fbpx
Wikipedia

Google effect

The Google effect, also called digital amnesia,[1] is the tendency to forget information that can be found readily online by using Internet search engines. According to the first study about the Google effect, people are less likely to remember certain details they believe will be accessible online. However, the study also claims that people's ability to learn information offline remains the same.[2] This effect may also be seen as a change to what information and what level of detail is considered to be important to remember.

History edit

The phenomenon was first described and named by Betsy Sparrow (Columbia University), Jenny Liu (University of Wisconsin–Madison) and Daniel M. Wegner (Harvard University) in their paper from July 2011.[3] The study was conducted in four parts. The first part had subjects answer a number of both easy and difficult trivia questions, and then perform a modified Stroop task involving both everyday words and technology-related words such as screen and Google. Subjects were slower to respond to technology words, especially after difficult questions, indicating that trivia questions primed them to think of computers. In the second experiment, the subjects read a number of trivia statements. Half of them were led to believe the statements would be saved and available to look up later; the other half were explicitly instructed to attempt to remember them. Both groups were then tested on recall of the statements. In part three, subjects read and typed in trivia statements, and informed that their entry was erased, saved, or saved in a specific location. Afterwards, they were given a recognition task and asked whether they had seen the exact statement, whether it had been saved, and if the statement had been saved, where it had been saved. In the final part, subjects again typed trivia statements, and were told each had been saved in a generically-named folder (e.g. items, facts). They were then given two different recall tasks: one for the statements, and one for the specific folder in which each statement was saved.[4]

The term "digital amnesia" was coined by Kaspersky Lab for the results of an unreviewed survey in 2015 by the security vendor, which said, "The results reveal that the 'Google Effect' likely extends beyond online facts to include important personal information."[1] Instead of remembering details, 91 percent of people used the Internet and 44 percent used their smartphone.[1] Kaspersky Lab surveyed 1000 consumers ranged from age 16 to 55+ in the United States. In most cases, people could not remember important information such as telephone numbers that should have been familiar, leading to the conclusion that they forgot the information because of the ease of finding it using devices.[5]

Phenomenon edit

The original 2011 study concluded with three main findings. First, people are primed to think of computers when asked general knowledge questions, even when they know the correct answer. In addition, this effect is especially pronounced if the question is difficult and the answer is unknown. Secondly, people do not tend to remember information if they believe it will be available to look up later. By contrast, an explicit instruction to remember the material does not have a significant effect on recall. Lastly, if the information is saved, people are much more likely to remember where the information is located than to recall the information itself. In addition, people tend to remember either the fact or the location, but not both; this effect persists even when the information is more memorable than the name of the location.[4][6]

A 2012 study by Lav R. Varshney has since proposed that the Google effect can also be seen in doctoral theses, claiming that a longitudinal increase in the number of references cited reflects a tendency for improved memory of where to find relevant information (i.e. which papers contain the information), rather than of the information itself.[7] Furthermore, a related phenomenon has been described in which information learned through the internet is recalled less accurately and with less confidence than information learned via an encyclopedia. Additionally, those recalling information learned via the Internet showed decreased activations in several brain regions—including the bilateral occipital gyrus, left temporal gyrus, and bilateral middle frontal gyrus—compared to the encyclopedia group.[8]

Transactive memory edit

Sparrow et al. originally claimed that reliance on computers is a form of transactive memory, because people share information easily, forget what they think will be available later, and remember the location of information better than the information itself. They posited that people and their computers are becoming "interconnected systems"; the same underlying processes used in traditional transactive memory to learn who in our social networks know what is also being extended to encompass what a computer knows and how to find it.[4]

The reliance on computers has raised concerns, such as when it prevents one from processing information and internalizing it. In addition, people appear less confident in recalling information learned through Internet searching and that recent Internet searching may promote motivation to use the Internet.[9]

However, several researchers have questioned whether the Google effect is a form of transactive memory, arguing that no transaction is going on between the person and the computer. Therefore, computer networks and the Internet cannot be conceived as a distributed cognitive system. Rather, computers are merely tools exploited to help trigger a memory or to easily look up information. Unlike in traditional transactive memory, the information is not lost without the Internet, but merely slower and more difficult to find.[10][11]

