fbpx
Wikipedia

Lightness

Lightness is a visual perception of the luminance of an object. It is often judged relative to a similarly lit object. In colorimetry and color appearance models, lightness is a prediction of how an illuminated color will appear to a standard observer. While luminance is a linear measurement of light, lightness is a linear prediction of the human perception of that light.

Three hues in the Munsell color model. Each color differs in value from top to bottom in equal perception steps. The right column undergoes a dramatic change in perceived color.

This distinction is meaningful because human vision's lightness perception is non-linear relative to light. Doubling the quantity of light does not result in a doubling in perceived lightness, only a modest increase.

The symbol for perceptual lightness is usually either as used in CIECAM02 or as used in CIELAB and CIELUV. ("Lstar") is not to be confused with as used for luminance. In some color ordering systems such as Munsell, Lightness is referenced as value.

Chiaroscuro and Tenebrism both take advantage of dramatic contrasts of value to heighten drama in art. Artists may also employ shading, subtle manipulation of value.

Lightness in different colorspaces edit

 
Fig 2a. The sRGB gamut mapped in CIELAB space. Notice that the lines pointing to the red, green, and blue primaries are not evenly spaced by hue angle, and are of unequal length. The primaries also have different L* values.
 
Fig 2b. The Adobe RGB gamut mapped in CIELAB space. Also notice that these two RGB spaces have different gamuts, and thus will have different HSL and HSV representations.

In some colorspaces or color systems such as Munsell, HCL, and CIELAB, the lightness (value) achromatically constrains the maximum and minimum limits, and operates independently of the hue and chroma. For example Munsell value 0 is pure black, and value 10 is pure white. Colors with a discernible hue must therefore have values in between these extremes.

In a subtractive color model (e.g. paint, dye, or ink) lightness changes to a color through various tints, shades, or tones can be achieved by adding white, black, or grey respectively. This also reduces saturation.

In HSL and HSV, the displayed luminance is relative to the hue and chroma for a given lightness value, in other words the selected lightness value does not predict the actual displayed luminance nor the perception thereof. Both systems use coordinate triples, where many triples can map onto the same color.

In HSV, all triples with value 0 are pure black. If the hue and saturation are held constant, then increasing the value increases the luminance, such that a value of 1 is the lightest color with the given hue and saturation. HSL is similar, except that all triples with lightness 1 are pure white. In both models, all pure saturated colors indicate the same lightness or value, but this does not relate to the displayed luminance which is determined by the hue. I.e. yellow is higher luminance than blue, even if the lightness value is set at a given number.

While HSL, HSV, and similar spaces serve well enough to choose or adjust a single color, they are not perceptually uniform. They trade off accuracy for computational simplicity, as they were created in an era where computer technology was restricted in performance.[1]

If we take an image and extract the hue, saturation, and lightness or value components for a given color space, we will see that they may differ substantially from a different color space or model. For example, examine the following images of a fire breather (fig. 1). The original is in the sRGB color space. CIELAB   is a perceptually uniform lightness prediction that is derived from luminance  , but discards the   and  , of the CIE XYZ color space. Notice this appears similar in perceived lightness to the original color image. Luma   is a gamma-encoded luminance component of some video encoding systems such as   and  . It is roughly similar, but differs at high chroma, deviating most from an achromatic signal such as linear luminance   or non-linear lightness  . HSL   and HSV   are neither perceptually uniform, nor uniform as to luminance.

 
Fig. 1a. Color photograph (sRGB color space).
 
Fig. 1b. CIELAB L* (further transformed back to sRGB for consistent display).
 
Fig. 1c. Rec. 601 luma Y.
 
Fig. 1d. Component average: "intensity" I.
 
Fig. 1e. HSV value V.
 
Fig. 1f. HSL lightness L.

Relationship to value and relative luminance edit

The Munsell value has long been used as a perceptually uniform lightness scale. A question of interest is the relationship between the Munsell value scale and the relative luminance. Aware of the Weber–Fechner law, Albert Munsell remarked "Should we use a logarithmic curve or curve of squares?"[2] Neither option turned out to be quite correct; scientists eventually converged on a roughly cube-root curve, consistent with the Stevens's power law for brightness perception, reflecting the fact that lightness is proportional to the number of nerve impulses per nerve fiber per unit time.[3] The remainder of this section is a chronology of lightness models, leading to CIECAM02.

Note. – Munsell's V runs from 0 to 10, while Y typically runs from 0 to 100 (often interpreted as a percentage). Typically, the relative luminance is normalized so that the "reference white" (say, magnesium oxide) has a tristimulus value of Y = 100. Since the reflectance of magnesium oxide (MgO) relative to the perfect reflecting diffuser is 97.5%, V = 10 corresponds to Y = 100/97.5% ≈ 102.6 if MgO is used as the reference.[4]

 
Observe that the lightness is 50% for a relative luminance of around 18% relative to the reference white.

