fbpx
Wikipedia

Expressive aphasia

Expressive aphasia (also known as Broca's aphasia) is a type of aphasia characterized by partial loss of the ability to produce language (spoken, manual,[1] or written), although comprehension generally remains intact.[2] A person with expressive aphasia will exhibit effortful speech. Speech generally includes important content words but leaves out function words that have more grammatical significance than physical meaning, such as prepositions and articles.[3] This is known as "telegraphic speech". The person's intended message may still be understood, but their sentence will not be grammatically correct. In very severe forms of expressive aphasia, a person may only speak using single word utterances.[4][5] Typically, comprehension is mildly to moderately impaired in expressive aphasia due to difficulty understanding complex grammar.[4][5]

Expressive aphasia
Other namesBroca's aphasia, non-fluent aphasia, agrammatic aphasia
Broca and Wernicke are two areas involved in language formation
SpecialtyNeurology, Psychiatry

It is caused by acquired damage to the frontal regions of the brain, such as Broca's area.[6] Expressive aphasia contrasts with receptive aphasia, in which patients are able to speak in grammatical sentences that lack semantic significance and generally also have trouble with comprehension.[3][7] Expressive aphasia differs from dysarthria, which is typified by a patient's inability to properly move the muscles of the tongue and mouth to produce speech. Expressive aphasia also differs from apraxia of speech, which is a motor disorder characterized by an inability to create and sequence motor plans for conscious speech.[8]

Signs and symptoms edit

Broca's (expressive) aphasia is a type of non-fluent aphasia in which an individual's speech is halting and effortful. Misarticulations or distortions of consonants and vowels, namely phonetic dissolution, are common. Individuals with expressive aphasia may only produce single words, or words in groups of two or three.[8] Long pauses between words are common and multi-syllabic words may be produced one syllable at a time with pauses between each syllable.[8] The prosody of a person with Broca's aphasia is compromised by shortened length of utterances and the presence of self-repairs and disfluencies.[9] Intonation and stress patterns are also deficient.[10]

For example, in the following passage, a patient with Broca's aphasia is trying to explain how he came to the hospital for dental surgery:

Yes... ah... Monday... er... Dad and Peter H... (his own name), and Dad.... er... hospital... and ah... Wednesday... Wednesday, nine o'clock... and oh... Thursday... ten o'clock, ah doctors... two... an' doctors... and er... teeth... yah.[10]

The speech of a person with expressive aphasia contains mostly content words such as nouns, verbs, and some adjectives. However, function words like conjunctions, articles, and prepositions are rarely used except for "and" which is prevalent in the speech of most patients with aphasia. The omission of function words makes the person's speech agrammatic.[8] A communication partner of a person with aphasia may say that the person's speech sounds telegraphic due to poor sentence construction and disjointed words.[8][10] For example, a person with expressive aphasia might say "Smart... university... smart... good... good..."[9]

Self-monitoring is typically well preserved in patients with Broca's aphasia.[8] They are usually aware of their communication deficits, and are more prone to depression and outbursts from frustration than are patients with other forms of aphasia.[11]

In general, word comprehension is preserved, allowing patients to have functional receptive language skills.[12] Individuals with Broca's aphasia understand most of the everyday conversation around them, but higher-level deficits in receptive language can occur.[13] Because comprehension is substantially impaired for more complex sentences, it is better to use simple language when speaking with an individual with expressive aphasia. This is exemplified by the difficulty to understand phrases or sentences with unusual structure. A typical patient with Broca's aphasia will misinterpret "the man is bitten by the dog" by switching the subject and object to "the dog is bitten by the man."[14]

Typically, people with expressive aphasia can understand speech and read better than they can produce speech and write.[8] The person's writing will resemble their speech and will be effortful, lacking cohesion, and containing mostly content words.[15] Letters will likely be formed clumsily and distorted and some may even be omitted. Although listening and reading are generally intact, subtle deficits in both reading and listening comprehension are almost always present during assessment of aphasia.[8]

Because Broca's area is anterior to the primary motor cortex, which is responsible for movement of the face, hands, and arms, a lesion affecting Broca's areas may also result in hemiparesis (weakness of both limbs on the same side of the body) or hemiplegia (paralysis of both limbs on the same side of the body).[8] The brain is wired contralaterally, which means the limbs on right side of the body are controlled by the left hemisphere and vice versa.[16] Therefore, when Broca's area or surrounding areas in the left hemisphere are damaged, hemiplegia or hemiparesis often occurs on the right side of the body in individuals with Broca's aphasia.

Severity of expressive aphasia varies among patients. Some people may only have mild deficits and detecting problems with their language may be difficult. In the most extreme cases, patients may be able to produce only a single word. Even in such cases, over-learned and rote-learned speech patterns may be retained–[17] for instance, some patients can count from one to ten, but cannot produce the same numbers in novel conversation.

Manual language and aphasia edit

In deaf patients who use manual language (such as American Sign Language), damage to the left hemisphere of the brain leads to disruptions in their signing ability.[1] Paraphasic errors similar to spoken language have been observed; whereas in spoken language a phonemic substitution would occur (e.g. "tagle" instead of "table"), in ASL case studies errors in movement, hand position, and morphology have been noted. Agrammatism, or the lack of grammatical morphemes in sentence production, has also been observed in lifelong users of ASL who have left hemisphere damage. The lack of syntactic accuracy shows that the errors in signing are not due to damage to the motor cortex, but rather area manifestation of the damage to the language-producing area of the brain. Similar symptoms have been seen in a patient with left hemisphere damage whose first language was British Sign Language,[18] further showing that damage to the left hemisphere primarily hinders linguistic ability, not motor ability. In contrast, patients who have damage to non-linguistic areas on the left hemisphere have been shown to be fluent in signing, but are unable to comprehend written language.[1]

Overlap with receptive aphasia edit

In addition to difficulty expressing oneself, individuals with expressive aphasia are also noted to commonly have trouble with comprehension in certain linguistic areas. This agrammatism overlaps with receptive aphasia, but can be seen in patients who have expressive aphasia without being diagnosed as having receptive aphasia. The most well-noted of these are object-relative clauses, object Wh- questions, and topicalized structures (placing the topic at the beginning of the sentence).[19] These three concepts all share phrasal movement, which can cause words to lose their thematic roles when they change order in the sentence.[19] This is often not an issue for people without agrammatic aphasias, but many people with aphasia rely heavily on word order to understand roles that words play within the sentence.[20]

Causes edit

More common edit

Less common edit

Common causes edit

The most common cause of expressive aphasia is stroke. A stroke is caused by hypoperfusion (lack of oxygen) to an area of the brain, which is commonly caused by thrombosis or embolism. Some form of aphasia occurs in 34 to 38% of stroke patients.[23] Expressive aphasia occurs in approximately 12% of new cases of aphasia caused by stroke.[24]

In most cases, expressive aphasia is caused by a stroke in Broca's area or the surrounding vicinity. Broca's area is in the lower part of the premotor cortex in the language dominant hemisphere and is responsible for planning motor speech movements. However, cases of expressive aphasia have been seen in patients with strokes in other areas of the brain.[8] Patients with classic symptoms of expressive aphasia in general have more acute brain lesions, whereas patients with larger, widespread lesions exhibit a variety of symptoms that may be classified as global aphasia or left unclassified.[23]

Expressive aphasia can also be caused by trauma to the brain, tumor, cerebral hemorrhage[25] and by extradural abscess.[26]

Understanding lateralization of brain function is important for understanding which areas of the brain cause expressive aphasia when damaged. In the past, it has been believed that the area for language production differs between left and right-handed individuals. If this were true, damage to the homologous region of Broca's area in the right hemisphere should cause aphasia in a left-handed individual. More recent studies have shown that even left-handed individuals typically have language functions only in the left hemisphere. However, left-handed individuals are more likely to have a dominance of language in the right hemisphere.[6]

Uncommon causes edit

Less common causes of expressive aphasia include primary autoimmune phenomenon and autoimmune phenomenon that are secondary to cancer (as a paraneoplastic syndrome) have been listed as the primary hypothesis for several cases of aphasia, especially when presenting with other psychiatric disturbances and focal neurological deficits. Many case reports exist describing paraneoplastic aphasia, and the reports that are specific tend to describe expressive aphasia.[27][28][29][30][31] Although most cases attempt to exclude micro-metastasis, it is likely that some cases of paraneoplastic aphasia are actually extremely small metastasis to the vocal motor regions.[30]

Neurodegenerative disorders may present with aphasia. Alzheimer's disease may present with either fluent aphasia or expressive aphasia. There are case reports of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease presenting with expressive aphasia.[32][33]

Diagnosis edit

Expressive aphasia is classified as non-fluent aphasia, as opposed to fluent aphasia.[5] Diagnosis is done on a case-by-case basis, as lesions often affect the surrounding cortex and deficits are highly variable among patients with aphasia.[34]

A physician is typically the first person to recognize aphasia in a patient who is being treated for damage to the brain. Routine processes for determining the presence and location of lesion in the brain include magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and computed tomography (CT) scans. The physician will complete a brief assessment of the patient's ability to understand and produce language. For further diagnostic testing, the physician will refer the patient to a speech-language pathologist, who will complete a comprehensive evaluation.[35]

In order to diagnose a patient with Broca's aphasia, there are certain commonly used tests and procedures. The Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) classifies individuals based on their scores on the subtests; spontaneous speech, auditory comprehension, repetition, and naming.[8] The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) can inform users what specific type of aphasia they may have, infer the location of lesion, and assess current language abilities. The Porch Index of Communication Ability (PICA) can predict potential recovery outcomes of the patients with aphasia. Quality of life measurement is also an important assessment tool.[36] Tests such as the Assessment for Living with Aphasia (ALA) and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) allow for therapists to target skills that are important and meaningful for the individual.[8]

In addition to formal assessments, patient and family interviews are valid and important sources of information. The patient's previous hobbies, interests, personality, and occupation are all factors that will not only impact therapy but may motivate them throughout the recovery process.[35] Patient interviews and observations allow professionals to learn the priorities of the patient and family and determine what the patient hopes to regain in therapy. Observations of the patient may also be beneficial to determine where to begin treatment. The current behaviors and interactions of the patient will provide the therapist with more insight about the client and their individual needs.[8] Other information about the patient can be retrieved from medical records, patient referrals from physicians, and the nursing staff.[35]