Replication edit

In a big Replication study published in Nature 2018,[12] the Google effect was one of the experiments which could not be replicated.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c "Study: Most Americans suffer from 'Digital Amnesia'". WTOP-FM. July 1, 2015. Retrieved November 11, 2015.
  2. ^ Krieger, Lisa M. (July 16, 2011). "Google is changing your brain, study says, and don't you forget it". San Jose Mercury News. Retrieved November 12, 2015.
  3. ^ . Columbia University. July 14, 2011. Archived from the original on July 17, 2011.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  4. ^ a b c Sparrow, B.; Liu, J.; Wegner, D. M. (August 5, 2011). (PDF). Science. 333 (6043): 776–778. Bibcode:2011Sci...333..776S. doi:10.1126/science.1207745. PMID 21764755. S2CID 7688306. Archived from the original on April 9, 2016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: bot: original URL status unknown (link)
  5. ^ Meyer, Dick (October 12, 2015). "Can't recall phone numbers? Blame 'digital amnesia'". Boston Herald. p. 15.
  6. ^ Olson, Curtis A. (2012-12-01). "Focused search and retrieval: The impact of technology on our brains". Journal of Continuing Education in the Health Professions. 32 (1): 1–3. doi:10.1002/chp.21117. PMID 22447705.
  7. ^ Varshney, Lav R. (2012-02-10). "The Google effect in doctoral theses". Scientometrics. 92 (3): 785–793. doi:10.1007/s11192-012-0654-4. S2CID 12426806.
  8. ^ Dong, Guangheng; Potenza, Marc N. (2015-10-01). "Behavioural and brain responses related to Internet search and memory". European Journal of Neuroscience. 42 (8): 2546–2554. doi:10.1111/ejn.13039. PMID 26262779. S2CID 30654435.
  9. ^ Rowlands, Ian; Nicholas, David; Williams, Peter; Huntington, Paul; Fieldhouse, Maggie; Gunter, Barrie; Withey, Richard; Jamali, Hamid R.; Dobrowolski, Tom; Tenopir, Carol (2008). "The Google generation: the information behaviour of the researcher of the future". ASLIB Proceedings. 60 (4): 290–310. doi:10.1108/00012530810887953. S2CID 15674801.
  10. ^ Huebner, Bryce (2016-03-01). "Transactive Memory Reconstructed: Rethinking Wegner's Research Program". The Southern Journal of Philosophy. 54 (1): 48–69. doi:10.1111/sjp.12160.
  11. ^ Huebner, Bryce (2013-12-01). "Socially embedded cognition". Cognitive Systems Research. Socially Extended Cognition. 25–26: 13–18. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.03.006. S2CID 25795227.
  12. ^ Camerer, Colin F.; Dreber, Anna; Holzmeister, Felix; Ho, Teck-Hua; Huber, Jürgen; Johannesson, Magnus; Kirchler, Michael; Nave, Gideon; Nosek, Brian A. (2018-08-27). "Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015". Nature Human Behaviour. 2 (9): 637–644. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z. ISSN 2397-3374. PMID 31346273. S2CID 52098703.

