figure believing their views to be more credible and hence place greater emphasis on the authority figure s viewpoint and are more likely to obey them This concept is considered one of the social cognitive biases or collective cognitive biases 2 Humans generally have a deep seated duty to authority and tend to comply when requested by an authority figure 3 Some scholars explain that individuals are motivated to view authority as deserving of their position and this legitimacy leads people to accept and obey the decisions that it makes 2 System justification theory articulates this phenomenon particularly within its position that there is a psychological motivation for believing in the steadiness stability and justness of the current social system 4 Authority bias can be measured concerning respect for authority where higher respect for authority positively correlates with the increased likelihood of exhibiting authority bias 5 Respect for authority is measured using the Respect for Authority Index RAI which averages responses on deference to the police A higher score on the RAI is indicative of higher respect for authority and hence strengthening the execution of authority bias Cultural differences in the strength of authority bias have been identified in which the differences in edits made to Wikipedia articles by administrators and regular users were compared for accuracy 6 self published source In Western Europe the bias has a negligible effect In Eastern Europe the bias is larger and the administrator s edits are perceived as more likely to be true despite the edits being inaccurate indicating a cultural difference in the extent to which authority bias is experienced Contents 1 Definition 2 Legitimacy of authority 2 1 Relative position in the social hierarchy 2 2 Uniform 2 3 Gender 3 Discovery 4 Real world effects 4 1 Advertising 4 2 Medicine 4 3 Business 5 Explanations 5 1 Evolution 5 2 Societal benefits 6 The role of other heuristics and biases 6 1 Confirmation bias 6 2 Bandwagon effect 7 See also 8 References 9 External linksDefinition EditAuthority bias a term popularised by American psychologist Stanley Milgram is defined as having an incorrectly high belief that the information verified by a person with formal authority is correct and therefore an individual is likely to be more influenced by them 6 self published source Individuals in positions of authority are seen to be treated more favourably where people believe their views with increased certainty though the role of authority alone is not always significant enough to directly affect decision making without this phenomenon being used in conjunction with other heuristics and biases The antonym of authority bias is blanket opposition to authority disregarding their knowledge and believing authority figures to have inherently false claims This relates to the view of anti authoritarianism Authority bias has many explanations rooted in the human need to obey authority figures Namely authority bias can be explained through evolutionary and social means The majority of people accept the police as a legitimate authority with their position in the social hierarchy giving them the right to apprehend those violating the law such as this Extinction Rebellion protester Legitimacy of authority EditAuthority bias is greater when the authority figure in question is seen to be legitimate 7 that is they are accepted to be in an authorized position of relative power and have the right to demand obedience Research has highlighted certain important characteristics that can mark one as a legitimate authority figure strengthening authority bias with individuals more likely to be influenced by and obey such figures These markers can affect whether people regard an individual as an authority figure Relative position in the social hierarchy Edit All societies are structured in a hierarchical manner 8 and often those who possess a higher social status are situated further up the hierarchy The majority of people accept that in order to maintain a co operative functioning society and avoid social chaos a certain degree of individual choice must be relinquished to authority figures often representing government institutions whose role means they have greater status in the social hierarchy For example most accept that in the criminal justice system the police have the right to exercise power over others and apprehend wrongdoers as their role indicates their position in the social hierarchy 9 and their authority This is often instilled in children from a young age with parents instructing them to defer to authority figures such as teachers and policemen and accept the opinions of authority figures as correct There is also a tendency to attribute greater accuracy to and respect the opinions of experts such as doctors For example most people have complete confidence in their doctor treating them as a trusted authority figure with a high social status 10 This normative behavior can be a useful shortcut but blind acceptance of expert advice can raise issues if it becomes an automatic response This was demonstrated by a study conducted by the psychiatrist Charles