In non-speaking patients who use manual languages, diagnosis is often based on interviews from the patient's acquaintances, noting the differences in sign production pre- and post-damage to the brain.[18] Many of these patients will also begin to rely on non-linguistic gestures to communicate, rather than signing since their language production is hindered.[37]

Treatment edit

Currently, there is no standard treatment for expressive aphasia. Most aphasia treatment is individualized based on a patient's condition and needs as assessed by a speech language pathologist. Patients go through a period of spontaneous recovery following brain injury in which they regain a great deal of language function.[38]

In the months following injury or stroke, most patients receive traditional treatment for a few hours per day. Among other exercises, patients practice the repetition of words and phrases. Mechanisms are also taught in traditional treatment to compensate for lost language function such as drawing and using phrases that are easier to pronounce.[39]

Emphasis is placed on establishing a basis for communication with family and caregivers in everyday life. Treatment is individualized based on the patient's own priorities, along with the family's input.[8][40]

A patient may have the option of individual or group treatment. Although less common, group treatment has been shown to have advantageous outcomes. Some types of group treatments include family counseling, maintenance groups, support groups and treatment groups.[41]

Augmentative and Alternative Communication edit

Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) refers to a set of tools and strategies that support or replace verbal communication for individuals with communication disorders, such as Broca's aphasia or other conditions that affect speech and language abilities. AAC is designed to enhance communication and may be used as a temporary or permanent solution, depending on the individual's needs. Here are some key aspects of AAC:

  1. Communication Aids:
    • Low-Tech AAC: This includes simple, non-electronic communication aids such as communication boards, picture books, or communication charts. Users can point to or select symbols or pictures to convey their messages[1]
    • High-Tech AAC: Involves electronic devices such as speech-generating devices (SGDs) or tablet-based communication apps. These devices use synthesized speech or recorded messages to facilitate communication. Users can select words, phrases, or symbols on a screen to express themselves.
  2. Symbols and Representations:
    • Symbols used in AAC can vary and may include pictures, icons, words, or a combination of these. Symbols are chosen based on the individual's cognitive and language abilities.
  3. Types of AAC Systems:
    • Unaided AAC: Relies on the user's body to convey messages without external tools, such as using gestures, facial expressions, or sign language.
    • Aided AAC: Involves external tools or devices, such as communication boards, speech-generating devices, or computer-based systems.
  4. Vocabulary and Language Systems:
    • Core Vocabulary: Focuses on essential words that are frequently used across various contexts. Core vocabulary systems aim to provide users with a versatile set of words to express a wide range of messages.
    • Fringe Vocabulary: Includes specific words related to an individual's unique needs, interests, or daily activities. Fringe vocabulary supplements core vocabulary to make communication more personalized.[17]
  5. Customization and Individualization:
    • AAC systems are highly customizable to meet the unique needs of each user. Therapists work with individuals and their families to tailor the system to the user's abilities, preferences, and communication goals.
  6. Training and Support:
    • Users of AAC systems, as well as their caregivers and support networks, receive training to effectively use the communication tools. Training may involve learning how to navigate electronic devices, program personalized messages, or understand the meaning of symbols.
  7. Integration with Therapy:
    • AAC is often integrated into speech and language therapy sessions. Therapists use AAC tools to facilitate communication practice and help individuals with communication disorders improve their language skills.[7]
  8. Social and Emotional Aspects:
    • AAC not only addresses the functional aspects of communication but also considers the social and emotional dimensions. It plays a crucial role in helping individuals with communication disorders participate more fully in social interactions and express their thoughts and feelings.

AAC is a dynamic and evolving field, and advancements in technology continue to enhance the range and effectiveness of communication tools available for individuals with speech and language challenges. The selection of AAC strategies depends on factors such as the individual's abilities, preferences, and the specific nature of their communication disorder.

Melodic intonation therapy edit

Melodic intonation therapy was inspired by the observation that individuals with non-fluent aphasia sometimes can sing words or phrases that they normally cannot speak. "Melodic Intonation Therapy was begun as an attempt to use the intact melodic/prosodic processing skills of the right hemisphere in those with aphasia to help cue retrieval words and expressive language."[42] It is believed that this is because singing capabilities are stored in the right hemisphere of the brain, which is likely to remain unaffected after a stroke in the left hemisphere.[43] However, recent evidence demonstrates that the capability of individuals with aphasia to sing entire pieces of text may actually result from rhythmic features and the familiarity with the lyrics.[44]

The goal of Melodic Intonation Therapy is to utilize singing to access the language-capable regions in the right hemisphere and use these regions to compensate for lost function in the left hemisphere. The natural musical component of speech was used to engage the patients' ability to produce phrases. A clinical study revealed that singing and rhythmic speech may be similarly effective in the treatment of non-fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech.[45] Moreover, evidence from randomized controlled trials is still needed to confirm that Melodic Intonation Therapy is suitable to improve propositional utterances and speech intelligibility in individuals with (chronic) non-fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech.[46][47]

Melodic Intonation Therapy appears to work particularly well in patients who have had a unilateral, left hemisphere stroke, show poor articulation, are non-fluent or have severely restricted speech output, have moderately preserved auditory comprehension, and show good motivation. MIT therapy on average lasts for 1.5 hours per day for five days per week. At the lowest level of therapy, simple words and phrases (such as "water" and "I love you") are broken down into a series of high- and low-pitch syllables. With increased treatment, longer phrases are taught and less support is provided by the therapist. Patients are taught to say phrases using the natural melodic component of speaking and continuous voicing is emphasized. The patient is also instructed to use the left hand to tap the syllables of the phrase while the phrases are spoken. Tapping is assumed to trigger the rhythmic component of speaking to utilize the right hemisphere.[43]

FMRI studies have shown that Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) uses both sides of the brain to recover lost function, as opposed to traditional therapies that utilize only the left hemisphere. In MIT, individuals with small lesions in the left hemisphere seem to recover by activation of the left hemisphere perilesional cortex. Meanwhile, individuals with larger left-hemisphere lesions show a recruitment of the use of language-capable regions in the right hemisphere.[43] The interpretation of these results is still a matter of debate. For example, it remains unclear whether changes in neural activity in the right hemisphere result from singing or from the intensive use of common phrases, such as "thank you", "how are you?" or "I am fine." This type of phrases falls into the category of formulaic language and is known to be supported by neural networks of the intact right hemisphere.[48]

A pilot study reported positive results when comparing the efficacy of a modified form of MIT to no treatment in people with nonfluent aphasia with damage to their left-brain. A randomized controlled trial was conducted and the study reported benefits of utilizing modified MIT treatment early in the recovery phase for people with nonfluent aphasia.[49]

Melodic Intonation Therapy is used by music therapists, board-certified professionals that use music as a therapeutic tool to effect certain non-musical outcomes in their patients. Speech language pathologists can also use this therapy for individuals who have had a left hemisphere stroke and non-fluent aphasias such as Broca's or even apraxia of speech.

Constraint-induced therapy edit

Constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) is based on similar principles as constraint-induced movement therapy developed by Dr. Edward Taub at the University of Alabama at Birmingham.[39][50] Constraint-induced movement therapy is based on the idea that a person with an impairment (physical or communicative) develops a "learned nonuse" by compensating for the lost function with other means such as using an unaffected limb by a paralyzed individual or drawing by a patient with aphasia.[51] In constraint-induced movement therapy, the alternative limb is constrained with a glove or sling and the patient is forced to use the affected limb. In constraint-induced aphasia therapy the interaction is guided by communicative need in a language game context, picture cards, barriers making it impossible to see other players' cards, and other materials, so that patients are encouraged ("constrained") to use the remaining verbal abilities to succeed in the communication game.[50]

Two important principles of constraint-induced aphasia therapy are that treatment is very intense, with sessions lasting for up to 6 hours over the course of 10 days and that language is used in a communication context in which it is closely linked to (nonverbal) actions.[39][50] These principles are motivated by neuroscience insights about learning at the level of nerve cells (synaptic plasticity) and the coupling between cortical systems for language and action in the human brain.[51] Constraint-induced therapy contrasts sharply with traditional therapy by the strong belief that mechanisms to compensate for lost language function, such as gesturing or writing, should not be used unless absolutely necessary, even in everyday life.[39]

It is believed that CIAT works by the mechanism of increased neuroplasticity. By constraining an individual to use only speech, it is believed that the brain is more likely to reestablish old neural pathways and recruit new neural pathways to compensate for lost function.[52]

The strongest results of CIAT have been seen in patients with chronic aphasia (lasting over 6 months). Studies of CIAT have confirmed that further improvement is possible even after a patient has reached a "plateau" period of recovery.[39][50] It has also been proven that the benefits of CIAT are retained long term. However, improvements only seem to be made while a patient is undergoing intense therapy.[39] Recent work has investigated combining constraint-induced aphasia therapy with drug treatment, which led to an amplification of therapy benefits.[53]

Medication edit

In addition to active speech therapy, pharmaceuticals have also been considered as a useful treatment for expressive aphasia. This area of study is relatively new and much research continues to be conducted.

The following drugs have been suggested for use in treating aphasia and their efficacy has been studied in control studies.