1920 edit

Irwin Priest, Kasson Gibson, and Harry McNicholas provide a basic estimate of the Munsell value (with Y running from 0 to 1 in this case):[5]

 

1933 edit

Alexander Munsell, Louise Sloan, and Isaac Godlove launch a study on the Munsell neutral value scale, considering several proposals relating the relative luminance to the Munsell value, and suggest:[6][7]

 

1943 edit

Sidney Newhall, Dorothy Nickerson, and Deane Judd prepare a report for the Optical Society of America (OSA) on Munsell renotation. They suggest a quintic parabola (relating the reflectance in terms of the value):[8]

 

1943 edit

Using Table II of the OSA report, Parry Moon and Domina Spencer express the value in terms of the relative luminance:[9]

 

1944 edit

Jason Saunderson and B.I. Milner introduce a subtractive constant in the previous expression, for a better fit to the Munsell value.[10] Later, Dorothea Jameson and Leo Hurvich claim that this corrects for simultaneous contrast effects.[11][12]

 

1955 edit

Ladd and Pinney of Eastman Kodak are interested in the Munsell value as a perceptually uniform lightness scale for use in television. After considering one logarithmic and five power-law functions (per Stevens' power law), they relate value to reflectance by raising the reflectance to the power of 0.352:[13]

 

Realizing this is quite close to the cube root, they simplify it to:

 

1958 edit

Glasser et al. define the lightness as ten times the Munsell value (so that the lightness ranges from 0 to 100):[14]

 

1964 edit

Günter Wyszecki simplifies this to:[15]

 

This formula approximates the Munsell value function for 1% < Y < 98% (it is not applicable for Y < 1%) and is used for the CIE 1964 color space.

1976 edit

CIELAB uses the following formula:

 

where Yn is the CIE XYZ Y tristimulus value of the reference white point (the subscript n suggests "normalized") and is subject to the restriction Y/Yn > 0.01. Pauli removes this restriction by computing a linear extrapolation which maps Y/Yn = 0 to L* = 0 and is tangent to the formula above at the point at which the linear extension takes effect. First, the transition point is determined to be Y/Yn = (6/29)3 ≈ 0.008856, then the slope of (29/3)3 ≈ 903.3 is computed. This gives the two-part function:[16]

 

The lightness is then:

 

At first glance, you might approximate the lightness function by a cube root, an approximation that is found in much of the technical literature. However, the linear segment near black is significant, and so the 116 and 16 coefficients. The best-fit pure power function has an exponent of about 0.42, far from 1/3.[17] An approximately 18% grey card, having an exact reflectance of (33/58)3, has a lightness value of 50. It is called "mid grey" because its lightness is midway between black and white.

1997 edit

As early as in 1967 a hyperbolic relationship between light intensity and cone cell responses was discovered in fish, in line with the Michaelis–Menten kinetics model of biochemical reactions.[18] In the 70s the same relationship was found in a number of other vertebrates and in 1982, using microelectrodes to measure cone responses in living rhesus macaques, Valeton and Van Norren found the following relationship:[19]

1 / V ~ 1 + (σ / I)0.74

where V is the measured potential, I the light intensity and σ a constant. In 1986 Seim and Valberg realised that this relationship might aid in the construction of a more uniform colour space.[20] This inspired advances in colour modelling and when the International Commission on Illumination held a symposium in 1996, objectives for a new standard colour model were formulated and in 1997 CIECAM97s (International Commission on Illumination, colour appearance model, 1997, simple version) was standardised.[21] CIECAM97s distinguishes between lightness, how light something appears compared to a similarly lit white object, and brightness, how much light appears to shine from something.[22] According to CIECAM97s the lightness of a sample is:

J = 100 (Asample / Awhite)cz

In this formula, for a small sample under bright conditions in a surrounding field with a relative luminance n compared to white, c has been chosen such that:

 

This models that a sample will appear darker on a light background than on a dark background. See contrast effect for more information on the topic. When n = 1/5, cz = 1, representing the assumption that most scenes have an average relative luminance of 1/5 compared to bright white, and that therefore a sample in such a surround should be perceived at its proper lightness. The quantity A models the achromatic cone response; it is colour dependent but for a grey sample under bright conditions it works out as:

 
Nbb is a fudge factor that is normally 1; it's only of concern when comparing brightness judgements based on slightly different reference whites.