Hofling who found that 21 out of 22 nurses would have given patients a potentially fatal dose of a drug when asked to by a doctor despite this instruction contravening official guidance forbidding the administration of the drug Similar studies in other areas of society have demonstrated the problems that can arise as a result of unquestioningly accepting the opinions of those regarded as having higher status in the social hierarchy 11 Uniform such as that of a security guard can be an important marker of a legitimate authority figure and cause or enhance authority bias Uniform Edit Outward appearance can signify an individual s social status and relative position in the social hierarchy and consequently whether they are perceived as a legitimate authority figure When examining authority bias outward appearance manifested in clothing in particular uniform seems to have a profound effect on whether an individual is respected and obeyed as an authority figure Research conducted by Bickman and colleagues found that passers by were twice as likely to obey a confederate dressed as a security guard rather than a milkman when asked by the individual to complete tasks such as picking up litter or lending the confederate a coin for the parking meter 12 The effects of uniform on authority bias has been a fairly consistent and reliable finding as other studies such as those run by Milgram have reached similar conclusions In a variation of his original experiment Milgram found that obedience levels dropped from 65 to 20 when the instructions to enable shocks were given by an ordinary member of the public wearing plain clothes rather than a researcher signified by a white lab coat 13 In the real world attention bias may also play a role here with people selectively paying more attention to the uniform as an important symbol of power attributing greater accuracy to the opinions of the wearer and displaying more obedience to that figure in response However it is important to recognize that factors like uniform are situational and easily changed Thus an individual may masquerade as an authority figure and can elicit the authority bias response from others even though their authority may not be truly legitimate Though situational factors may point to an authority figure the effects of authority bias may be stronger when such situational factors are combined with innate factors such as gender Gender Edit Evidence also suggests that gender can be important in signifying an authority figure A subset of authority bias namely gender authority bias explains how in particular women may be more susceptible to authority bias than males In addition to women being more influenced by authority figures than males female authority figures may be less influential than their male counterparts Research has shown that both men and women associated men with high authority roles and women with low authority roles showing how gender can signify an authority figure and subsequently influence authority bias 14 As a result confirmation bias may also play a role here in what people interpret to be a characteristic of an authority figure as many associate higher authority roles with males confirmation bias suggests that people will tend to look for favor interpret a recall information that fits with this belief perpetuating this bias reinforcing the idea of males being more legitimate as authority figures Gender authority bias has been demonstrated across a variety of real world sectors In politics research into leadership using the Implicit Association Test IAT found that female political leaders tend to face greater resistance to their authority in comparison to their male counterparts 15 Authority bias is therefore strengthened when an authority figure is male instead of female in politics In finance males are often favored as authority figures being perceived to have greater control of resources and able to make better decisions demonstrated by the fact that they hold higher positions and on average earn 66 more than females in the finance industry 16 Research also suggests that women are more susceptible to authority bias as they are more influenced than men by hearing fake news from an authority figure 17 Discovery EditPrior to psychological research the most common example of this phenomenon was when people obeyed Hitler during World War II though such effects have been evident throughout history This relates to pluralistic ignorance in which authority figures are obeyed regardless of immorality The term authority bias was first mentioned in literature in reference to state authority bias in which it simply indicated a preference for being pro state or anti state in the US federal election 18 Nevertheless the first time authority bias was referenced in literature as a cognitive bias was a result of Milgram s experiment in which it was used to explain obedience to authority figures 19 Whilst Milgram did not directly use the term authority bias in his 1963 paper the obedience effect identified from his study became the primary example of authority bias Milgram s findings succeeded the reason why people during World War II obeyed