The most effect has been shown by piracetam and amphetamine, which may increase cerebral plasticity and result in an increased capability to improve language function. It has been seen that piracetam is most effective when treatment is begun immediately following stroke. When used in chronic cases it has been much less efficient.[55]

Bromocriptine has been shown by some studies to increase verbal fluency and word retrieval with therapy than with just therapy alone.[54] Furthermore, its use seems to be restricted to non-fluent aphasia.[53]

Donepezil has shown a potential for helping chronic aphasia.[53]

No study has established irrefutable evidence that any drug is an effective treatment for aphasia therapy.[54] Furthermore, no study has shown any drug to be specific for language recovery.[53] Comparison between the recovery of language function and other motor function using any drug has shown that improvement is due to a global increase plasticity of neural networks.[54]

Transcranial magnetic stimulation edit

In transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), magnetic fields are used to create electrical currents in specified cortical regions. The procedure is a painless and noninvasive method of stimulating the cortex. TMS works by suppressing the inhibition process in certain areas of the brain.[56] By suppressing the inhibition of neurons by external factors, the targeted area of the brain may be reactivated and thereby recruited to compensate for lost function. Research has shown that patients can demonstrate increased object naming ability with regular transcranial magnetic stimulation than patients not receiving TMS.[56] Furthermore, research suggests this improvement is sustained upon the completion of TMS therapy.[56] However, some patients fail to show any significant improvement from TMS which indicates the need for further research of this treatment.[57]

Treatment of underlying forms edit

Described as the linguistic approach to the treatment of expressive aphasia, treatment begins by emphasizing and educating patients on the thematic roles of words within sentences.[58] Sentences that are usually problematic will be reworded into active-voiced, declarative phrasings of their non-canonical counterparts.[58] The simpler sentence phrasings are then transformed into variations that are more difficult to interpret. For example, many individuals who have expressive aphasia struggle with Wh- sentences. "What" and "who" questions are problematic sentences that this treatment method attempts to improve, and they are also two interrogative particles that are strongly related to each other because they reorder arguments from the declarative counterparts.[58] For instance, therapists have used sentences like, "Who is the boy helping?" and "What is the boy fixing?" because both verbs are transitive- they require two arguments in the form of a subject and a direct object, but not necessarily an indirect object.[58] In addition, certain question particles are linked together based on how the reworded sentence is formed. Training "who" sentences increased the generalizations of non-trained "who" sentences as well as untrained "what" sentences, and vice versa.[58] Likewise, "where" and "when" question types are very closely linked. "What" and "who" questions alter placement of arguments, and "where" and "when" sentences move adjunct phrases.[58] Training is in the style of: "The man parked the car in the driveway. What did the man park in the driveway?"[58] Sentence training goes on in this manner for more domains, such as clefts and sentence voice.[58]

Results: Patients' use of sentence types used in the TUF treatment will improve, subjects will generalize sentences of similar category to those used for treatment in TUF, and results are applied to real-world conversations with others.[58] Generalization of sentence types used can be improved when the treatment progresses in the order of more complex sentences to more elementary sentences. Treatment has been shown to affect on-line (real-time) processing of trained sentences and these results can be tracked using fMRI mappings.[58] Training of Wh- sentences has led improvements in three main areas of discourse for aphasics: increased average length of utterances, higher proportions of grammatical sentences, and larger ratios of numbers of verbs to nouns produced.[58] Patients also showed improvements in verb argument structure productions and assigned thematic roles to words in utterances with more accuracy.[58] In terms of on-line sentence processing, patients having undergone this treatment discriminate between anomalous and non-anomalous sentences with more accuracy than control groups and are closer to levels of normalcy than patients not having participated in this treatment.[58]

Mechanisms of recovery edit

Mechanisms for recovery differ from patient to patient. Some mechanisms for recovery occur spontaneously after damage to the brain, whereas others are caused by the effects of language therapy.[53] FMRI studies have shown that recovery can be partially attributed to the activation of tissue around the damaged area and the recruitment of new neurons in these areas to compensate for the lost function. Recovery may also be caused in very acute lesions by a return of blood flow and function to damaged tissue that has not died around an injured area.[53] It has been stated by some researchers that the recruitment and recovery of neurons in the left hemisphere opposed to the recruitment of similar neurons in the right hemisphere is superior for long-term recovery and continued rehabilitation.[59] It is thought that, because the right hemisphere is not intended for full language function, using the right hemisphere as a mechanism of recovery is effectively a "dead-end" and can lead only to partial recovery.[60]

There is evidence to support that, among all types of therapies, one of the most important factors and best predictors for a successful outcome is the intensity of the therapy. By comparing the length and intensity of various methods of therapies, it was proven that intensity is a better predictor of recovery than the method of therapy used.[61]

Prognosis edit

In most individuals with expressive aphasia, the majority of recovery is seen within the first year following a stroke or injury. The majority of this improvement is seen in the first four weeks in therapy following a stroke and slows thereafter.[23] However, this timeline will vary depending upon the type of stroke experienced by the patient. Patients who experienced an ischemic stroke may recover in the days and weeks following the stroke, and then experience a plateau and gradual slowing of recovery. On the contrary, patients who experienced a hemorrhagic stroke experience a slower recovery in the first 4–8 weeks, followed by a faster recovery which eventually stabilizes.[62]

Numerous factors impact the recovery process and outcomes. Site and extent of lesion greatly impacts recovery. Other factors that may affect prognosis are age, education, gender, and motivation.[63] Occupation, handedness, personality, and emotional state may also be associated with recovery outcomes.[8]

Studies have also found that prognosis of expressive aphasia correlates strongly with the initial severity of impairment.[24] However, it has been seen that continued recovery is possible years after a stroke with effective treatment.[39] Timing and intensity of treatment is another factor that impacts outcomes. Research suggests that even in later stages of recovery, intervention is effective at improving function, as well as, preventing loss of function.[38]

Unlike receptive aphasia, patients with expressive aphasia are aware of their errors in language production. This may further motivate a person with expressive aphasia to progress in treatment, which would affect treatment outcomes.[23] On the other hand, awareness of impairment may lead to higher levels of frustration, depression, anxiety, or social withdrawal, which have been proven to negatively affect a person's chance of recovery.[64]

History edit

Expressive aphasia was first identified by the French neurologist Paul Broca. By examining the brains of deceased individuals having acquired expressive aphasia in life, he concluded that language ability is localized in the ventroposterior region of the frontal lobe. One of the most important aspects of Paul Broca's discovery was the observation that the loss of proper speech in expressive aphasia is due to the brain's loss of ability to produce language, as opposed to the mouth's loss of ability to produce words.[6]

