fbpx
Wikipedia

Hypercorrection (psychology)

Hypercorrection is the higher likelihood of correcting a general knowledge error when originally certain that the information they understand is accurate as opposed to unsure of the information.[1] The phenomenon suggests that once a general knowledge information is confidently misremembered by someone and the person learns the right version after their initial response is corrected, their likelihood of remembering this piece of information will be higher than someone who was unsure of their initial answer. It refers to the finding that when given corrective feedback, errors that are committed with high confidence are easier to correct than low confidence errors.[2]

For example, a student taking a test on state capitals is certain that Pittsburgh is the capital of Pennsylvania. When the test is returned, the answer has been corrected to Harrisburg. Shocked that the answer was incorrect, the student is more likely to make sure to remember the correct answer than the student who was originally unsure about the answer.

The hypercorrection effect explores whether making mistakes early on in the learning process can be beneficial to the learner and their encoding of the material.[3]

History edit

The pattern was named "hypercorrection" by psychologists Janet Metcalfe and Brady Butterfield of Columbia University in 2001.[4] However, it was originally noticed by Raymond W. Kulhavy (1977), who wrote an educational review focusing on students and the correction process. Kulhavy discovered that those students who had confidently answered incorrectly on tests or other modes of examination such as homework, when corrected, were much more likely to remember the material on later tests.[5]

In a study by the same researchers in 2006, Metcalfe and Butterfield, hypercorrection effect and its implications are further examined. Beginning with the presupposition that an error committed with high confidence would require a great deal of effort to overwrite, the researchers concluded that errors committed with a great deal of confidence were among the easiest errors to correct.[6]

Research edit

Even though conceptions about hypercorrection emerged in educational research, it is not limited only to the learning environment. General knowledge errors can be learned from books, movies, or television, especially with the natural tendencies to believe things are true.[7] A common example is the misconception that raindrops are tear-shaped. Understandably, many believe this because of depictions of such raindrops on weather channels.

In the past few years, hypercorrection research has focused on the factors behind it and whether people of all ages exhibit this phenomenon. There has been evidence that surprise or embarrassment of getting the answer wrong has an important role in hypercorrection. As people fear being ridiculed for answering a general knowledge question incorrectly, they will be more likely to remember a confident mistake they had made once. For example, a person suggests that scallops come from trees. This person's friends laugh, pointing out that scallops come from the ocean. Embarrassed that the response was incorrect, the person makes sure to remember this fact to avoid embarrassment.

Another factor implicated in hypercorrection is that there will be more vocabulary or knowledge used about answers that the individual is certain of instead of unsure about, possibly implying that people are more familiar with the answers they are confident with. As research on hypercorrection shows, subjects are likely to guess or pick the correct answer on retests when they were sure about their response on the original test.[8] This suggests that familiarity with the information may be part of producing the hypercorrection effect.

The claims about the hypercorrection effect stating that it has a significant part in the correction of mistakes have also been supported by a study done using brain imaging. Janet Metcalfe, Brady Butterfield, Christian Habeck, and Yaakov Stern (2012) conducted an experiment using fMRI to observe neural correlations related to the hypercorrection effects in people. 15 people participated in the experiment; 10 women and 5 men. The experimenters delivered a questionnaire to the participants and recorded brain activities using fMRI, while correcting incorrect answers. It was discovered that while participants received their correction after committing a mistake, the brain was showing activation of the Temporoparietal Junction (TPJ) which indicates that they were entertaining the original false belief as well as the true belief.[9]

Because the hypercorrection effect is all about our knowledge, knowledge errors, and their effects on our correction of knowledge errors, it seems likely that this effect will have been studied in genuine classroom settings. Yet, hypercorrection effects have mostly been studied by observation only of past scenarios. This effect has mostly been studied only in typical laboratory settings. A study done by Carpenter, Haynes, Corral, and Yeung (2018) is one of the first times this effect has been studied in an authentic educational context. This study was conducted in a university’s introductory horticulture class. The students in this class were first presented with questionnaires related to the educational content of that course. They were then presented with the correct answers and were later given a post-test to test the same information in the same way once again. When analyzing the test results, the researchers determined that a powerful hypercorrection effect was present. In analyzing these results, the researchers found that students who had a higher previous knowledge and understanding of the material not only showed a higher confidence in their original answers but also exhibited a much more prevalent and more impressive hypercorrection effect of their inaccurate answers. We see from this and the other studies mentioned that a higher general knowledge of the topic seems to also bring along with it a more potent hypercorrection effect.[10]