Here Y is the relative luminance compared to white on a scale of 0 to 1 and LA is the average luminance of the adapting visual field as a whole, measured in cd/m2. The achromatic response follows a kind of S-curve, ranging from 1 to 123, numbers which follow from the way the cone responses are averaged and which are ultimately based on a rough estimate for the useful range of nerve impulses per second, and which has a fairly large intermediate range where it roughly follows a square root curve. The brightness according to CIECAM97s is then:

Q = (1.24 / c) (J / 100)0.67 (Awhite + 3)0.9

The factor 1.24 / c is a surround factor that reflects that scenes appear brighter in dark surrounding conditions. Suggestions for a more comprehensive model, CIECAM97C, were also formulated, to take into account several effects at extremely dark or bright conditions, coloured lighting, as well as the Helmholtz–Kohlrausch effect, where highly chromatic samples appear lighter and brighter in comparison to a neutral grey. To model the latter effect, in CIECAM97C the formula for J is adjusted as follows:

JHK = J + (100 – J) (C / 300) |sin(½h – 45°)|,

where C is the chroma and h the hue angle

Q is then calculated from JHK instead of from J. This formula has the effect of pulling up the lightness and brightness of coloured samples. The larger the chroma, the stronger the effect; for very saturated colours C can be close to 100 or even higher. The absolute sine term has a sharp V-shaped valley with a zero at yellow and a broad plateau in the deep blues.[23]

2002 edit

The achromatic response in CIECAM97s is a weighted addition of cone responses minus 2.05. Since the total noise term adds up to 3.05, this means that A and consequentially J and Q aren't zero for absolute black. To fix this, Li, Luo & Hunt suggested subtracting 3.05 instead, so the scale starts at zero.[24] Although CIECAM97s was a successful model to spur and direct colorimetric research, Fairchild felt that for practical applications some changes were necessary. Those relevant for lightness calculations were to, rather than use several discrete values for the surround factor c, allow for linear interpolation of c and thereby allowing the model to be used under intermediate surround conditions, and to simplify z to remove the special case for large stimuli because he felt it was irrelevant for imaging applications.[25] Based on experimental results, Hunt, Li, Juan and Luo proposed a number of improvements. Relevant for the topic at hand is that they suggested lowering z slightly.[26] Li and Luo found that a colour space based on such a modified CIECAM97s using lightness as one of the coordinates was more perceptually uniform than CIELAB.[27]

Because of the shape of the cone response S-curve, when the luminance of a colour is reduced, even if its spectral composition remains the same, the different cone responses do not quite change at the same rate with respect to each other. It is plausible therefore that the perceived hue and saturation will change at low luminance levels. But CIECAM97s predicts much larger deviations than are generally thought likely and therefore Hunt, Li and Luo suggested using a cone response curve which approximates a power curve for a much larger range of stimuli, so hue and saturation are better preserved.[28]

All these proposals, as well as others relating to chromaticity, resulted in a new colour appearance model, CIECAM02. In this model, the formula for lightness remains the same:

J = 100 (Asample / Awhite)cz

But all the quantities that feed into this formula change in some way. The parameter c is now continuously variable as discussed above and z = 1.48 + √n. Although this is higher than z in CIECAM97s, the total effective power factor is very similar because the effective power factor of the achromatic response is much lower:

 

As before, this formula assumes bright conditions. Apart from 1220, which results from an arbitrarily assumed cone response constant, the various constants in CIECAM02 were fitted to experimental data sets. The expression for the brightness has also changed considerably:

 

Note that contrary to the suggestion from CIECAM97C, CIECAM02 contains no provision for the Helmholtz–Kohlrausch effect.[29][30]