Hitler participants voluntarily submitted to the authority figure the experimenter wearing a white lab coat signifying professionalism Authority bias is further strengthened through the use of uniforms to signify authority initially investigated in Milgram s situational variable where obedience decreased when the uniform of the experimenter was changed from a lab coat to everyday clothes but further replicated through Bickman s infamous research into obedience 20 where security guards are more likely to be obeyed without question and thus contributing directly to authority bias Real world effects EditAdvertising Edit Authority bias is used as a marketing strategy in order to increase the legitimacy of claims made about a product A common example in advertising is where toothpaste companies such as Sensodyne promote the validity of their claims by ensuring the dentists wear lab coats resulting in the consumer being more trustworthy of the product and consequently more likely to buy the product Personalised advertising in relation to political voting attitudes particularly in the US election relies on authority bias 21 Political campaigns are targeted specifically towards female voters who are more susceptible to the bias amplified through the use of social media in which political leaders and other figures of authority are used in ad campaigns to increase the effectiveness of their claims Medicine Edit The expert halo effect is synonymous with authority bias in medicine where the expert is seen as infallible 22 Issues arise in pharmaceutical settings in which non experts blindly follow expert s commands resulting in the distribution of harmful drugs 23 and inappropriate healthcare practices 24 A further issue concerning the extent to which an authority figure is perceived to be providing accurate information is apparent in cases such as that of Willie Jackson Forensic dentistry falsely proved Jackson to be guilty yet the authority bias strengthened the doctor s standpoint in a court of law as they had expert authority bias Consequently the negative effect of authority bias has led to wrongful convictions Business Edit The authority bias is demonstrated in the case of the highest paid persons opinion HIPPO impact which describes how employees and other stakeholders in the solution environment tend to go with the opinions and impressions of the highly paid people in an organization 25 Explanations EditEvolution Edit Evolution has established a dominance hierarchy in which it is an evolutionary advantage to obey authority figures as figures of authority have a greater allocation of resources and other means of survival The logical fallacy of ad verecundiam is evolutionary highlighting that experts are more reliable due to a lack of opposing information increasing trustworthiness 26 Societal benefits Edit In any society a diverse and widely accepted system of authority allows the development of sophisticated structures for the production of resources trade expansion and social control Notions of obedience and loyalty to the legitimate rule of others are generally accorded values in schools the law the military and in political systems The strength of the bias to obey a legitimate authority figure comes from systemic socialization practices designed to instill the perception that such obedience constitutes correct behavior and that genuine authority figures usually possess higher degrees of knowledge wisdom and power Different societies vary the terms of this dimension 6 self published source As a result authority bias can be rooted in the underlying social norms of society Consequently deference to authority can occur mindlessly as a kind of decision making short cut 27 The role of other heuristics and biases EditResearch support for the strength of authority bias is evident however the effect is not significant in some instances 28 Research is merely correlational and hence other behavioural effects experienced in conjunction with authority bias strengthen its effects Confirmation bias Edit An individual exhibiting authority bias may also be subject to experiencing confirmation bias which is the tendency to search for information that confirms your own existing beliefs Research suggests that greater authority is given to financial advisors who confirm one s existing opinions implying that authority bias is strengthened when it coincides with confirmation bias 29 Bandwagon effect Edit The bandwagon effect is where people adopt the ideologies of those surrounding them Society favours the opinions of authority figures hence it is a majority view which others support 30 See also EditAppeal to authority Asch conformity experiments Gaslighting Halo effect Misplaced loyalty Sheeple Speaking truth to power Systemic biasReferences Edit Milgram Stanley 1963 Behavioral study of obedience The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67 4 371 378 doi 10 1037 h0040525 PMID 14049516 a b Juarez Ramos Veronica 2019 Analyzing the Role of Cognitive Biases in the Decision Making Process Hershey PA IGI Global p 113 ISBN 9781522529798 Milgram Stanley 1974 Obedience to Authority An Experimental View