Janet Metcalfe (2017) explores the effect and possible repercussions of not using this hypercorrection effect to our advantage in the classroom. In an annual psychology review she wrote on this topic; she explains how focusing solely on avoiding errors completely before testing in the classroom may actually be a disadvantageous method of teaching and learning. This claim is made only for neurologically typical students. Metcalfe even goes as far to say that it may actually be a beneficial practice for students in education to commit and correct errors while in low-stakes situations as a method of coming to learn and understand the given material more unshakably.[3] In addition to the benefits of making errors to the learners, Metcalfe also claims that the process of hearing the errors and correcting them can be helpful for the teachers: Aside from the direct benefit to learners, teachers gain valuable information from errors, and error tolerance encourages students’ active, exploratory, generative engagement.[11] This adds another beneficial aspect of hypercorrection in education, further proving the importance of the theory.

Hypercorrection and age edit

There have been implications that age plays a role within the hypercorrection effect, after research showed that not all people showed this effect. Most studies in the past have asked young adults to answer general knowledge questions. Recently, older adults have been tested and have not shown the hypercorrection effect, though this does not provide definitive evidence to state that older adults cannot exhibit this phenomenon.[12] However, whether older adults are better at correcting knowledge they are unsure about or they are worse at hypercorrection is still to be determined.[13] The result for children are not concrete, but some say that the prefrontal cortex, a part of the brain that is crucial to memory, is important for the hypercorrection effect. This would be a plausible explanation since elders may have impaired prefrontal cortices and children's may be underdeveloped.

In speaking about the difference in hypercorrection’s effects relative to age, we learn from Metcalfe, Stern, and Eich (2014) that while older adults were seen to do a better job with general test accuracy, they had a low occurrence of the hypercorrection effect while younger adults were shown to display this phenomenon more frequently.[12] However, the researchers found out later that this was not because of a problem with their memory and processing mechanisms of older people. They tend to hypercorrect less, because older adults tended to correct all of their errors rather than just focusing on high-confidence errors.[14] Although this finding raises another question regarding the learning abilities of older adults, if they are so better at focusing on all errors they make than young people, how is it harder to learn at an older age? The researchers suggest that their experiment, that was conducted in vitro with factual pieces of information, can have a different outcome than real life, since older adults could be particularly motivated to learn the truth, and capable of engaging their attention to this end.[14] However, this suggestion remains unexplored in the field and needs further research to be proven.

Problems edit

A research conducted by Andrew Butler, Lisa Fazio and Elizabeth Marsh found out that high-confidence errors are more likely to be corrected, but they are also more likely to be reproduced if the correct answer is forgotten.[7] For the research, subjects were presented with general knowledge questions and asked about their confidence levels for their answers, as the wrong answers were corrected afterwards. Half of the subjects were asked the same question right after the test, while the other half were asked a week later. The research showed that after an initial period of one week, subjects were less likely to remember the answers to the same general knowledge questions correctly. Even more importantly, high-confidence errors were more likely than low-confidence errors to be reproduced on the delayed test.[7] The findings suggest an important fact about hypercorrection: high-confidence errors are more likely to be corrected, but they are also more likely to be reproduced if the correct answer is forgotten,[7] especially after a period of time.