Other psychological effects edit

This subjective perception of luminance in a non-linear fashion is one thing that makes gamma compression of images worthwhile. Beside this phenomenon there are other effects involving perception of lightness. Chromaticity can affect perceived lightness as described by the Helmholtz–Kohlrausch effect. Though the CIELAB space and relatives do not account for this effect on lightness, it may be implied in the Munsell color model. Light levels may also affect perceived chromaticity, as with the Purkinje effect.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Most of the disadvantages below are listed in A Technical Introduction to Digital Video (1996) by Charles Poynton, though as mere statements, without examples.
  2. ^ Kuehni, Rolf G. (February 2002). "The early development of the Munsell system". Color Research & Application. 27 (1): 20–27. doi:10.1002/col.10002.
  3. ^ Hunt, Robert W. G. (May 18, 1957). "Light Energy and Brightness Sensation". Nature. 179 (4568): 1026. Bibcode:1957Natur.179Q1026H. doi:10.1038/1791026a0. PMID 13430776.
  4. ^ Valberg, Arne (2006). Light Vision Color. John Wiley and Sons. p. 200. ISBN 978-0470849026.
  5. ^ Priest, Irwin G.; Gibson, K.S.; McNicholas, H.J. (September 1920). "An examination of the Munsell color system. I: Spectral and total reflection and the Munsell scale of Value". Technical Paper 167 (3). United States Bureau of Standards: 27.
  6. ^ Munsell, A.E.O.; Sloan, L.L.; Godlove, I.H. (November 1933). "Neutral value scales. I. Munsell neutral value scale". JOSA. 23 (11): 394–411. Bibcode:1933JOSA...23..394M. doi:10.1364/JOSA.23.000394. Note: This paper contains a historical survey stretching to 1760.
  7. ^ Munsell, A.E.O.; Sloan, L.L.; Godlove, I.H. (December 1933). "Neutral value scales. II. A comparison of results and equations describing value scales". JOSA. 23 (12): 419–425. Bibcode:1933JOSA...23..419G. doi:10.1364/JOSA.23.000419.
  8. ^ Newhall, Sidney M.; Nickerson, Dorothy; Judd, Deane B (May 1943). "Final report of the O.S.A. subcommittee on the spacing of the Munsell colors". Journal of the Optical Society of America. 33 (7): 385–418. Bibcode:1943JOSA...33..385N. doi:10.1364/JOSA.33.000385.
  9. ^ Moon, Parry; Spencer, Domina Eberle (May 1943). "Metric based on the composite color stimulus". JOSA. 33 (5): 270–277. Bibcode:1943JOSA...33..270M. doi:10.1364/JOSA.33.000270.
  10. ^ Saunderson, Jason L.; Milner, B.I. (March 1944). "Further study of ω space". JOSA. 34 (3): 167–173. Bibcode:1944JOSA...34..167S. doi:10.1364/JOSA.34.000167.
  11. ^ Hurvich, Leo M.; Jameson, Dorothea (November 1957). "An Opponent-Process Theory of Color Vision". Psychological Review. 64 (6): 384–404. doi:10.1037/h0041403. PMID 13505974. S2CID 27613265.
  12. ^ Jameson, Dorothea; Leo M. Hurvich (May 1964). "Theory of brightness and color contrast in human vision". Vision Research. 4 (1–2): 135–154. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(64)90037-9. PMID 5888593.
  13. ^ Ladd, J.H.; Pinney, J.E. (September 1955). "Empirical relationships with the Munsell Value scale". Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers. 43 (9): 1137. doi:10.1109/JRPROC.1955.277892.
  14. ^ Glasser, L.G.; A.H. McKinney; C.D. Reilly; P.D. Schnelle (October 1958). "Cube-root color coordinate system". JOSA. 48 (10): 736–740. Bibcode:1958JOSA...48..736G. doi:10.1364/JOSA.48.000736.
  15. ^ Wyszecki, Günter (November 1963). "Proposal for a New Color-Difference Formula". JOSA. 53 (11): 1318–1319. Bibcode:1963JOSA...53.1318W. doi:10.1364/JOSA.53.001318. Note: The asterisks are not used in the paper.
  16. ^ Pauli, Hartmut K.A. (1976). "Proposed extension of the CIE recommendation on "Uniform color spaces, color spaces, and color-difference equations, and metric color terms"". JOSA. 66 (8): 866–867. doi:10.1364/JOSA.66.000866.
  17. ^ Poynton, Charles; Funt, Brian (February 2014). "Perceptual uniformity in digital image representation and display". Color Research and Application. 39 (1): 6–15. doi:10.1002/col.21768.
  18. ^ Ken-Ichi Naka & William Albert Hugh Rushton: The generation and spread of S-potentials in fish (Cyprinidae)
  19. ^ Jean Mathieu Valeton & Dirk van Norren: Light adaptation of primate cones: An analysis based on extracellular data
  20. ^ Thorstein Seim & Arne Valberg: Towards a uniform color space: A better formula to describe the Munsell and OSA color scales
  21. ^ Mark D. Fairchild: Color Appearance Models § The CIE Color Appearance Model (1997), CIECAM97s
  22. ^ Robert William Gainer Hunt: Some comments on using the CIECAM97s colour-appearance model
  23. ^ Ming Ronnier Luo & Robert William Gainer Hunt: The structure of the CIE 1997 colour appearance model
  24. ^ Changjun Li, Ming Ronnier Luo & Robert William Gainer Hunt: A revision of the CIECAM97s model
  25. ^ Mark D. Fairchild: A revision of CIECAM97s for practical applications
  26. ^ Robert William Gainer Hunt, Changjun Li, Lu-Yin Grace Juan & Ming Ronnier Luo: Further improvements to CIECAM97s (also cited as Further refinements to CIECAM97s)
  27. ^ Changjun Li & Ming Ronnier Luo: A uniform colour space based upon CIECAM97s
  28. ^ Robert William Gainer Hunt, Changjun Li & Ming Ronnier Luo: Dynamic cone response functions for models of colour appearance
  29. ^ Nathan Moroney, Mark D. Fairchild, Robert William Gainer Hunt, Changjun Li, Ming Ronnier Luo & Todd Newman: The CIECAM02 color appearance model
  30. ^