Browstein Michael 2016 Implicit Bias and Philosophy Volume 1 Metaphysics and Epistemology Oxford Oxford University Press p 240 ISBN 9780198766186 Respect for Authority Cato Institute Retrieved 2021 02 16 a b c Hinnosaar Marit Hinnosaar Toomas 2012 Authority Bias marit hinnosaar net van der Toorn Jojanneke Tyler Tom R Jost John T 2011 01 01 More than fair Outcome dependence system justification and the perceived legitimacy of authority figures Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 1 127 138 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2010 09 003 ISSN 0022 1031 Cloutier Jasmin Cardenas Iniguez Carlos Gyurovski Ivo Barakzai Anam Li Tianyi 2016 01 01 Absher John R Cloutier Jasmin eds Chapter 9 Neuroimaging Investigations of Social Status and Social Hierarchies Neuroimaging Personality Social Cognition and Character San Diego Academic Press pp 187 203 doi 10 1016 b978 0 12 800935 2 00009 9 ISBN 978 0 12 800935 2 retrieved 2022 02 09 Jackson Jonathan Bradford Ben Hough Mike Myhill Andy Quinton Paul King Tyler Tom 2012 Why Do People Comply with the Law Legitimacy and the Influence of Legal Institutions SSRN Electronic Journal doi 10 2139 ssrn 1994490 ISSN 1556 5068 Confidence and trust in clinicians The Nuffield Trust 2018 10 15 Retrieved 2022 02 10 Tarnow Eugen 2000 01 01 Towards the Zero Accident Goal Assisting the First Officer Monitor and Challenge Captain Errors Journal of Aviation Aerospace Education amp Research 10 1 doi 10 15394 jaaer 2000 1269 ISSN 2329 258X Bickman Leonard 1974 The Social Power of a Uniform1 Journal of Applied Social Psychology 4 1 47 61 doi 10 1111 j 1559 1816 1974 tb02599 x ISSN 1559 1816 Milgram Experiment Simply Psychology www simplypsychology org Retrieved 2022 02 14 Rudman Laurie A Kilianski Stephen E 2000 Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Toward Female Authority Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26 11 1315 1328 doi 10 1177 0146167200263001 ISSN 0146 1672 Yang Ke Zhu John Jianjun Santoro Michael D 2017 08 01 Inter Firm Managerial Social Ties and Strategic Alliances Formation A Multiplexity Perspective Academy of Management Proceedings 2017 1 10151 doi 10 5465 AMBPP 2017 27 ISSN 0065 0668 Women finance firm directors earn 66 less than men in UK study finds Reuters 2021 03 01 Retrieved 2022 02 09 Jost Peter J Punder Johanna Schulze Lohoff Isabell 2020 04 01 Fake news Does perception matter more than the truth Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics 85 101513 doi 10 1016 j socec 2020 101513 ISSN 2214 8043 Sprague John D Incorporated Macmillan Publishing Company 1968 Voting Patterns of the United States Supreme Court Cases in Federalism 1889 1959 Ardent Media Ellis RM 2015 Middle Way Philosophy Omnibus Edition Lulu Press ISBN 9781326351892 Bickman Leonard 1974 The Social Power of a Uniform1 Journal of Applied Social Psychology 4 1 47 61 doi 10 1111 j 1559 1816 1974 tb02599 x ISSN 1559 1816 Kruikemeier Sanne Sezgin Minem Boerman Sophie C June 2016 Political Microtargeting Relationship Between Personalized Advertising on Facebook and Voters Responses Cyberpsychology Behavior and Social Networking 19 6 367 372 doi 10 1089 cyber 2015 0652 ISSN 2152 2715 PMID 27327063 Austin Jared P Foster Byron A 2019 07 01 How Pediatric Hospitalists Must Contend With the Expert Halo Effect Hospital Pediatrics 9 7 560 562 doi 10 1542 hpeds 2019 0053 ISSN 2154 1663 PMID 31175143 Austin Jared P Halvorson Stephanie A C 2019 02 05 Reducing the Expert Halo Effect on Pharmacy and Therapeutics Committees JAMA 321 5 453 454 doi 10 1001 jama 2018 20789 ISSN 1538 3598 PMID 30657521 Seshia Shashi Makhinson Michael Young G Bryan 2015 Cognitive biases plus covert subverters of healthcare evidence Evidence Based Medicine 21 2 41 5 doi 10 1136 ebmed 2015 110302 PMID 26612071 S2CID 353523 Garrett John 2018 10 24 Data Analytics for IT Networks Developing Innovative Use Cases Cisco Press ISBN 9780135183441 Oswald Steve Hart Christopher 2013 05 22 Trust based on bias Cognitive constraints on source related fallacies OSSA Conference Archive Huczynski Andrzej 2004 Influencing within organizations Routledge Scruggs Meredith 2020 01 01 Impact of the CSI Effect and Authority Bias on Juror Decisions Undergraduate Honors Theses Zaleskiewicz Tomasz Gasiorowska Agata 2020 Evaluating experts may serve psychological needs Self esteem bias blind spot and processing fluency explain confirmation effect in assessing financial advisors authority Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 27 1 27 45 doi 10 1037 xap0000308 PMID 32597675 Howard Jonathan 2019 Howard Jonathan ed Bandwagon Effect and Authority Bias Cognitive Errors and Diagnostic Mistakes A Case Based Guide to Critical Thinking in Medicine Cham Springer International Publishing pp 21 56 doi 10 1007 978 3 319 93224 8 3 ISBN 978 3 319 93224 8 retrieved 2021 02 17External links EditShatz I Authority Bias What We Can Learn from the Milgram Obedience Experiments Effectiviology Reijman M Don t fall for the authority bias The Star Online 9 Jul 2016 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Authority bias amp oldid 1110474224, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.