The discoveries of Paul Broca were made during the same period of time as the German Neurologist Carl Wernicke, who was also studying brains of aphasiacs post-mortem and identified the region now known as Wernicke's area. Discoveries of both men contributed to the concept of localization, which states that specific brain functions are all localized to a specific area of the brain. While both men made significant contributions to the field of aphasia, it was Carl Wernicke who realized the difference between patients with aphasia that could not produce language and those that could not comprehend language (the essential difference between expressive and receptive aphasia).[6]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d Hicoka, Gregory (1 April 1998). "The neural organization of language: evidence from sign language aphasia". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (4): 129–136. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01154-1. PMID 21227109. S2CID 7018568.
  2. ^ "Broca's Aphasia - National Aphasia Association". National Aphasia Association. Retrieved 2017-04-11.
  3. ^ a b Fromkin, Victoria; Rodman, Robert; Hyams, Nina (2014). An Introduction to Language. Boston, MA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. pp. 464–465. ISBN 978-1133310686.
  4. ^ a b ASHA.org
  5. ^ a b c . American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Archived from the original on 2017-05-07. Retrieved 2024-02-21.
  6. ^ a b c d Purves, D. (2008). Neuroscience (fourth ed.). Sinauer Associates, Inc. ISBN 978-0-87893-742-4.
  7. ^ a b Nakai, Y; Jeong, JW; Brown, EC; Rothermel, R; Kojima, K; Kambara, T; Shah, A; Mittal, S; Sood, S; Asano, E (2017). "Three- and four-dimensional mapping of speech and language in patients with epilepsy". Brain. 140 (5): 1351–1370. doi:10.1093/brain/awx051. PMC 5405238. PMID 28334963.
  8. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Brookshire, Robert (2007). Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders. St. Louis, MO: Mosby. ISBN 978-0323045315.
  9. ^ a b Manasco, H. (2014). The Aphasias. In Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders (Vol. 1, p. 91). Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning.
  10. ^ a b c Goodglass, H.; N. Geschwind (1976). "Language disorders". In E. Carterette and M.P. Friedman (ed.). Handbook of Perception: Language and Speech. Vol VII. New York: Academic Press.
  11. ^ Chapey, Roberta (2008). Language Intervention Strategies in Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. p. 8. ISBN 978-0-7817-6981-5. [verification needed]
  12. ^ Manasco (2014). Katey Birtcher; et al. (eds.). INtroduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Pennsylvania, USA: William Brottmiller. pp. 80–81. ISBN 9781449652449.
  13. ^ Manasco, M. Hunter (2013-02-06). Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Jones & Bartlett Learning. p. 80. ISBN 9781449652449.
  14. ^ . Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (1). Archived from the original on 2004-05-18. Retrieved 2006-05-10.
  15. ^ Chapey, Roberta (2008). Language Intervention Strategies in Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. p. 8. ISBN 978-0-7817-6981-5.
  16. ^ Teasell, Robert (2003). "Stroke recovery and rehabilitation". Stroke. 34 (2): 365–366. doi:10.1161/01.str.0000054630.33395.e2. PMID 12574538.
  17. ^ a b . Neuropathologies of Language and Cognition. Archived from the original on 2006-04-27. Retrieved 2006-05-10.
  18. ^ a b Marshall, Jane (15 June 2004). "Aphasia in a user of British Sign Language: Dissociation between sign and gesture". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 21 (5): 537–554. doi:10.1080/02643290342000249. PMID 21038221. S2CID 27849117.
  19. ^ a b Friedmann, Naama; Gvion, Aviah; Novogrodsky, Rama (2006). "Syntactic Movement in Agrammatism and S-SLI: Two Different Impairments". In Adriana Belletti; et al. (eds.). (PDF). Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars Press. pp. 197–210. ISBN 9781847180285. OCLC 133524617. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-12-11. Retrieved 2024-02-21.
  20. ^ Bates, Elizabeth A.; Friederici, Angela D.; Wulfeck, Beverly B.; Juarez, Larry A. (1988-03-01). "On the preservation of word order in aphasia: Cross-linguistic evidence". Brain and Language. 33 (2): 323–364. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(88)90072-7. ISSN 0093-934X. PMID 3359173. S2CID 23236428.
  21. ^ Marienfeld, Carla B.; DiCapua, Daniel B.; Sze, Gordon K.; Goldstein, Jonathan M. (June 2010). "Expressive Aphasia as a Presentation of Encephalitis with Bartonella henselae Infection in an Immunocompetent Adult". The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine. 83 (2): 67–71. ISSN 0044-0086. PMC 2892771. PMID 20589186.
  22. ^ Lee, Ji Hyun; Kim, Ye An; Moon, Joon Ho; Min, Se Hee; Song, Young Shin; Choi, Sung Hee (November 2016). "Expressive aphasia as the manifestation of hyperglycemic crisis in type 2 diabetes". The Korean Journal of Internal Medicine. 31 (6): 1187–1190. doi:10.3904/kjim.2014.379. ISSN 1226-3303. PMC 5094916. PMID 26968185.
  23. ^ a b c d Bakheit, AMO; Shaw, S; Carrington, S; Griffiths, S (2007). "The rate and extent of improvement with therapy from the different types of aphasia in the first year of stroke". Integumentary Rehabilitation. 21 (10): 941–949. doi:10.1177/0269215507078452. PMID 17981853. S2CID 25995618.
  24. ^ a b Pedersen, PM; Vinter, K; Olsen, TS (2004). "Aphasia after stroke: Type, severity, and prognosis - The Copenhagen aphasia study". Cerebrovascular Diseases. 17 (1): 35–43. doi:10.1159/000073896. PMID 14530636. S2CID 11754713.
  25. ^ Orzeren, A; F Koc; M Demirkiran; A Sonmezler (2006). "Global aphasia due to left thalamic hemorrhage". Neurology India. 54 (4): 415–417. doi:10.4103/0028-3886.28118. PMID 17114855. S2CID 23023504.
  26. ^ Commondoor, R.; Eisenhut, M.; Fowler, C.; Kirollos, R. W. & Nathwani, N. (2009). "Transient Broca's Aphasia as Feature of an Extradural Abscess". Pediatric Neurology. 40 (1): 50–53. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2008.06.018. PMID 19068255.
  27. ^ McKeon, Andrew (April 2013). "Paraneoplastic and Other Autoimmune Disorders of the Central Nervous System". The Neurohospitalist. 3 (2): 53–64. doi:10.1177/1941874412453339. ISSN 1941-8744. PMC 3726118. PMID 23983888.
  28. ^ Yeung, Darwin F; Hsu, Rose (2014-08-05). "Expressive aphasia in a patient with chronic myelomonocytic leukemia". SpringerPlus. 3: 406. doi:10.1186/2193-1801-3-406. ISSN 2193-1801. PMC 4130962. PMID 25126489.
  29. ^ Will, A.; Akalin, Murat (2012-04-24). "Paraneoplastic Limbic Encephalitis with NMDA Receptor (NR1) Antibodies in Breast Cancer (S08.007)". Neurology. 78 (1 Supplement): S08.007. doi:10.1212/WNL.78.1_MeetingAbstracts.S08.007. ISSN 0028-3878.
  30. ^ a b Darnell, Robert; Darnell, Robert B.; Posner, Jerome B. (2011-08-22). Paraneoplastic Syndromes. Oxford University Press, USA. ISBN 9780199772735.
  31. ^ Lancaster, Eric (April 2015). "Continuum: The Paraneoplastic Disorders". Continuum (Minneapolis, Minn.). 21 (2 0): 452–475. doi:10.1212/01.CON.0000464180.89580.88. ISSN 1080-2371. PMC 4443809. PMID 25837906.
  32. ^ Mahboob, Hafiz B.; Kaokaf, Kazi H.; Gonda, Jeremy M. (2018-02-14). "Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease Presenting as Expressive Aphasia and Nonconvulsive Status Epilepticus". Case Reports in Critical Care. 2018: 5053175. doi:10.1155/2018/5053175. ISSN 2090-6420. PMC 5832162. PMID 29666711.
  33. ^ "Primary Progressive Aphasia - National Aphasia Association". National Aphasia Association. Retrieved 2018-11-26.
  34. ^ "Aphasia FAQ's". National Aphasia Association. November 7, 2017.
  35. ^ a b c "Aphasia". National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. March 6, 2017. Retrieved 2024-02-21.
  36. ^ McCaffrey, Patrick (1999). . The Neuroscience on the Web Series. California State University, Chico. Archived from the original on 2007-06-15. Retrieved 2017-11-08.
  37. ^ Hogrefe, Katharina; Ziegler, Wolfram; Wiesmayer, Susanne; Weidinger, Nicole; Goldenberg, Georg (2013-09-01). "The actual and potential use of gestures for communication in aphasia". Aphasiology. 27 (9): 1070–1089. doi:10.1080/02687038.2013.803515. ISSN 0268-7038. S2CID 145152352.
  38. ^ a b Raymer, Anastasia (February 2008). "Translational Research in Aphasia: from Neuroscience to Neurorehabilitation". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 51 (1): 259–275. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/020). PMID 18230850.
  39. ^ a b c d e f g Meinzer, Marcus; Thomas Elbert; Daniela Djundja; Edward Taub (2007). "Extending the Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy (CIMT) approach to cognitive functions: Constraint-Induced Aphasia Therapy (CIAT) of chronic aphasia". NeuroRehabilitation. 22 (4): 311–318. doi:10.3233/NRE-2007-22409. PMID 17971622.
  40. ^ "Aphasia - Treatment". ASHA Practice Portal. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Retrieved August 7, 2017.
  41. ^ Fama, Mackenzie E.; Baron, Christine R.; Hatfield, Brooke; Turkeltaub, Peter E. (August 2016). "Group Therapy as a Social Context for Aphasia Recovery: A pilot, observational study in an acute rehabilitation hospital". Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation. 23 (4): 276–283. doi:10.1080/10749357.2016.1155277. ISSN 1074-9357. PMC 4949973. PMID 27077989.
  42. ^ Wilson Sarah J (2006). "A Case Study of the Efficacy of Melodic Intonation Therapy" (PDF). Music Perception. 24 (1): 23–36. doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.23. ISSN 0730-7829.
  43. ^ a b c Schlaug, Gottfried; Sarah Marchina; Andrea Norton (2008). "From Singing to Speaking: Why singing may lead to recovery of expressive language function in patients with Broca's Aphasia". Music Perception. 25 (4): 315–319. doi:10.1525/mp.2008.25.4.315. PMC 3010734. PMID 21197418.
  44. ^ Stahl, Benjamin; Kotz, Sonja A.; Henseler, Ilona; Turner, Robert; Geyer, Stefan (2011). "Rhythm in disguise: why singing may not hold the key to recovery from aphasia". Brain. 134 (10): 3083–3093. doi:10.1093/brain/awr240. ISSN 0006-8950. PMC 3187543. PMID 21948939.
  45. ^ Stahl, Benjamin; Henseler, Ilona; Turner, Robert; Geyer, Stefan; Kotz, Sonja A. (2013). "How to engage the right brain hemisphere in aphasics without even singing: Evidence for two paths of speech recovery". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7 (35): 1–12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00035. ISSN 1662-5161. PMC 3583105. PMID 23450277.
  46. ^ van der Meulen, I.; van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. W.; Heijenbrok-Kal, M. H.; Visch-Brink, E. G.; Ribbers, G. M. (2014). "The efficacy and timing of Melodic Intonation Therapy in subacute aphasia". Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 28 (6): 536–544. doi:10.1177/1545968313517753. PMID 24449708. S2CID 6495987.
  47. ^ Zumbansen, Anna; Peretz, Isabelle; Hébert, Sylvie (2014). "Melodic Intonation Therapy: Back to Basics for Future Research". Frontiers in Neurology. 5 (7): 7. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00007. PMC 3904283. PMID 24478754.
  48. ^ Stahl, Benjamin; Kotz, Sonja A. (2013). "Facing the music: Three issues in current research on singing and aphasia". Frontiers in Psychology. 5 (1033): 1–4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01033. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 4172097. PMID 25295017.
  49. ^ Conklyn, D; Novak, E; Boissy, A; Bethoux, F; Chemali, K (2012). "The Effects of Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy on Nonfluent Aphasia: A Pilot Study". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 55 (5): 1463–1471. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0105). PMID 22411278.
  50. ^ a b c d Pulvermuller, Friedemann; et al. (2001). "Constraint-Induced Therapy of Chronic Aphasia following Stroke". Stroke. 32 (7): 1621–1626. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.492.3416. doi:10.1161/01.STR.32.7.1621. PMID 11441210. S2CID 673662.
  51. ^ a b Pulvermuller, Friedemann; Marcelo Berthier (2008). "Aphasia therapy on a neuroscience basis". Aphasiology. 22 (6): 563–599. doi:10.1080/02687030701612213. PMC 2557073. PMID 18923644.
  52. ^ Balardin, Joana Bisol; Miotto, Eliane Correa (December 2009). "A review of Constraint-Induced Therapy applied to aphasia rehabilitation in stroke patients". Dementia & Neuropsychologia. 3 (4): 275–282. doi:10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30400003. ISSN 1980-5764. PMC 5619412. PMID 29213640.
  53. ^ a b c d e f Berthier, Marcelo; et al. (2009). "Memantine and constraint-induced aphasia therapy in chronic poststroke aphasia". Annals of Neurology. 65 (5): 577–578. doi:10.1002/ana.21597. PMID 19475666. S2CID 31528532.
  54. ^ a b c d e f g Xavier, de Boissezon; Patrice Peran (2007). "Pharmacotherapy of aphasia: Myth or reality?". Brain and Language. 102 (1): 114–125. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.07.004. PMID 16982084. S2CID 38304960.
  55. ^ Berthier 2005.
  56. ^ a b c Margaret, Naeser; Paula Martin; Marjorie Nicholas; Errol Baker (2004). "Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca". Brain and Language. 93 (1): 95–105. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.004. PMID 15766771. S2CID 9348149.
  57. ^ Martin, Paula; Margaret Naeser; Michael Ho; Karl Doron; Jacquie Kurland (2009). "Overt Naming fMRI Pre- and Post- TMS: Two Nonfluent Aphasia Patients, with and without Improved Naming Post- TMS". Brain and Language. 111 (1): 20–35. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.007. PMC 2803355. PMID 19695692.
  58. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m Thompson CK, Shapiro LP (November 2005). "Treating agrammatic aphasia within a linguistic framework: Treatment of Underlying Forms". Aphasiology. 19 (10–11): 1021–1036. doi:10.1080/02687030544000227. PMC 1847567. PMID 17410280.
  59. ^ Heiss, W-D; Kessler, J; Thiel, A; Ghaemi, M; Karbe, H (1999). "Differential capacity of left and right hemispheric areas for compensation of poststroke". Ann Neurol. 45 (4): 430–438. doi:10.1002/1531-8249(199904)45:4<430::AID-ANA3>3.0.CO;2-P. PMID 10211466. S2CID 13377946.
  60. ^ Heiss, W-D; Thiel, A (2006). "A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of post-stroke aphasia". Brain and Language. 98 (1): 118–123. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.002. PMID 16564566. S2CID 22877982. Retrieved 2024-02-21.
  61. ^ Sanjit, Bhogal; Robert Teasell; Mark Speechley; Martin Albert (2003). "Intensity of Aphasia Therapy, Impact on Recovery * Aphasia Therapy Works!". Stroke. 34 (4): 987–993. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000062343.64383.D0. PMID 12649521.
  62. ^ Manasco, M. Hunter (2014). Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Jones & Bartlett Learning.
  63. ^ Thompson, Cynthia K. (2000). "Neuroplasticity: Evidence from Aphasia". Journal of Communication Disorders. 33 (4): 33 (4): 357–366. doi:10.1016/s0021-9924(00)00031-9. PMC 3086401. PMID 11001162.
  64. ^ Code, C; Hemsley, G; Herrmann, M (1999). "The emotional impact of aphasia". Semin Speech Lang. 20 (1): 19–31. doi:10.1055/s-2008-1064006. PMID 10100374. S2CID 260318407.