External links edit

  Media related to Lightness at Wikimedia Commons

lightness, other, uses, disambiguation, visual, perception, luminance, displaystyle, object, often, judged, relative, similarly, object, colorimetry, color, appearance, models, lightness, prediction, illuminated, color, will, appear, standard, observer, while,. For other uses see Lightness disambiguation Lightness is a visual perception of the luminance L displaystyle L of an object It is often judged relative to a similarly lit object In colorimetry and color appearance models lightness is a prediction of how an illuminated color will appear to a standard observer While luminance is a linear measurement of light lightness is a linear prediction of the human perception of that light Three hues in the Munsell color model Each color differs in value from top to bottom in equal perception steps The right column undergoes a dramatic change in perceived color This distinction is meaningful because human vision s lightness perception is non linear relative to light Doubling the quantity of light does not result in a doubling in perceived lightness only a modest increase The symbol for perceptual lightness is usually either J displaystyle J as used in CIECAM02 or L displaystyle L as used in CIELAB and CIELUV L displaystyle L Lstar is not to be confused with L displaystyle L as used for luminance In some color ordering systems such as Munsell Lightness is referenced as value Chiaroscuro and Tenebrism both take advantage of dramatic contrasts of value to heighten drama in art Artists may also employ shading subtle manipulation of value Contents 1 Lightness in different colorspaces 2 Relationship to value and relative luminance 2 1 1920 2 2 1933 2 3 1943 2 4 1943 2 5 1944 2 6 1955 2 7 1958 2 8 1964 2 9 1976 2 10 1997 2 11 2002 3 Other psychological effects 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksLightness in different colorspaces edit nbsp Fig 2a The sRGB gamut mapped in CIELAB space Notice that the lines pointing to the red green and blue primaries are not evenly spaced by hue angle and are of unequal length The primaries also have different L values nbsp Fig 2b The Adobe RGB gamut mapped in CIELAB space Also notice that these two RGB spaces have different gamuts and thus will have different HSL and HSV representations In some colorspaces or color systems such as Munsell HCL and CIELAB the lightness value achromatically constrains the maximum and minimum limits and operates independently of the hue and chroma For example Munsell value 0 is pure black and value 10 is pure white Colors with a discernible hue must therefore have values in between these extremes In a subtractive color model e g paint dye or ink lightness changes to a color through various tints shades or tones can be achieved by adding white black or grey respectively This also reduces saturation In HSL and HSV the displayed luminance is relative to the hue and chroma for a given lightness value in other words the selected lightness value does not predict the actual displayed luminance nor the perception thereof Both systems use coordinate triples where many triples can map onto the same color In HSV all triples with value 0 are pure black If the hue and saturation are held constant then increasing the value increases the luminance such that a value of 1 is the lightest color with the given hue and saturation HSL is similar except that all triples with lightness 1 are pure white In both models all pure saturated colors indicate the same lightness or value but this does not relate to the displayed luminance which is determined by the hue I e yellow is higher luminance than blue even if the lightness value is set at a given number While HSL HSV and similar spaces serve well enough to choose or adjust a single color they are not perceptually uniform They trade off accuracy for computational simplicity as they were created in an era where computer technology was restricted in performance 1 If we take an image and extract the hue saturation and lightness or value components for a given color space we will see that they may differ substantially from a different color space or model For example examine the following images of a fire breather fig 1 The original is in the sRGB color space CIELAB L displaystyle L nbsp is a perceptually uniform lightness prediction that is derived from luminance Y displaystyle Y nbsp but discards the X displaystyle X nbsp and Z displaystyle Z nbsp of the CIE XYZ color space Notice this appears similar in perceived lightness to the original color image Luma Y displaystyle Y prime nbsp is a gamma encoded luminance component of some video encoding systems such as Y I Q displaystyle Y prime IQ nbsp and Y U V displaystyle Y prime UV nbsp It is roughly similar but differs at high chroma deviating most from an achromatic signal such as linear luminance Y displaystyle Y nbsp or non linear lightness L displaystyle L nbsp HSL L displaystyle L nbsp and HSV V displaystyle V nbsp are neither perceptually uniform nor uniform as to luminance nbsp Fig 1a Color photograph sRGB color space nbsp Fig 1b CIELAB L further transformed back to sRGB for consistent display nbsp Fig 1c Rec 601 luma Y nbsp Fig 1d Component average intensity I nbsp Fig 1e HSV value V nbsp Fig 1f HSL lightness L Relationship to value and relative luminance editThe Munsell value has long been used as a perceptually uniform lightness scale A question of interest is the relationship between the Munsell