Recent developments edit

The hypercorrection effect has been demonstrated and replicated in several settings and with many different types of participants in recent years. Metcalfe, J., & Miele, D. B. (2014). Hypercorrection of high confidence errors: Prior testing both enhances delayed performance and blocks the return of the errors.[8] The hypercorrection effect was tested with participants from the general population but was also demonstrated with a group of children with autism spectrum disorder.[15] Though those tested with autism spectrum disorder had a significantly weaker general metacognitive ability than previous participants without any mental disorders, they did not show any evidence of a weakened hypercorrection effect. Thus, the hypercorrection effect is seen by many to be completely disconnected from general metacognitive ability and is a phenomenon common among us all.[15]

References edit

  1. ^ Metcalfe, J. "Older Beats Younger When It Comes to Correcting Mistakes". Psychological Science. Association for Psychological Science. Retrieved April 19, 2016.
  2. ^ Metcalfe, Janet; Finn, Bridgid (March 2011). "People's Hypercorrection of High Confidence Errors: Did They Know it All Along?". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 37 (2): 437–448. doi:10.1037/a0021962. ISSN 0278-7393. PMC 3079415. PMID 21355668.
  3. ^ a b Metcalfe, Janet (2017). "Learning from Errors". Annual Review of Psychology. 68: 465–489. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044022. PMID 27648988.
  4. ^ Butterfield, B.; Metcalfe, J. (2001). "Errors committed with high confidence are hypercorrected". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (6): 1491–1494. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1491. PMID 11713883.
  5. ^ Kulhavy, Raymond W. (1977). "Feedback in Written Instruction". Review of Educational Research. 47 (2): 211–232. doi:10.2307/1170128. JSTOR 1170128.
  6. ^ Butterfield, Brady; Metcalfe, Janet (2006). "The correction of errors committed with high confidence". Metacognition and Learning. 1: 69–84. doi:10.1007/s11409-006-6894-z. S2CID 11430724.
  7. ^ a b c d Butler, Andrew C.; Fazio, Lisa K.; Marsh, Elizabeth J. (2011-12-01). "The hypercorrection effect persists over a week, but high-confidence errors return". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18 (6): 1238–1244. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0173-y. ISSN 1531-5320. PMID 21989771.
  8. ^ a b Metcalfe, Janet; Miele, David B. (2014). "Hypercorrection of high confidence errors: Prior testing both enhances delayed performance and blocks the return of the errors". Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 3 (3): 189–197. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.04.001.
  9. ^ Metcalfe, Janet; Butterfield, Brady; Habeck, Christian; Stern, Yaakov (2012). "Neural Correlates of People's Hypercorrection of Their False Beliefs". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24 (7): 1571–1583. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00228. PMC 3970786. PMID 22452558.
  10. ^ Carpenter, Shana K.; Haynes, Cynthia L.; Corral, Daniel; Yeung, Kam Leung (2018). "Hypercorrection of high-confidence errors in the classroom". Memory. 26 (10): 1379–1384. doi:10.1080/09658211.2018.1477164. PMID 29781391. S2CID 29160922.
  11. ^ Metcalfe, Janet (2017-01-03). "Learning from Errors". Annual Review of Psychology. 68 (1): 465–489. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044022. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 27648988.
  12. ^ a b Eich, Teal S.; Stern, Yaakov; Metcalfe, Janet (2013). "The hypercorrection effect in younger and older adults". Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 20 (5): 511–521. doi:10.1080/13825585.2012.754399. PMC 3604148. PMID 23241028.
  13. ^ Metcalfe, J.; Casal-Roscum, L.; Radin, A.; Friedman, D. (2015). "On Teaching Old Dogs New Tricks". Psychological Science. 26 (12): 1833–1842. doi:10.1177/0956797615597912. PMC 4679660. PMID 26494598.
  14. ^ a b Metcalfe, Janet; Casal-Roscum, Lindsey; Radin, Arielle; Friedman, David (December 2015). "On Teaching Old Dogs New Tricks". Psychological Science. 26 (12): 1833–1842. doi:10.1177/0956797615597912. ISSN 0956-7976. PMC 4679660. PMID 26494598.
  15. ^ a b Williams, David M; Bergström, Zara; Grainger, Catherine (2016-12-15). "Metacognitive monitoring and the hypercorrection effect in autism and the general population: Relation to autism(-like) traits and mindreading" (PDF). Autism. 22 (3): 259–270. doi:10.1177/1362361316680178. hdl:1893/24780. ISSN 1362-3613. PMID 29671645. S2CID 4951642.