Sources edit

  • Berthier, Marcelo (2005). "Poststroke Aphasia". Drugs Aging. 22 (2): 163–182. doi:10.2165/00002512-200522020-00006. PMID 15733022. S2CID 22725166.

External links edit

expressive, aphasia, confused, with, receptive, aphasia, also, known, broca, aphasia, type, aphasia, characterized, partial, loss, ability, produce, language, spoken, manual, written, although, comprehension, generally, remains, intact, person, with, expressiv. Not to be confused with receptive aphasia Expressive aphasia also known as Broca s aphasia is a type of aphasia characterized by partial loss of the ability to produce language spoken manual 1 or written although comprehension generally remains intact 2 A person with expressive aphasia will exhibit effortful speech Speech generally includes important content words but leaves out function words that have more grammatical significance than physical meaning such as prepositions and articles 3 This is known as telegraphic speech The person s intended message may still be understood but their sentence will not be grammatically correct In very severe forms of expressive aphasia a person may only speak using single word utterances 4 5 Typically comprehension is mildly to moderately impaired in expressive aphasia due to difficulty understanding complex grammar 4 5 Expressive aphasiaOther namesBroca s aphasia non fluent aphasia agrammatic aphasiaBroca and Wernicke are two areas involved in language formationSpecialtyNeurology Psychiatry It is caused by acquired damage to the frontal regions of the brain such as Broca s area 6 Expressive aphasia contrasts with receptive aphasia in which patients are able to speak in grammatical sentences that lack semantic significance and generally also have trouble with comprehension 3 7 Expressive aphasia differs from dysarthria which is typified by a patient s inability to properly move the muscles of the tongue and mouth to produce speech Expressive aphasia also differs from apraxia of speech which is a motor disorder characterized by an inability to create and sequence motor plans for conscious speech 8 Contents 1 Signs and symptoms 1 1 Manual language and aphasia 1 2 Overlap with receptive aphasia 2 Causes 2 1 More common 2 2 Less common 2 2 1 Common causes 2 2 2 Uncommon causes 3 Diagnosis 4 Treatment 4 1 Augmentative and Alternative Communication 4 2 Melodic intonation therapy 4 3 Constraint induced therapy 4 4 Medication 4 5 Transcranial magnetic stimulation 4 6 Treatment of underlying forms 4 7 Mechanisms of recovery 5 Prognosis 6 History 7 See also 8 References 9 Sources 10 External linksSigns and symptoms editBroca s expressive aphasia is a type of non fluent aphasia in which an individual s speech is halting and effortful Misarticulations or distortions of consonants and vowels namely phonetic dissolution are common Individuals with expressive aphasia may only produce single words or words in groups of two or three 8 Long pauses between words are common and multi syllabic words may be produced one syllable at a time with pauses between each syllable 8 The prosody of a person with Broca s aphasia is compromised by shortened length of utterances and the presence of self repairs and disfluencies 9 Intonation and stress patterns are also deficient 10 For example in the following passage a patient with Broca s aphasia is trying to explain how he came to the hospital for dental surgery Yes ah Monday er Dad and Peter H his own name and Dad er hospital and ah Wednesday Wednesday nine o clock and oh Thursday ten o clock ah doctors two an doctors and er teeth yah 10 The speech of a person with expressive aphasia contains mostly content words such as nouns verbs and some adjectives However function words like conjunctions articles and prepositions are rarely used except for and which is prevalent in the speech of most patients with aphasia The omission of function words makes the person s speech agrammatic 8 A communication partner of a person with aphasia may say that the person s speech sounds telegraphic due to poor sentence construction and disjointed words 8 10 For example a person with expressive aphasia might say Smart university smart good good 9 Self monitoring is typically well preserved in patients with Broca s aphasia 8 They are usually aware of their communication deficits and are more prone to depression and outbursts from frustration than are patients with other forms of aphasia 11 In general word comprehension is preserved allowing patients to have functional receptive language skills 12 Individuals with Broca s aphasia understand most of the everyday conversation around them but higher level deficits in receptive language can occur 13 Because comprehension is substantially impaired for more complex sentences it is better to use simple language when speaking with an individual with expressive aphasia This is exemplified by the difficulty to understand phrases or sentences with unusual structure A typical patient with Broca s aphasia will misinterpret the man is bitten by the dog by switching the subject and object to the dog is bitten by the man 14 Typically people with expressive aphasia can understand speech and read better than they can produce speech and write 8 The person s writing will resemble their speech and will be effortful lacking cohesion and containing mostly content words 15 Letters will likely be formed clumsily and distorted and some may even be omitted Although listening and reading are generally intact subtle deficits in both reading and listening comprehension are almost always present during assessment of aphasia 8 Because Broca s area is anterior to the primary motor cortex which is responsible for movement of the face hands and arms a lesion affecting Broca s areas may also result in hemiparesis weakness of both limbs on the same side of the body or hemiplegia paralysis of both limbs on the same side of the body 8 The brain is wired contralaterally which means the limbs on right side of the body are controlled by the left hemisphere and vice versa 16 Therefore when Broca s area or surrounding areas in the left hemisphere are damaged hemiplegia or hemiparesis often occurs on the right side of the body in individuals with Broca s aphasia Severity of expressive aphasia varies among patients Some people may only have mild deficits and detecting problems with their language may be difficult In the most extreme cases patients may be able to produce only a single word Even in such cases over learned and rote learned speech patterns may be retained 17 for instance some patients can count from one to ten but cannot produce the same numbers in novel conversation Manual language and aphasia edit In deaf patients who use manual language such as American Sign Language damage to the left hemisphere of the brain leads to disruptions in their signing ability 1 Paraphasic errors similar to spoken language have been observed whereas in spoken language a phonemic substitution would occur e g tagle instead of table in ASL case studies errors in movement hand position and morphology have been noted Agrammatism or the lack of grammatical morphemes in sentence production has also been observed in lifelong users of ASL who have left hemisphere damage The lack of syntactic accuracy shows that the errors in signing are not due to damage to the motor cortex but rather area manifestation of the damage to the language producing area of the brain Similar symptoms have been seen in a patient with left hemisphere damage whose first language was British Sign Language 18 further showing that damage to the left hemisphere primarily hinders linguistic ability not motor ability In contrast patients who have damage to non linguistic areas on the left hemisphere have been shown to be fluent in signing but are unable to comprehend written language 1 Overlap with receptive aphasia edit In addition to difficulty expressing oneself individuals with expressive aphasia are also noted to commonly have trouble with comprehension in certain linguistic areas This agrammatism overlaps with receptive aphasia but can be seen in patients who have expressive aphasia without being diagnosed as having receptive aphasia The most well noted of these are object relative clauses object Wh questions and topicalized structures placing the topic at the beginning of the sentence 19 These three concepts all share phrasal movement which can cause words to lose their thematic roles when they change order in the sentence 19 This is often not an issue for people without agrammatic aphasias but many people with aphasia rely heavily on word order to understand roles that words play within the sentence 20 Causes editMore common edit Stroke or brain anoxia Brain tumor Brain trauma Less common edit Autoimmune disease Paraneoplastic syndrome Micrometastasis neurodegenerative disorders Certain infections e g Bartonella henselae 21 Metabolic disease e g hyperosmolar hyperglycemic state 22 Common causes edit The most common cause of expressive aphasia is stroke A stroke is caused by hypoperfusion lack of oxygen to an area of the brain which is commonly caused by thrombosis or embolism Some form of aphasia occurs in 34 to 38 of stroke patients 23 Expressive aphasia occurs in approximately 12 of new cases of aphasia caused by stroke 24 In most cases expressive aphasia is caused by a stroke in Broca s area or the surrounding vicinity Broca s area is in the lower part of the premotor cortex in the language dominant hemisphere and is responsible for planning motor speech movements However cases of expressive aphasia have been seen in patients with strokes in other areas of the brain 8 Patients with classic symptoms of expressive aphasia in general have more acute brain lesions whereas patients with larger widespread lesions exhibit a variety of symptoms that may be classified as global aphasia or left unclassified 23 Expressive aphasia can also be caused by trauma to the brain tumor cerebral hemorrhage 25 and by extradural abscess 26 Understanding lateralization of brain function is important for understanding which areas of the brain cause expressive aphasia when damaged In the past it has been believed that the area for language production differs between left and right handed individuals If this were true damage to the homologous region of Broca s area in the right hemisphere should cause aphasia in a left handed individual More recent studies have shown that even left handed individuals typically have language functions only in the left hemisphere However left handed individuals are more likely to have a dominance of language in the right hemisphere 6 Uncommon causes edit Less common causes of expressive aphasia include primary autoimmune phenomenon and autoimmune phenomenon that are secondary to cancer as a paraneoplastic syndrome have been listed as the primary hypothesis for several cases of aphasia especially when presenting with other psychiatric disturbances and focal neurological deficits Many case reports exist describing paraneoplastic aphasia and the reports that are specific tend to describe expressive aphasia 27 28 29 30 31 Although most cases attempt to exclude micro metastasis it is likely that some cases of paraneoplastic aphasia are actually extremely small metastasis to the vocal motor regions 30 Neurodegenerative disorders may present with aphasia Alzheimer s disease may present with either fluent aphasia or expressive aphasia There are case reports of Creutzfeldt Jakob disease presenting with expressive aphasia 32 33 Diagnosis editExpressive aphasia is classified as non fluent aphasia as opposed to fluent aphasia 5 Diagnosis is done on a case by case basis as lesions often affect the surrounding cortex and deficits are highly variable among patients with aphasia 34 A physician is typically the first person to recognize aphasia in a patient who is being treated for damage to the brain Routine processes for determining the presence and location of lesion in the brain include magnetic resonance imaging MRI and computed tomography CT scans The physician will complete a brief assessment of the patient s ability to understand and produce language For further diagnostic testing the physician will refer the patient to a speech language pathologist who will complete a