External links edit

    google, effect, also, called, digital, amnesia, tendency, forget, information, that, found, readily, online, using, internet, search, engines, according, first, study, about, people, less, likely, remember, certain, details, they, believe, will, accessible, on. The Google effect also called digital amnesia 1 is the tendency to forget information that can be found readily online by using Internet search engines According to the first study about the Google effect people are less likely to remember certain details they believe will be accessible online However the study also claims that people s ability to learn information offline remains the same 2 This effect may also be seen as a change to what information and what level of detail is considered to be important to remember Contents 1 History 2 Phenomenon 2 1 Transactive memory 2 2 Replication 3 See also 4 References 5 External linksHistory editThe phenomenon was first described and named by Betsy Sparrow Columbia University Jenny Liu University of Wisconsin Madison and Daniel M Wegner Harvard University in their paper from July 2011 3 The study was conducted in four parts The first part had subjects answer a number of both easy and difficult trivia questions and then perform a modified Stroop task involving both everyday words and technology related words such as screen and Google Subjects were slower to respond to technology words especially after difficult questions indicating that trivia questions primed them to think of computers In the second experiment the subjects read a number of trivia statements Half of them were led to believe the statements would be saved and available to look up later the other half were explicitly instructed to attempt to remember them Both groups were then tested on recall of the statements In part three subjects read and typed in trivia statements and informed that their entry was erased saved or saved in a specific location Afterwards they were given a recognition task and asked whether they had seen the exact statement whether it had been saved and if the statement had been saved where it had been saved In the final part subjects again typed trivia statements and were told each had been saved in a generically named folder e g items facts They were then given two different recall tasks one for the statements and one for the specific folder in which each statement was saved 4 The term digital amnesia was coined by Kaspersky Lab for the results of an unreviewed survey in 2015 by the security vendor which said The results reveal that the Google Effect likely extends beyond online facts to include important personal information 1 Instead of remembering details 91 percent of people used the Internet and 44 percent used their smartphone 1 Kaspersky Lab surveyed 1000 consumers ranged from age 16 to 55 in the United States In most cases people could not remember important information such as telephone numbers that should have been familiar leading to the conclusion that they forgot the information because of the ease of finding it using devices 5 Phenomenon editThe original 2011 study concluded with three main findings First people are primed to think of computers when asked general knowledge questions even when they know the correct answer In addition this effect is especially pronounced if the question is difficult and the answer is unknown Secondly people do not tend to remember information if they believe it will be available to look up later By contrast an explicit instruction to remember the material does not have a significant effect on recall Lastly if the information is saved people are much more likely to remember where the information is located than to recall the information itself In addition people tend to remember either the fact or the location but not both this effect persists even when the information is more memorable than the name of the location 4 6 A 2012 study by Lav R Varshney has since proposed that the Google effect can also be seen in doctoral theses claiming that a longitudinal increase in the number of references cited reflects a tendency for improved memory of where to find relevant information i e which papers contain the information rather than of the information itself 7 Furthermore a related phenomenon has been described in which information learned through the internet is recalled less accurately and with less confidence than information learned via an encyclopedia Additionally those recalling information learned via the Internet showed decreased activations in several brain regions including the bilateral occipital gyrus left temporal gyrus and bilateral middle frontal gyrus compared to the encyclopedia group 8 Transactive memory edit Sparrow et al originally claimed that reliance on computers is a form of transactive memory because people share information easily forget what they think will be available later and remember the location of information better than the information itself They posited that people and their computers are becoming interconnected systems the same underlying processes used in traditional transactive memory to learn who in our social networks know what is also being extended to encompass what a computer knows and how to find it 4 The reliance on computers has raised concerns such as when it prevents one from processing information and internalizing it In addition people appear less confident in recalling information learned through Internet searching and that recent Internet searching may promote motivation to use the Internet 9 However several researchers have questioned whether the Google effect is a form of transactive memory arguing that no transaction is going on between the person and the computer Therefore computer networks and the Internet cannot be conceived as a distributed cognitive system Rather computers are merely tools exploited to help trigger a memory or to easily look up information Unlike in traditional transactive memory the information is not lost without the Internet but merely slower and more difficult to find 10 11 Replication edit In a big Replication study published in Nature 2018 12 the Google effect was one of the experiments which could not be replicated See also editExternal memory psychology ExocortexReferences edit a b c Study Most Americans suffer from Digital Amnesia WTOP FM July 1 2015 Retrieved November 11 2015 Krieger Lisa M July 16 2011 Google is changing your brain study says and don t you forget it San Jose Mercury News Retrieved November 12 2015 Study Finds That Memory Works Differently in the Age of Google Columbia University July 14 2011 Archived from the original on July 17 2011 a href Template Cite web html title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint bot original URL status unknown link a b c Sparrow B Liu J Wegner D M August 5 2011 Google Effects on Memory Cognitive Consequences of Having Information at Our Fingertips PDF Science 333 6043 776 778 Bibcode 2011Sci 333 776S doi 10 1126 science 1207745 PMID 21764755 S2CID 7688306 Archived from the original on April 9 2016 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint bot original URL status unknown link Meyer Dick October 12 2015 Can t recall phone numbers Blame digital amnesia Boston Herald p 15 Olson Curtis A 2012 12 01 Focused search and retrieval The impact of technology on our brains Journal of Continuing Education in the Health Professions 32 1 1 3 doi 10 1002 chp 21117 PMID 22447705 Varshney Lav R 2012 02 10 The Google effect in doctoral theses Scientometrics 92 3 785 793 doi 10 1007 s11192 012 0654 4 S2CID 12426806 Dong Guangheng Potenza Marc N 2015 10 01 Behavioural and brain responses related to Internet search and memory European Journal of Neuroscience 42 8 2546 2554 doi 10 1111 ejn 13039 PMID 26262779 S2CID 30654435 Rowlands Ian Nicholas David Williams Peter Huntington Paul Fieldhouse Maggie Gunter Barrie Withey Richard Jamali Hamid R Dobrowolski Tom Tenopir Carol 2008 The Google generation the information behaviour of the researcher of the future ASLIB Proceedings 60 4 290 310 doi 10 1108 00012530810887953 S2CID 15674801 Huebner Bryce 2016 03 01 Transactive Memory Reconstructed Rethinking Wegner s Research Program The Southern Journal of Philosophy 54 1 48 69 doi 10 1111 sjp 12160 Huebner Bryce 2013 12 01 Socially embedded cognition Cognitive Systems Research Socially Extended Cognition 25 26 13 18 doi 10 1016 j cogsys 2013 03 006 S2CID 25795227 Camerer Colin F Dreber Anna Holzmeister Felix Ho Teck Hua Huber Jurgen Johannesson Magnus Kirchler Michael Nave Gideon Nosek Brian A 2018 08 27 Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015 Nature Human Behaviour 2 9 637 644 doi 10 1038 s41562 018 0399 z ISSN 2397 3374 PMID 31346273 S2CID 52098703 External links editLink to the Kaspersky survey Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Google effect amp oldid 1209273664, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

    article

    , read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.