value scale and the relative luminance Aware of the Weber Fechner law Albert Munsell remarked Should we use a logarithmic curve or curve of squares 2 Neither option turned out to be quite correct scientists eventually converged on a roughly cube root curve consistent with the Stevens s power law for brightness perception reflecting the fact that lightness is proportional to the number of nerve impulses per nerve fiber per unit time 3 The remainder of this section is a chronology of lightness models leading to CIECAM02 Note Munsell s V runs from 0 to 10 while Y typically runs from 0 to 100 often interpreted as a percentage Typically the relative luminance is normalized so that the reference white say magnesium oxide has a tristimulus value of Y 100 Since the reflectance of magnesium oxide MgO relative to the perfect reflecting diffuser is 97 5 V 10 corresponds to Y 100 97 5 102 6 if MgO is used as the reference 4 nbsp Observe that the lightness is 50 for a relative luminance of around 18 relative to the reference white 1920 edit Irwin Priest Kasson Gibson and Harry McNicholas provide a basic estimate of the Munsell value with Y running from 0 to 1 in this case 5 V 10 Y displaystyle V 10 sqrt Y nbsp 1933 edit Alexander Munsell Louise Sloan and Isaac Godlove launch a study on the Munsell neutral value scale considering several proposals relating the relative luminance to the Munsell value and suggest 6 7 V 2 1 4742 Y 0 004743 Y 2 displaystyle V 2 1 4742Y 0 004743Y 2 nbsp 1943 edit Sidney Newhall Dorothy Nickerson and Deane Judd prepare a report for the Optical Society of America OSA on Munsell renotation They suggest a quintic parabola relating the reflectance in terms of the value 8 Y 1 2219 V 0 23111 V 2 0 23951 V 3 0 021009 V 4 0 0008404 V 5 displaystyle Y 1 2219V 0 23111V 2 0 23951V 3 0 021009V 4 0 0008404V 5 nbsp 1943 edit Using Table II of the OSA report Parry Moon and Domina Spencer express the value in terms of the relative luminance 9 V 5 Y 19 77 0 426 1 4 Y 0 426 displaystyle V 5 left frac Y 19 77 right 0 426 1 4Y 0 426 nbsp 1944 edit Jason Saunderson and B I Milner introduce a subtractive constant in the previous expression for a better fit to the Munsell value 10 Later Dorothea Jameson and Leo Hurvich claim that this corrects for simultaneous contrast effects 11 12 V 2 357 Y 0 343 1 52 displaystyle V 2 357Y 0 343 1 52 nbsp 1955 edit Ladd and Pinney of Eastman Kodak are interested in the Munsell value as a perceptually uniform lightness scale for use in television After considering one logarithmic and five power law functions per Stevens power law they relate value to reflectance by raising the reflectance to the power of 0 352 13 V 2 217 Y 0 352 1 324 displaystyle V 2 217Y 0 352 1 324 nbsp Realizing this is quite close to the cube root they simplify it to V 2 468 Y 3 1 636 displaystyle V 2 468 sqrt 3 Y 1 636 nbsp 1958 edit Glasser et al define the lightness as ten times the Munsell value so that the lightness ranges from 0 to 100 14 L 25 29 Y 3 18 38 displaystyle L star 25 29 sqrt 3 Y 18 38 nbsp 1964 edit Gunter Wyszecki simplifies this to 15 W 25 Y 3 17 displaystyle W star 25 sqrt 3 Y 17 nbsp This formula approximates the Munsell value function for 1 lt Y lt 98 it is not applicable for Y lt 1 and is used for the CIE 1964 color space 1976 edit CIELAB uses the following formula L 116 Y Y n 1 3 16 displaystyle L star 116 left frac Y Y mathrm n right frac 1 3 16 nbsp where Yn is the CIE XYZ Y tristimulus value of the reference white point the subscript n suggests normalized and is subject to the restriction Y Yn gt 0 01 Pauli removes this restriction by computing a linear extrapolation which maps Y Yn 0 to L 0 and is tangent to the formula above at the point at which the linear extension takes effect First the transition point is determined to be Y Yn 6 29 3 0 008856 then the slope of 29 3 3 903 3 is computed This gives the two part function 16 f t t 1 3 if t gt 6 29 3 1 3 29 6 2 t 4 29 otherwise displaystyle f t begin cases t frac 1 3 amp text if t gt left frac 6 29 right 3 frac 1 3 left frac 29 6 right 2 t frac 4 29 amp text otherwise end cases nbsp The lightness is then L 116 f Y Y n 16 displaystyle L star 116f left frac Y Y mathrm n right 16 nbsp At first glance you might approximate the lightness function by a cube root an approximation that is found in much of the technical literature However the linear segment near black is significant and so the 116 and 16 coefficients The best fit pure power function has an exponent of about 0 42 far from 1 3 17 An approximately 18 grey card having an exact reflectance of 33 58 3 has a lightness value of 50 It is called mid grey because its lightness is midway between black and white 1997 edit As early as in 1967 a hyperbolic relationship between light intensity and cone cell responses was discovered in fish in line with the Michaelis Menten kinetics model of biochemical reactions 18 In the 70s the same relationship was found in a number of other vertebrates and in 1982 using microelectrodes to measure cone responses in living rhesus macaques Valeton and Van Norren found the following relationship 19 1 V 1 s I 0 74 where V is the measured potential I the light intensity and s a constant In 1986 Seim and Valberg realised that this relationship might aid in the construction of a more uniform colour space 20 This inspired advances in colour modelling and when the International Commission on Illumination held a symposium in 1996 objectives for a new standard colour model