comprehensive evaluation 35 In order to diagnose a patient with Broca s aphasia there are certain commonly used tests and procedures The Western Aphasia Battery WAB classifies individuals based on their scores on the subtests spontaneous speech auditory comprehension repetition and naming 8 The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination BDAE can inform users what specific type of aphasia they may have infer the location of lesion and assess current language abilities The Porch Index of Communication Ability PICA can predict potential recovery outcomes of the patients with aphasia Quality of life measurement is also an important assessment tool 36 Tests such as the Assessment for Living with Aphasia ALA and the Satisfaction with Life Scale SWLS allow for therapists to target skills that are important and meaningful for the individual 8 In addition to formal assessments patient and family interviews are valid and important sources of information The patient s previous hobbies interests personality and occupation are all factors that will not only impact therapy but may motivate them throughout the recovery process 35 Patient interviews and observations allow professionals to learn the priorities of the patient and family and determine what the patient hopes to regain in therapy Observations of the patient may also be beneficial to determine where to begin treatment The current behaviors and interactions of the patient will provide the therapist with more insight about the client and their individual needs 8 Other information about the patient can be retrieved from medical records patient referrals from physicians and the nursing staff 35 In non speaking patients who use manual languages diagnosis is often based on interviews from the patient s acquaintances noting the differences in sign production pre and post damage to the brain 18 Many of these patients will also begin to rely on non linguistic gestures to communicate rather than signing since their language production is hindered 37 Treatment editCurrently there is no standard treatment for expressive aphasia Most aphasia treatment is individualized based on a patient s condition and needs as assessed by a speech language pathologist Patients go through a period of spontaneous recovery following brain injury in which they regain a great deal of language function 38 In the months following injury or stroke most patients receive traditional treatment for a few hours per day Among other exercises patients practice the repetition of words and phrases Mechanisms are also taught in traditional treatment to compensate for lost language function such as drawing and using phrases that are easier to pronounce 39 Emphasis is placed on establishing a basis for communication with family and caregivers in everyday life Treatment is individualized based on the patient s own priorities along with the family s input 8 40 A patient may have the option of individual or group treatment Although less common group treatment has been shown to have advantageous outcomes Some types of group treatments include family counseling maintenance groups support groups and treatment groups 41 Augmentative and Alternative Communication edit This section may be in need of reorganization to comply with Wikipedia s layout guidelines Please help by editing the article to make improvements to the overall structure February 2024 Learn how and when to remove this message Augmentative and Alternative Communication AAC refers to a set of tools and strategies that support or replace verbal communication for individuals with communication disorders such as Broca s aphasia or other conditions that affect speech and language abilities AAC is designed to enhance communication and may be used as a temporary or permanent solution depending on the individual s needs Here are some key aspects of AAC Communication Aids Low Tech AAC This includes simple non electronic communication aids such as communication boards picture books or communication charts Users can point to or select symbols or pictures to convey their messages 1 High Tech AAC Involves electronic devices such as speech generating devices SGDs or tablet based communication apps These devices use synthesized speech or recorded messages to facilitate communication Users can select words phrases or symbols on a screen to express themselves Symbols and Representations Symbols used in AAC can vary and may include pictures icons words or a combination of these Symbols are chosen based on the individual s cognitive and language abilities Types of AAC Systems Unaided AAC Relies on the user s body to convey messages without external tools such as using gestures facial expressions or sign language Aided AAC Involves external tools or devices such as communication boards speech generating devices or computer based systems Vocabulary and Language Systems Core Vocabulary Focuses on essential words that are frequently used across various contexts Core vocabulary systems aim to provide users with a versatile set of words to express a wide range of messages Fringe Vocabulary Includes specific words related to an individual s unique needs interests or daily activities Fringe vocabulary supplements core vocabulary to make communication more personalized 17 Customization and Individualization AAC systems are highly customizable to meet the unique needs of each user Therapists work with individuals and their families to tailor the system to the user s abilities preferences and communication goals Training and Support Users of AAC systems as well as their caregivers and support networks receive training to effectively use the communication tools Training may involve learning how to navigate electronic devices program personalized messages or understand the meaning of symbols Integration with Therapy AAC is often integrated into speech and language therapy sessions Therapists use AAC tools to facilitate communication practice and help individuals with communication disorders improve their language skills 7 Social and Emotional Aspects AAC not only addresses the functional aspects of communication but also considers the social and emotional dimensions It plays a crucial role in helping individuals with communication disorders participate more fully in social interactions and express their thoughts and feelings AAC is a dynamic and evolving field and advancements in technology continue to enhance the range and effectiveness of communication tools available for individuals with speech and language challenges The selection of AAC strategies depends on factors such as the individual s abilities preferences and the specific nature of their communication disorder Melodic intonation therapy edit Melodic intonation therapy was inspired by the observation that individuals with non fluent aphasia sometimes can sing words or phrases that they normally cannot speak Melodic Intonation Therapy was begun as an attempt to use the intact melodic prosodic processing skills of the right hemisphere in those with aphasia to help cue retrieval words and expressive language 42 It is believed that this is because singing capabilities are stored in the right hemisphere of the brain which is likely to remain unaffected after a stroke in the left hemisphere 43 However recent evidence demonstrates that the capability of individuals with aphasia to sing entire pieces of text may actually result from rhythmic features and the familiarity with the lyrics 44 The goal of Melodic Intonation Therapy is to utilize singing to access the language capable regions in the right hemisphere and use these regions to compensate for lost function in the left hemisphere The natural musical component of speech was used to engage the patients ability to produce phrases A clinical study revealed that singing and rhythmic speech may be similarly effective in the treatment of non fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech 45 Moreover evidence from randomized controlled trials is still needed to confirm that Melodic Intonation Therapy is suitable to improve propositional utterances and speech intelligibility in individuals with chronic non fluent aphasia and apraxia of speech 46 47 Melodic Intonation Therapy appears to work particularly well in patients who have had a unilateral left hemisphere stroke show poor articulation are non fluent or have severely restricted speech output have moderately preserved auditory comprehension and show good motivation MIT therapy on average lasts for 1 5 hours per day for five days per week At the lowest level of therapy simple words and phrases such as water and I love you are broken down into a series of high and low pitch syllables With increased treatment longer phrases are taught and less support is provided by the therapist Patients are taught to say phrases using the natural melodic component of speaking and continuous voicing is emphasized The patient is also instructed to use the left hand to tap the syllables of the phrase while the phrases are spoken Tapping is assumed to trigger the rhythmic component of speaking to utilize the right hemisphere 43 FMRI studies have shown that Melodic Intonation Therapy MIT uses both sides of the brain to recover lost function as opposed to traditional therapies that utilize only the left hemisphere In MIT individuals with small lesions in the left hemisphere seem to recover by activation of the left hemisphere perilesional cortex Meanwhile individuals with larger left hemisphere lesions show a recruitment of the use of language capable regions in the right hemisphere 43 The interpretation of these results is still a matter of debate For example it remains unclear whether changes in neural activity in the right hemisphere result from singing or from the intensive use of common phrases such as thank you how are you or I am fine This type of phrases falls into the category of formulaic language and is known to be supported by neural networks of the intact right hemisphere 48 A pilot study reported positive results when comparing the efficacy of a modified form of MIT to no treatment in people with nonfluent aphasia with damage to their left brain A randomized controlled trial was conducted and the study reported benefits of utilizing modified MIT treatment early in the recovery phase for people with nonfluent aphasia 49 Melodic Intonation Therapy is used by music therapists board certified professionals that use music as a therapeutic tool to effect certain non musical outcomes in their patients Speech language pathologists can also use this therapy for individuals who have had a left hemisphere stroke and non fluent aphasias such as Broca s or even apraxia of speech Further information Music therapy for non fluent aphasia Constraint induced therapy edit Constraint induced aphasia therapy CIAT is based on similar principles as constraint induced movement therapy developed by Dr Edward Taub at the University of Alabama at Birmingham 39 50 Constraint induced movement therapy is based on the idea that a person with an impairment physical or communicative develops a learned nonuse by compensating for the lost function with other means such as using an unaffected limb by a paralyzed individual or drawing by a patient with aphasia 51 In constraint induced movement therapy the alternative limb is constrained with a glove or sling and the patient is forced to use the affected limb In constraint induced aphasia therapy the interaction is guided by communicative need in a language game context picture cards barriers making it impossible to see other players cards and other materials so that patients are encouraged constrained to use the remaining verbal abilities to succeed in the communication game 50 Two important principles of constraint induced aphasia therapy are that treatment is very intense with sessions lasting for up to 6 hours over the course of 10 days and that language is used in a communication context in which it is closely linked to nonverbal actions 39 50 These principles are motivated by neuroscience insights about learning at the level of nerve cells synaptic plasticity and the coupling between cortical systems for language and action in the human brain 51 Constraint induced therapy contrasts sharply with