hypercorrection, psychology, linguistic, usage, hypercorrection, compensation, psychological, tactic, compensation, psychology, hypercorrection, higher, likelihood, correcting, general, knowledge, error, when, originally, certain, that, information, they, unde. For the linguistic usage see Hypercorrection For compensation as psychological tactic see Compensation psychology Hypercorrection is the higher likelihood of correcting a general knowledge error when originally certain that the information they understand is accurate as opposed to unsure of the information 1 The phenomenon suggests that once a general knowledge information is confidently misremembered by someone and the person learns the right version after their initial response is corrected their likelihood of remembering this piece of information will be higher than someone who was unsure of their initial answer It refers to the finding that when given corrective feedback errors that are committed with high confidence are easier to correct than low confidence errors 2 For example a student taking a test on state capitals is certain that Pittsburgh is the capital of Pennsylvania When the test is returned the answer has been corrected to Harrisburg Shocked that the answer was incorrect the student is more likely to make sure to remember the correct answer than the student who was originally unsure about the answer The hypercorrection effect explores whether making mistakes early on in the learning process can be beneficial to the learner and their encoding of the material 3 Contents 1 History 2 Research 3 Hypercorrection and age 4 Problems 5 Recent developments 6 ReferencesHistory editThe pattern was named hypercorrection by psychologists Janet Metcalfe and Brady Butterfield of Columbia University in 2001 4 However it was originally noticed by Raymond W Kulhavy 1977 who wrote an educational review focusing on students and the correction process Kulhavy discovered that those students who had confidently answered incorrectly on tests or other modes of examination such as homework when corrected were much more likely to remember the material on later tests 5 In a study by the same researchers in 2006 Metcalfe and Butterfield hypercorrection effect and its implications are further examined Beginning with the presupposition that an error committed with high confidence would require a great deal of effort to overwrite the researchers concluded that errors committed with a great deal of confidence were among the easiest errors to correct 6 Research editEven though conceptions about hypercorrection emerged in educational research it is not limited only to the learning environment General knowledge errors can be learned from books movies or television especially with the natural tendencies to believe things are true 7 A common example is the misconception that raindrops are tear shaped Understandably many believe this because of depictions of such raindrops on weather channels In the past few years hypercorrection research has focused on the factors behind it and whether people of all ages exhibit this phenomenon There has been evidence that surprise or embarrassment of getting the answer wrong has an important role in hypercorrection As people fear being ridiculed for answering a general knowledge question incorrectly they will be more likely to remember a confident mistake they had made once For example a person suggests that scallops come from trees This person s friends laugh pointing out that scallops come from the ocean Embarrassed that the response was incorrect the person makes sure to remember this fact to avoid embarrassment Another factor implicated in hypercorrection is that there will be more vocabulary or knowledge used about answers that the individual is certain of instead of unsure about possibly implying that people are more familiar with the answers they are confident with As research on hypercorrection shows subjects are likely to guess or pick the correct answer on retests when they were sure about their response on the original test 8 This suggests that familiarity with the information may be part of producing the hypercorrection effect The claims about the hypercorrection effect stating that it has a significant part in the correction of mistakes have also been supported by a study done using brain imaging Janet Metcalfe Brady Butterfield Christian Habeck and Yaakov Stern 2012 conducted an experiment using fMRI to observe neural correlations related to the hypercorrection effects in people 15 people participated in the experiment 10 women and 5 men The experimenters delivered a questionnaire to the participants and recorded brain activities using fMRI while correcting incorrect answers It was discovered that while participants received their correction after committing a mistake the brain was showing activation of the Temporoparietal Junction TPJ which indicates that they were entertaining the original false belief as well as the true