were formulated and in 1997 CIECAM97s International Commission on Illumination colour appearance model 1997 simple version was standardised 21 CIECAM97s distinguishes between lightness how light something appears compared to a similarly lit white object and brightness how much light appears to shine from something 22 According to CIECAM97s the lightness of a sample is J 100 Asample Awhite cz In this formula for a small sample under bright conditions in a surrounding field with a relative luminance n compared to white c has been chosen such that cz 1 n 1 1 5 displaystyle text cz frac 1 sqrt n 1 sqrt frac 1 5 nbsp This models that a sample will appear darker on a light background than on a dark background See contrast effect for more information on the topic When n 1 5 cz 1 representing the assumption that most scenes have an average relative luminance of 1 5 compared to bright white and that therefore a sample in such a surround should be perceived at its proper lightness The quantity A models the achromatic cone response it is colour dependent but for a grey sample under bright conditions it works out as A N bb 122 1 2 1 10 Y 5 L A 3 0 73 1 displaystyle frac text A text N text bb frac 122 1 2 Bigl tfrac 1 10 Y sqrt 3 5L A Bigr 0 73 1 nbsp Nbb is a fudge factor that is normally 1 it s only of concern when comparing brightness judgements based on slightly different reference whites Here Y is the relative luminance compared to white on a scale of 0 to 1 and LA is the average luminance of the adapting visual field as a whole measured in cd m2 The achromatic response follows a kind of S curve ranging from 1 to 123 numbers which follow from the way the cone responses are averaged and which are ultimately based on a rough estimate for the useful range of nerve impulses per second and which has a fairly large intermediate range where it roughly follows a square root curve The brightness according to CIECAM97s is then Q 1 24 c J 100 0 67 Awhite 3 0 9 The factor 1 24 c is a surround factor that reflects that scenes appear brighter in dark surrounding conditions Suggestions for a more comprehensive model CIECAM97C were also formulated to take into account several effects at extremely dark or bright conditions coloured lighting as well as the Helmholtz Kohlrausch effect where highly chromatic samples appear lighter and brighter in comparison to a neutral grey To model the latter effect in CIECAM97C the formula for J is adjusted as follows JHK J 100 J C 300 sin h 45 where C is the chroma and h the hue angleQ is then calculated from JHK instead of from J This formula has the effect of pulling up the lightness and brightness of coloured samples The larger the chroma the stronger the effect for very saturated colours C can be close to 100 or even higher The absolute sine term has a sharp V shaped valley with a zero at yellow and a broad plateau in the deep blues 23 2002 edit The achromatic response in CIECAM97s is a weighted addition of cone responses minus 2 05 Since the total noise term adds up to 3 05 this means that A and consequentially J and Q aren t zero for absolute black To fix this Li Luo amp Hunt suggested subtracting 3 05 instead so the scale starts at zero 24 Although CIECAM97s was a successful model to spur and direct colorimetric research Fairchild felt that for practical applications some changes were necessary Those relevant for lightness calculations were to rather than use several discrete values for the surround factor c allow for linear interpolation of c and thereby allowing the model to be used under intermediate surround conditions and to simplify z to remove the special case for large stimuli because he felt it was irrelevant for imaging applications 25 Based on experimental results Hunt Li Juan and Luo proposed a number of improvements Relevant for the topic at hand is that they suggested lowering z slightly 26 Li and Luo found that a colour space based on such a modified CIECAM97s using lightness as one of the coordinates was more perceptually uniform than CIELAB 27 Because of the shape of the cone response S curve when the luminance of a colour is reduced even if its spectral composition remains the same the different cone responses do not quite change at the same rate with respect to each other It is plausible therefore that the perceived hue and saturation will change at low luminance levels But CIECAM97s predicts much larger deviations than are generally thought likely and therefore Hunt Li and Luo suggested using a cone response curve which approximates a power curve for a much larger range of stimuli so hue and saturation are better preserved 28 All these proposals as well as others relating to chromaticity resulted in a new colour appearance model CIECAM02 In this model the formula for lightness remains the same J 100 Asample Awhite cz But all the quantities that feed into this formula change in some way The parameter c is now continuously variable as discussed above and z 1 48 n Although this is higher than z in CIECAM97s the total effective power factor is very similar because the effective power factor of the achromatic response is much lower A N bb 1220 1 27 13 5 L A 3 Y 10 0 42 displaystyle frac text A text N text bb frac 1220 1 frac 27 13 left frac sqrt 3 5 text L text A text Y 10 right 0 42 nbsp As before this formula assumes bright conditions Apart from 1220 which results from an arbitrarily assumed cone response constant the various constants in CIECAM02 were fitted to experimental data