traditional therapy by the strong belief that mechanisms to compensate for lost language function such as gesturing or writing should not be used unless absolutely necessary even in everyday life 39 It is believed that CIAT works by the mechanism of increased neuroplasticity By constraining an individual to use only speech it is believed that the brain is more likely to reestablish old neural pathways and recruit new neural pathways to compensate for lost function 52 The strongest results of CIAT have been seen in patients with chronic aphasia lasting over 6 months Studies of CIAT have confirmed that further improvement is possible even after a patient has reached a plateau period of recovery 39 50 It has also been proven that the benefits of CIAT are retained long term However improvements only seem to be made while a patient is undergoing intense therapy 39 Recent work has investigated combining constraint induced aphasia therapy with drug treatment which led to an amplification of therapy benefits 53 Medication edit In addition to active speech therapy pharmaceuticals have also been considered as a useful treatment for expressive aphasia This area of study is relatively new and much research continues to be conducted The following drugs have been suggested for use in treating aphasia and their efficacy has been studied in control studies Bromocriptine acts on Catecholamine Systems 54 Piracetam mechanism not fully understood but most likely interacts with cholinergic and glutamatergic receptors among others 54 Cholinergic drugs Donepezil Aniracetam Bifemelane acts on acetylcholine systems 54 Dopaminergic psychostimulants Dexamphetamine Methylphenidate 54 The most effect has been shown by piracetam and amphetamine which may increase cerebral plasticity and result in an increased capability to improve language function It has been seen that piracetam is most effective when treatment is begun immediately following stroke When used in chronic cases it has been much less efficient 55 Bromocriptine has been shown by some studies to increase verbal fluency and word retrieval with therapy than with just therapy alone 54 Furthermore its use seems to be restricted to non fluent aphasia 53 Donepezil has shown a potential for helping chronic aphasia 53 No study has established irrefutable evidence that any drug is an effective treatment for aphasia therapy 54 Furthermore no study has shown any drug to be specific for language recovery 53 Comparison between the recovery of language function and other motor function using any drug has shown that improvement is due to a global increase plasticity of neural networks 54 Transcranial magnetic stimulation edit In transcranial magnetic stimulation TMS magnetic fields are used to create electrical currents in specified cortical regions The procedure is a painless and noninvasive method of stimulating the cortex TMS works by suppressing the inhibition process in certain areas of the brain 56 By suppressing the inhibition of neurons by external factors the targeted area of the brain may be reactivated and thereby recruited to compensate for lost function Research has shown that patients can demonstrate increased object naming ability with regular transcranial magnetic stimulation than patients not receiving TMS 56 Furthermore research suggests this improvement is sustained upon the completion of TMS therapy 56 However some patients fail to show any significant improvement from TMS which indicates the need for further research of this treatment 57 Treatment of underlying forms edit Described as the linguistic approach to the treatment of expressive aphasia treatment begins by emphasizing and educating patients on the thematic roles of words within sentences 58 Sentences that are usually problematic will be reworded into active voiced declarative phrasings of their non canonical counterparts 58 The simpler sentence phrasings are then transformed into variations that are more difficult to interpret For example many individuals who have expressive aphasia struggle with Wh sentences What and who questions are problematic sentences that this treatment method attempts to improve and they are also two interrogative particles that are strongly related to each other because they reorder arguments from the declarative counterparts 58 For instance therapists have used sentences like Who is the boy helping and What is the boy fixing because both verbs are transitive they require two arguments in the form of a subject and a direct object but not necessarily an indirect object 58 In addition certain question particles are linked together based on how the reworded sentence is formed Training who sentences increased the generalizations of non trained who sentences as well as untrained what sentences and vice versa 58 Likewise where and when question types are very closely linked What and who questions alter placement of arguments and where and when sentences move adjunct phrases 58 Training is in the style of The man parked the car in the driveway What did the man park in the driveway 58 Sentence training goes on in this manner for more domains such as clefts and sentence voice 58 Results Patients use of sentence types used in the TUF treatment will improve subjects will generalize sentences of similar category to those used for treatment in TUF and results are applied to real world conversations with others 58 Generalization of sentence types used can be improved when the treatment progresses in the order of more complex sentences to more elementary sentences Treatment has been shown to affect on line real time processing of trained sentences and these results can be tracked using fMRI mappings 58 Training of Wh sentences has led improvements in three main areas of discourse for aphasics increased average length of utterances higher proportions of grammatical sentences and larger ratios of numbers of verbs to nouns produced 58 Patients also showed improvements in verb argument structure productions and assigned thematic roles to words in utterances with more accuracy 58 In terms of on line sentence processing patients having undergone this treatment discriminate between anomalous and non anomalous sentences with more accuracy than control groups and are closer to levels of normalcy than patients not having participated in this treatment 58 Mechanisms of recovery edit Mechanisms for recovery differ from patient to patient Some mechanisms for recovery occur spontaneously after damage to the brain whereas others are caused by the effects of language therapy 53 FMRI studies have shown that recovery can be partially attributed to the activation of tissue around the damaged area and the recruitment of new neurons in these areas to compensate for the lost function Recovery may also be caused in very acute lesions by a return of blood flow and function to damaged tissue that has not died around an injured area 53 It has been stated by some researchers that the recruitment and recovery of neurons in the left hemisphere opposed to the recruitment of similar neurons in the right hemisphere is superior for long term recovery and continued rehabilitation 59 It is thought that because the right hemisphere is not intended for full language function using the right hemisphere as a mechanism of recovery is effectively a dead end and can lead only to partial recovery 60 There is evidence to support that among all types of therapies one of the most important factors and best predictors for a successful outcome is the intensity of the therapy By comparing the length and intensity of various methods of therapies it was proven that intensity is a better predictor of recovery than the method of therapy used 61 Prognosis editIn most individuals with expressive aphasia the majority of recovery is seen within the first year following a stroke or injury The majority of this improvement is seen in the first four weeks in therapy following a stroke and slows thereafter 23 However this timeline will vary depending upon the type of stroke experienced by the patient Patients who experienced an ischemic stroke may recover in the days and weeks following the stroke and then experience a plateau and gradual slowing of recovery On the contrary patients who experienced a hemorrhagic stroke experience a slower recovery in the first 4 8 weeks followed by a faster recovery which eventually stabilizes 62 Numerous factors impact the recovery process and outcomes Site and extent of lesion greatly impacts recovery Other factors that may affect prognosis are age education gender and motivation 63 Occupation handedness personality and emotional state may also be associated with recovery outcomes 8 Studies have also found that prognosis of expressive aphasia correlates strongly with the initial severity of impairment 24 However it has been seen that continued recovery is possible years after a stroke with effective treatment 39 Timing and intensity of treatment is another factor that impacts outcomes Research suggests that even in later stages of recovery intervention is effective at improving function as well as preventing loss of function 38 Unlike receptive aphasia patients with expressive aphasia are aware of their errors in language production This may further motivate a person with expressive aphasia to progress in treatment which would affect treatment outcomes 23 On the other hand awareness of impairment may lead to higher levels of frustration depression anxiety or social withdrawal which have been proven to negatively affect a person s chance of recovery 64 History editExpressive aphasia was first identified by the French neurologist Paul Broca By examining the brains of deceased individuals having acquired expressive aphasia in life he concluded that language ability is localized in the ventroposterior region of the frontal lobe One of the most important aspects of Paul Broca s discovery was the observation that the loss of proper speech in expressive aphasia is due to the brain s loss of ability to produce language as opposed to the mouth s loss of ability to produce words 6 The discoveries of Paul Broca were made during the same period of time as the German Neurologist Carl Wernicke who was also studying brains of aphasiacs post mortem and identified the region now known as Wernicke s area Discoveries of both men contributed to the concept of localization which states that specific brain functions are all localized to a specific area of the brain While both men made significant contributions to the field of aphasia it was Carl Wernicke who realized the difference between patients with aphasia that could not produce language and those that could not comprehend language the essential difference between expressive and receptive aphasia 6 See also editBroca s area Transcortical sensory aphasia Wernicke s aphasia Word saladReferences edit a b c d Hicoka Gregory 1 April 1998 The neural organization of language evidence from sign language aphasia Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 4 129 136 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 98 01154 1 PMID 21227109 S2CID 7018568 Broca s Aphasia National Aphasia Association National Aphasia Association Retrieved 2017 04 11 a b Fromkin Victoria Rodman Robert Hyams Nina 2014 An Introduction to Language Boston MA Wadsworth Cengage Learning pp 464 465 ISBN 978 1133310686 a b ASHA org a b c Common Classifications of Aphasia American Speech Language Hearing Association Archived from the original on 2017 05 07 Retrieved 2024 02 21 a b c d Purves D 2008 Neuroscience fourth ed Sinauer Associates Inc ISBN 978 0 87893 742 4 a b Nakai Y Jeong JW Brown EC Rothermel R Kojima K Kambara T Shah A Mittal S Sood S Asano E 2017 Three and four dimensional mapping of speech and language in patients with epilepsy Brain 140 5 1351 1370 doi 10 1093 brain awx051 PMC 5405238 PMID 28334963 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Brookshire Robert 2007 Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders St Louis MO Mosby ISBN 978 0323045315 a b Manasco H 2014 The Aphasias In Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders Vol 1 p 91 Burlington MA Jones amp Bartlett Learning a b c Goodglass H N Geschwind 1976 Language disorders In E Carterette and M P Friedman ed Handbook