belief 9 Because the hypercorrection effect is all about our knowledge knowledge errors and their effects on our correction of knowledge errors it seems likely that this effect will have been studied in genuine classroom settings Yet hypercorrection effects have mostly been studied by observation only of past scenarios This effect has mostly been studied only in typical laboratory settings A study done by Carpenter Haynes Corral and Yeung 2018 is one of the first times this effect has been studied in an authentic educational context This study was conducted in a university s introductory horticulture class The students in this class were first presented with questionnaires related to the educational content of that course They were then presented with the correct answers and were later given a post test to test the same information in the same way once again When analyzing the test results the researchers determined that a powerful hypercorrection effect was present In analyzing these results the researchers found that students who had a higher previous knowledge and understanding of the material not only showed a higher confidence in their original answers but also exhibited a much more prevalent and more impressive hypercorrection effect of their inaccurate answers We see from this and the other studies mentioned that a higher general knowledge of the topic seems to also bring along with it a more potent hypercorrection effect 10 Janet Metcalfe 2017 explores the effect and possible repercussions of not using this hypercorrection effect to our advantage in the classroom In an annual psychology review she wrote on this topic she explains how focusing solely on avoiding errors completely before testing in the classroom may actually be a disadvantageous method of teaching and learning This claim is made only for neurologically typical students Metcalfe even goes as far to say that it may actually be a beneficial practice for students in education to commit and correct errors while in low stakes situations as a method of coming to learn and understand the given material more unshakably 3 In addition to the benefits of making errors to the learners Metcalfe also claims that the process of hearing the errors and correcting them can be helpful for the teachers Aside from the direct benefit to learners teachers gain valuable information from errors and error tolerance encourages students active exploratory generative engagement 11 This adds another beneficial aspect of hypercorrection in education further proving the importance of the theory Hypercorrection and age editThere have been implications that age plays a role within the hypercorrection effect after research showed that not all people showed this effect Most studies in the past have asked young adults to answer general knowledge questions Recently older adults have been tested and have not shown the hypercorrection effect though this does not provide definitive evidence to state that older adults cannot exhibit this phenomenon 12 However whether older adults are better at correcting knowledge they are unsure about or they are worse at hypercorrection is still to be determined 13 The result for children are not concrete but some say that the prefrontal cortex a part of the brain that is crucial to memory is important for the hypercorrection effect This would be a plausible explanation since elders may have impaired prefrontal cortices and children s may be underdeveloped In speaking about the difference in hypercorrection s effects relative to age we learn from Metcalfe Stern and Eich 2014 that while older adults were seen to do a better job with general test accuracy they had a low occurrence of the hypercorrection effect while younger adults were shown to display this phenomenon more frequently 12 However the researchers found out later that this was not because of a problem with their memory and processing mechanisms of older people They tend to hypercorrect less because older adults tended to correct all of their errors rather than just focusing on high confidence errors 14 Although this finding raises another question regarding the learning abilities of older adults if they are so better at focusing on all errors they make than young people how is it harder to learn at an older age The researchers suggest that their experiment that was conducted in vitro with factual pieces of information can have a different outcome than real life since older adults could be particularly motivated to learn the truth and capable of engaging their attention to this end 14 However this suggestion remains unexplored in the field and needs further research to be proven Problems editA research conducted by Andrew Butler Lisa Fazio and Elizabeth Marsh found out that high confidence errors are more likely to be corrected but they are also more likely to be reproduced if the correct answer is forgotten 7 For the research subjects were presented with general knowledge questions and