sets The expression for the brightness has also changed considerably Q 4 c J 10 A w h i t e 4 5 L A 3 10 0 25 displaystyle Q frac 4 c frac sqrt J 10 A white 4 left frac sqrt 3 5L A 10 right 0 25 nbsp Note that contrary to the suggestion from CIECAM97C CIECAM02 contains no provision for the Helmholtz Kohlrausch effect 29 30 Other psychological effects editThis subjective perception of luminance in a non linear fashion is one thing that makes gamma compression of images worthwhile Beside this phenomenon there are other effects involving perception of lightness Chromaticity can affect perceived lightness as described by the Helmholtz Kohlrausch effect Though the CIELAB space and relatives do not account for this effect on lightness it may be implied in the Munsell color model Light levels may also affect perceived chromaticity as with the Purkinje effect See also editBrightness Tint shade and toneReferences edit Most of the disadvantages below are listed in A Technical Introduction to Digital Video 1996 by Charles Poynton though as mere statements without examples Kuehni Rolf G February 2002 The early development of the Munsell system Color Research amp Application 27 1 20 27 doi 10 1002 col 10002 Hunt Robert W G May 18 1957 Light Energy and Brightness Sensation Nature 179 4568 1026 Bibcode 1957Natur 179Q1026H doi 10 1038 1791026a0 PMID 13430776 Valberg Arne 2006 Light Vision Color John Wiley and Sons p 200 ISBN 978 0470849026 Priest Irwin G Gibson K S McNicholas H J September 1920 An examination of the Munsell color system I Spectral and total reflection and the Munsell scale of Value Technical Paper 167 3 United States Bureau of Standards 27 Munsell A E O Sloan L L Godlove I H November 1933 Neutral value scales I Munsell neutral value scale JOSA 23 11 394 411 Bibcode 1933JOSA 23 394M doi 10 1364 JOSA 23 000394 Note This paper contains a historical survey stretching to 1760 Munsell A E O Sloan L L Godlove I H December 1933 Neutral value scales II A comparison of results and equations describing value scales JOSA 23 12 419 425 Bibcode 1933JOSA 23 419G doi 10 1364 JOSA 23 000419 Newhall Sidney M Nickerson Dorothy Judd Deane B May 1943 Final report of the O S A subcommittee on the spacing of the Munsell colors Journal of the Optical Society of America 33 7 385 418 Bibcode 1943JOSA 33 385N doi 10 1364 JOSA 33 000385 Moon Parry Spencer Domina Eberle May 1943 Metric based on the composite color stimulus JOSA 33 5 270 277 Bibcode 1943JOSA 33 270M doi 10 1364 JOSA 33 000270 Saunderson Jason L Milner B I March 1944 Further study of w space JOSA 34 3 167 173 Bibcode 1944JOSA 34 167S doi 10 1364 JOSA 34 000167 Hurvich Leo M Jameson Dorothea November 1957 An Opponent Process Theory of Color Vision Psychological Review 64 6 384 404 doi 10 1037 h0041403 PMID 13505974 S2CID 27613265 Jameson Dorothea Leo M Hurvich May 1964 Theory of brightness and color contrast in human vision Vision Research 4 1 2 135 154 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 64 90037 9 PMID 5888593 Ladd J H Pinney J E September 1955 Empirical relationships with the Munsell Value scale Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers 43 9 1137 doi 10 1109 JRPROC 1955 277892 Glasser L G A H McKinney C D Reilly P D Schnelle October 1958 Cube root color coordinate system JOSA 48 10 736 740 Bibcode 1958JOSA 48 736G doi 10 1364 JOSA 48 000736 Wyszecki Gunter November 1963 Proposal for a New Color Difference Formula JOSA 53 11 1318 1319 Bibcode 1963JOSA 53 1318W doi 10 1364 JOSA 53 001318 Note The asterisks are not used in the paper Pauli Hartmut K A 1976 Proposed extension of the CIE recommendation on Uniform color spaces color spaces and color difference equations and metric color terms JOSA 66 8 866 867 doi 10 1364 JOSA 66 000866 Poynton Charles Funt Brian February 2014 Perceptual uniformity in digital image representation and display Color Research and Application 39 1 6 15 doi 10 1002 col 21768 Ken Ichi Naka amp William Albert Hugh Rushton The generation and spread of S potentials in fish Cyprinidae Jean Mathieu Valeton amp Dirk van Norren Light adaptation of primate cones An analysis based on extracellular data Thorstein Seim amp Arne Valberg Towards a uniform color space A better formula to describe the Munsell and OSA color scales Mark D Fairchild Color Appearance Models The CIE Color Appearance Model 1997 CIECAM97s Robert William Gainer Hunt Some comments on using the CIECAM97s colour appearance model Ming Ronnier Luo amp Robert William Gainer Hunt The structure of the CIE 1997 colour appearance model Changjun Li Ming Ronnier Luo amp Robert William Gainer Hunt A revision of the CIECAM97s model Mark D Fairchild A revision of CIECAM97s for practical applications Robert William Gainer Hunt Changjun Li Lu Yin Grace Juan amp Ming Ronnier Luo Further improvements to CIECAM97s also cited as Further refinements to CIECAM97s Changjun Li amp Ming Ronnier Luo A uniform colour space based upon CIECAM97s Robert William Gainer Hunt Changjun Li amp Ming Ronnier Luo Dynamic cone response functions for models of colour appearance Nathan Moroney Mark D Fairchild Robert William Gainer Hunt Changjun Li Ming Ronnier Luo amp Todd Newman The CIECAM02 color appearance model CIE technical committee Colour Appearance Models for Color Management ApplicationsExternal links edit nbsp Media related to Lightness at Wikimedia Commons Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Lightness amp oldid 1220868760, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.