of Perception Language and Speech Vol VII New York Academic Press Chapey Roberta 2008 Language Intervention Strategies in Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders Philadelphia PA Lippincott Williams amp Wilkins p 8 ISBN 978 0 7817 6981 5 verification needed Manasco 2014 Katey Birtcher et al eds INtroduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders Pennsylvania USA William Brottmiller pp 80 81 ISBN 9781449652449 Manasco M Hunter 2013 02 06 Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders Jones amp Bartlett Learning p 80 ISBN 9781449652449 Neurology of Syntax Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 1 Archived from the original on 2004 05 18 Retrieved 2006 05 10 Chapey Roberta 2008 Language Intervention Strategies in Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders Philadelphia PA Lippincott Williams amp Wilkins p 8 ISBN 978 0 7817 6981 5 Teasell Robert 2003 Stroke recovery and rehabilitation Stroke 34 2 365 366 doi 10 1161 01 str 0000054630 33395 e2 PMID 12574538 a b Specific Syndromes The Nonfluent Aphasias Neuropathologies of Language and Cognition Archived from the original on 2006 04 27 Retrieved 2006 05 10 a b Marshall Jane 15 June 2004 Aphasia in a user of British Sign Language Dissociation between sign and gesture Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 5 537 554 doi 10 1080 02643290342000249 PMID 21038221 S2CID 27849117 a b Friedmann Naama Gvion Aviah Novogrodsky Rama 2006 Syntactic Movement in Agrammatism and S SLI Two Different Impairments In Adriana Belletti et al eds Language Acquisition and Development Proceedings of GALA2005 PDF Newcastle UK Cambridge Scholars Press pp 197 210 ISBN 9781847180285 OCLC 133524617 Archived from the original PDF on 2014 12 11 Retrieved 2024 02 21 Bates Elizabeth A Friederici Angela D Wulfeck Beverly B Juarez Larry A 1988 03 01 On the preservation of word order in aphasia Cross linguistic evidence Brain and Language 33 2 323 364 doi 10 1016 0093 934X 88 90072 7 ISSN 0093 934X PMID 3359173 S2CID 23236428 Marienfeld Carla B DiCapua Daniel B Sze Gordon K Goldstein Jonathan M June 2010 Expressive Aphasia as a Presentation of Encephalitis with Bartonella henselae Infection in an Immunocompetent Adult The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine 83 2 67 71 ISSN 0044 0086 PMC 2892771 PMID 20589186 Lee Ji Hyun Kim Ye An Moon Joon Ho Min Se Hee Song Young Shin Choi Sung Hee November 2016 Expressive aphasia as the manifestation of hyperglycemic crisis in type 2 diabetes The Korean Journal of Internal Medicine 31 6 1187 1190 doi 10 3904 kjim 2014 379 ISSN 1226 3303 PMC 5094916 PMID 26968185 a b c d Bakheit AMO Shaw S Carrington S Griffiths S 2007 The rate and extent of improvement with therapy from the different types of aphasia in the first year of stroke Integumentary Rehabilitation 21 10 941 949 doi 10 1177 0269215507078452 PMID 17981853 S2CID 25995618 a b Pedersen PM Vinter K Olsen TS 2004 Aphasia after stroke Type severity and prognosis The Copenhagen aphasia study Cerebrovascular Diseases 17 1 35 43 doi 10 1159 000073896 PMID 14530636 S2CID 11754713 Orzeren A F Koc M Demirkiran A Sonmezler 2006 Global aphasia due to left thalamic hemorrhage Neurology India 54 4 415 417 doi 10 4103 0028 3886 28118 PMID 17114855 S2CID 23023504 Commondoor R Eisenhut M Fowler C Kirollos R W amp Nathwani N 2009 Transient Broca s Aphasia as Feature of an Extradural Abscess Pediatric Neurology 40 1 50 53 doi 10 1016 j pediatrneurol 2008 06 018 PMID 19068255 McKeon Andrew April 2013 Paraneoplastic and Other Autoimmune Disorders of the Central Nervous System The Neurohospitalist 3 2 53 64 doi 10 1177 1941874412453339 ISSN 1941 8744 PMC 3726118 PMID 23983888 Yeung Darwin F Hsu Rose 2014 08 05 Expressive aphasia in a patient with chronic myelomonocytic leukemia SpringerPlus 3 406 doi 10 1186 2193 1801 3 406 ISSN 2193 1801 PMC 4130962 PMID 25126489 Will A Akalin Murat 2012 04 24 Paraneoplastic Limbic Encephalitis with NMDA Receptor NR1 Antibodies in Breast Cancer S08 007 Neurology 78 1 Supplement S08 007 doi 10 1212 WNL 78 1 MeetingAbstracts S08 007 ISSN 0028 3878 a b Darnell Robert Darnell Robert B Posner Jerome B 2011 08 22 Paraneoplastic Syndromes Oxford University Press USA ISBN 9780199772735 Lancaster Eric April 2015 Continuum The Paraneoplastic Disorders Continuum Minneapolis Minn 21 2 0 452 475 doi 10 1212 01 CON 0000464180 89580 88 ISSN 1080 2371 PMC 4443809 PMID 25837906 Mahboob Hafiz B Kaokaf Kazi H Gonda Jeremy M 2018 02 14 Creutzfeldt Jakob Disease Presenting as Expressive Aphasia and Nonconvulsive Status Epilepticus Case Reports in Critical Care 2018 5053175 doi 10 1155 2018 5053175 ISSN 2090 6420 PMC 5832162 PMID 29666711 Primary Progressive Aphasia National Aphasia Association National Aphasia Association Retrieved 2018 11 26 Aphasia FAQ s National Aphasia Association November 7 2017 a b c Aphasia National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders March 6 2017 Retrieved 2024 02 21 McCaffrey Patrick 1999 The Diagnosis of Aphasia The Neuroscience on the Web Series California State University Chico Archived from the original on 2007 06 15 Retrieved 2017 11 08 Hogrefe Katharina Ziegler Wolfram Wiesmayer Susanne Weidinger Nicole Goldenberg Georg 2013 09 01 The actual and potential use of gestures for communication in aphasia Aphasiology 27 9 1070 1089 doi 10 1080 02687038 2013 803515 ISSN 0268 7038 S2CID 145152352 a b Raymer Anastasia February 2008 Translational Research in Aphasia from Neuroscience to Neurorehabilitation Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 51 1 259 275 doi 10 1044 1092 4388 2008 020 PMID 18230850 a b c d e f g Meinzer Marcus Thomas Elbert Daniela Djundja Edward Taub 2007 Extending the Constraint Induced Movement Therapy CIMT approach to cognitive functions Constraint Induced Aphasia Therapy CIAT of chronic aphasia NeuroRehabilitation 22 4 311 318 doi 10 3233 NRE 2007 22409 PMID 17971622 Aphasia Treatment ASHA Practice Portal American Speech Language Hearing Association Retrieved August 7 2017 Fama Mackenzie E Baron Christine R Hatfield Brooke Turkeltaub Peter E August 2016 Group Therapy as a Social Context for Aphasia Recovery A pilot observational study in an acute rehabilitation hospital Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation 23 4 276 283 doi 10 1080 10749357 2016 1155277 ISSN 1074 9357 PMC 4949973 PMID 27077989 Wilson Sarah J 2006 A Case Study of the Efficacy of Melodic Intonation Therapy PDF Music Perception 24 1 23 36 doi 10 1525 mp 2006 24 1 23 ISSN 0730 7829 a b c Schlaug Gottfried Sarah Marchina Andrea Norton 2008 From Singing to Speaking Why singing may lead to recovery of expressive language function in patients with Broca s Aphasia Music Perception 25 4 315 319 doi 10 1525 mp 2008 25 4 315 PMC 3010734 PMID 21197418 Stahl Benjamin Kotz Sonja A Henseler Ilona Turner Robert Geyer Stefan 2011 Rhythm in disguise why singing may not hold the key to recovery from aphasia Brain 134 10 3083 3093 doi 10 1093 brain awr240 ISSN 0006 8950 PMC 3187543 PMID 21948939 Stahl Benjamin Henseler Ilona Turner Robert Geyer Stefan Kotz Sonja A 2013 How to engage the right brain hemisphere in aphasics without even singing Evidence for two paths of speech recovery Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7 35 1 12 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2013 00035 ISSN 1662 5161 PMC 3583105 PMID 23450277 van der Meulen I van de Sandt Koenderman M W Heijenbrok Kal M H Visch Brink E G Ribbers G M 2014 The efficacy and timing of Melodic Intonation Therapy in subacute aphasia Neurorehabil Neural Repair 28 6 536 544 doi 10 1177 1545968313517753 PMID 24449708 S2CID 6495987 Zumbansen Anna Peretz Isabelle Hebert Sylvie 2014 Melodic Intonation Therapy Back to Basics for Future Research Frontiers in Neurology 5 7 7 doi 10 3389 fneur 2014 00007 PMC 3904283 PMID 24478754 Stahl Benjamin Kotz Sonja A 2013 Facing the music Three issues in current research on singing and aphasia Frontiers in Psychology 5 1033 1 4 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2014 01033 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 4172097 PMID 25295017 Conklyn D Novak E Boissy A Bethoux F Chemali K 2012 The Effects of Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy on Nonfluent Aphasia A Pilot Study Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 55 5 1463 1471 doi 10 1044 1092 4388 2012 11 0105 PMID 22411278 a b c d Pulvermuller Friedemann et al 2001 Constraint Induced Therapy of Chronic Aphasia following Stroke Stroke 32 7 1621 1626 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 492 3416 doi 10 1161 01 STR 32 7 1621 PMID 11441210 S2CID 673662 a b Pulvermuller Friedemann Marcelo Berthier 2008 Aphasia therapy on a neuroscience basis Aphasiology 22 6 563 599 doi 10 1080 02687030701612213 PMC 2557073 PMID 18923644 Balardin Joana Bisol Miotto Eliane Correa December 2009 A review of Constraint Induced Therapy applied to aphasia rehabilitation in stroke patients Dementia amp Neuropsychologia 3 4 275 282 doi 10 1590 S1980 57642009DN30400003 ISSN 1980 5764 PMC 5619412 PMID 29213640 a b c d e f Berthier Marcelo et al 2009 Memantine and constraint induced aphasia therapy in chronic poststroke aphasia Annals of Neurology 65 5 577 578 doi 10 1002 ana 21597 PMID 19475666 S2CID 31528532 a b c d e f g Xavier de Boissezon Patrice Peran 2007 Pharmacotherapy of aphasia Myth or reality Brain and Language 102 1 114 125 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2006 07 004 PMID 16982084 S2CID 38304960 Berthier 2005 a b c Margaret Naeser Paula Martin Marjorie Nicholas Errol Baker 2004 Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca Brain and Language 93 1 95 105 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2004 08 004 PMID 15766771 S2CID 9348149 Martin Paula Margaret Naeser Michael Ho Karl Doron Jacquie Kurland 2009 Overt Naming fMRI Pre and Post TMS Two Nonfluent Aphasia Patients with and without Improved Naming Post TMS Brain and Language 111 1 20 35 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2009 07 007 PMC 2803355 PMID 19695692 a b c d e f g h i j k l m Thompson CK Shapiro LP November 2005 Treating agrammatic aphasia within a linguistic framework Treatment of Underlying Forms Aphasiology 19 10 11 1021 1036 doi 10 1080 02687030544000227 PMC 1847567 PMID 17410280 Heiss W D Kessler J Thiel A Ghaemi M Karbe H 1999 Differential capacity of left and right hemispheric areas for compensation of poststroke Ann Neurol 45 4 430 438 doi 10 1002 1531 8249 199904 45 4 lt 430 AID ANA3 gt 3 0 CO 2 P PMID 10211466 S2CID 13377946 Heiss W D Thiel A 2006 A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of post stroke aphasia Brain and Language 98 1 118 123 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2006 02 002 PMID 16564566 S2CID 22877982 Retrieved 2024 02 21 Sanjit Bhogal Robert Teasell Mark Speechley Martin Albert 2003 Intensity of Aphasia Therapy Impact on Recovery Aphasia Therapy Works Stroke 34 4 987 993 doi 10 1161 01 STR 0000062343 64383 D0 PMID 12649521 Manasco M Hunter 2014 Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders Jones amp Bartlett Learning Thompson Cynthia K 2000 Neuroplasticity Evidence from Aphasia Journal of Communication Disorders 33 4 33 4 357 366 doi 10 1016 s0021 9924 00 00031 9 PMC 3086401 PMID 11001162 Code C Hemsley G Herrmann M 1999 The emotional impact of aphasia Semin Speech Lang 20 1 19 31 doi 10 1055 s 2008 1064006 PMID 10100374 S2CID 260318407 ASHA Glossary Broca s Aphasia n d Retrieved November 18 2015 from http www asha org Glossary Brocas Aphasia Sources editBerthier Marcelo 2005 Poststroke Aphasia Drugs Aging 22 2 163 182 doi 10 2165 00002512 200522020 00006 PMID 15733022 S2CID 22725166 External links editNational Aphasia Association Aphasia Center of California in Oakland CA U S video of person with Broca s Aphasia Broca s aphasia Discovery of the area of the brain governing articulated language analysis of Broca s 1861 article on BibNum click a telecharger for English version Appendix Common Classifications of Aphasia n d Retrieved from http www asha org Practice Portal Clinical Topics Aphasia Common Classifications of Aphasia Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Expressive aphasia amp oldid 1219383341, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.