asked about their confidence levels for their answers as the wrong answers were corrected afterwards Half of the subjects were asked the same question right after the test while the other half were asked a week later The research showed that after an initial period of one week subjects were less likely to remember the answers to the same general knowledge questions correctly Even more importantly high confidence errors were more likely than low confidence errors to be reproduced on the delayed test 7 The findings suggest an important fact about hypercorrection high confidence errors are more likely to be corrected but they are also more likely to be reproduced if the correct answer is forgotten 7 especially after a period of time Recent developments editThe hypercorrection effect has been demonstrated and replicated in several settings and with many different types of participants in recent years Metcalfe J amp Miele D B 2014 Hypercorrection of high confidence errors Prior testing both enhances delayed performance and blocks the return of the errors 8 The hypercorrection effect was tested with participants from the general population but was also demonstrated with a group of children with autism spectrum disorder 15 Though those tested with autism spectrum disorder had a significantly weaker general metacognitive ability than previous participants without any mental disorders they did not show any evidence of a weakened hypercorrection effect Thus the hypercorrection effect is seen by many to be completely disconnected from general metacognitive ability and is a phenomenon common among us all 15 References edit Metcalfe J Older Beats Younger When It Comes to Correcting Mistakes Psychological Science Association for Psychological Science Retrieved April 19 2016 Metcalfe Janet Finn Bridgid March 2011 People s Hypercorrection of High Confidence Errors Did They Know it All Along Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 37 2 437 448 doi 10 1037 a0021962 ISSN 0278 7393 PMC 3079415 PMID 21355668 a b Metcalfe Janet 2017 Learning from Errors Annual Review of Psychology 68 465 489 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 010416 044022 PMID 27648988 Butterfield B Metcalfe J 2001 Errors committed with high confidence are hypercorrected Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 27 6 1491 1494 doi 10 1037 0278 7393 27 6 1491 PMID 11713883 Kulhavy Raymond W 1977 Feedback in Written Instruction Review of Educational Research 47 2 211 232 doi 10 2307 1170128 JSTOR 1170128 Butterfield Brady Metcalfe Janet 2006 The correction of errors committed with high confidence Metacognition and Learning 1 69 84 doi 10 1007 s11409 006 6894 z S2CID 11430724 a b c d Butler Andrew C Fazio Lisa K Marsh Elizabeth J 2011 12 01 The hypercorrection effect persists over a week but high confidence errors return Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 18 6 1238 1244 doi 10 3758 s13423 011 0173 y ISSN 1531 5320 PMID 21989771 a b Metcalfe Janet Miele David B 2014 Hypercorrection of high confidence errors Prior testing both enhances delayed performance and blocks the return of the errors Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 3 3 189 197 doi 10 1016 j jarmac 2014 04 001 Metcalfe Janet Butterfield Brady Habeck Christian Stern Yaakov 2012 Neural Correlates of People s Hypercorrection of Their False Beliefs Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 7 1571 1583 doi 10 1162 jocn a 00228 PMC 3970786 PMID 22452558 Carpenter Shana K Haynes Cynthia L Corral Daniel Yeung Kam Leung 2018 Hypercorrection of high confidence errors in the classroom Memory 26 10 1379 1384 doi 10 1080 09658211 2018 1477164 PMID 29781391 S2CID 29160922 Metcalfe Janet 2017 01 03 Learning from Errors Annual Review of Psychology 68 1 465 489 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 010416 044022 ISSN 0066 4308 PMID 27648988 a b Eich Teal S Stern Yaakov Metcalfe Janet 2013 The hypercorrection effect in younger and older adults Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition 20 5 511 521 doi 10 1080 13825585 2012 754399 PMC 3604148 PMID 23241028 Metcalfe J Casal Roscum L Radin A Friedman D 2015 On Teaching Old Dogs New Tricks Psychological Science 26 12 1833 1842 doi 10 1177 0956797615597912 PMC 4679660 PMID 26494598 a b Metcalfe Janet Casal Roscum Lindsey Radin Arielle Friedman David December 2015 On Teaching Old Dogs New Tricks Psychological Science 26 12 1833 1842 doi 10 1177 0956797615597912 ISSN 0956 7976 PMC 4679660 PMID 26494598 a b Williams David M Bergstrom Zara Grainger Catherine 2016 12 15 Metacognitive monitoring and the hypercorrection effect in autism and the general population Relation to autism like traits and mindreading PDF Autism 22 3 259 270 doi 10 1177 1362361316680178 hdl 1893 24780 ISSN 1362 3613 PMID 29671645 S2CID 4951642 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Hypercorrection psychology amp oldid 1073079440, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.