fbpx
Wikipedia

George Sperling

George Sperling (born 1934)[1] is an American cognitive psychologist, researcher, and educator. Sperling documented the existence of iconic memory (one of the sensory memory subtypes). Through several experiments, he showed support for his hypothesis that human beings store a perfect image of the visual world for a brief moment, before it is discarded from memory. He was in the forefront in wanting to help the deaf population in terms of speech recognition.[2] He argued that the telephone was created originally for the hearing impaired but it became popularized by the hearing community. He suggested with a sevenfold reduction in the bandwidth for video transmission, it can be useful for the improvement in American Sign Language communication.[2] Sperling used a method of partial report to measure the time course of visual persistence (sensory memory).[3]

George Sperling
Born1934 (age 89–90)
NationalityAmerican
Alma materUniversity of Michigan,
Columbia University,
Harvard University
Known forResearch in Cognitive Psychology, Iconic Memory

He is a Distinguished Professor of both Cognitive Science and Neurobiology & Behavior at the University of California, Irvine.

Education edit

In 1955, George Sperling graduated with a B.S. degree at the University of Michigan with hopes to become a scientist in one of the major scientific field such as biology, chemistry, mathematics, and physics.[1] In 1956, he went on to receive an M.A. degree in psychology from Columbia University.[3][1] His passion for physiological psychology began accidentally in university and caused him to pursue a career in cognitive psychology. He received his Ph.D. from Harvard University in 1959, and his thesis paper was focused on short-term memory.[4]

Career edit

In summer 1958, Sperling went to work at Bell Laboratories where numerous experiments were conducted. Sperling was originally attracted to psychology because he wanted to apply quantitative methods and theories used by physicists to describe the brain's mental microprocesses.[3]

In the early 1960s, George Sperling proposed a method of measuring visual persistence duration, an auditory synchronization method of measuring visual persistence duration. This approach had the synchrony of a click and the onset/termination of a light, this synchrony being judged by the subject. Later the method was innovated by with Erich Weichselgartner so that the entire rise and fall of the temporal brightness function was also measured, contrasting the initial method that only measured the moment and which visual persistence stopped.[3]

Throughout Sperling's career, he has contributed very much to the fields of visual information processing and theory and empirical research.[3] In 1960, Sperling performed an experiment using a matrix with three rows of three letters. Participants of the study were asked to look at the letters, for a brief period of time, and then recall them immediately afterwards. This technique, called free recall, showed that participants were able to, on average, recall 4–5 letters of the 9 they were given. This however, was already generally accepted in the psychological community, because it was understandable that people simply could not retain all the letters in their mind in such a brief period of time. Sperling, on the other hand, felt that they had encoded all of the letters in their mind, but had simply forgotten them while trying to recall this information on what they had seen.[5] He believed that all nine letters were stored in the viewer's memory for a short period of time, but the memory failed leading to only 4 or 5 being recalled. Sperling called this iconic memory. This was exemplified through Sperling's Iconic Memory Test, which involves having a grid of letters being flashed for 1/20 of a second. If individuals were prompted to recall a particular row immediately after the grid was shown, opposed to being asked to recall the entire grid, participants experienced higher accuracy. This procedure demonstrated that although iconic memory can store the whole grid, information tends to fade away too rapidly for a person to recall all of the information.[5] Sperling also showed this with his experiment of cued recall. This trial was similar to free recall; however, instead of allowing participants to recall any of the letters, it would allow them to view the same matrix for the same amount of time, and then hear a pitch corresponding to a different row in the matrix. The viewer was to recall the letters in that corresponding row. On average, viewers were able to recall more during cued recall trials than free recall.

Sperling built upon this experiment to then determine the amount of time before information was discarded from a person's memory. Using the same matrix, allowing viewers to see the matrix for the same amount of time, and still giving the pitches to cue the viewer which row to recall, Sperling added a twist: there would be a 5-millisecond delay after the letters disappeared before the cue would appear. The participants were unable to recall as many letters, thus showing that visual stimuli that are not added to short-term memory are discarded less than 5 milliseconds of initial introduction. (It was later agreed upon [who?] that most visual icons are eliminated from memory before 250 milliseconds.) [citation needed]

Sperling has lectured at Stanford University, University of Washington, University of Western Australia, University of London, University of California: Los Angeles, Columbia University, Duke University and New York University.[citation needed] He was elected a Fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1992.[6]

Publications edit

Sperling's first publication, "Negative Afterimages Without Prior Positive Images," was in visual psychophysics. He then went on to publish mathematical models for adaptation and flicker, contrast detection, binocular vision, and motion perception.[3]

Select representative publications edit

  • Sperling, George (1963). "A model for visual memory tasks". Human Factors. 5: 19–31. doi:10.1177/001872086300500103. PMID 13990068. S2CID 5347138.
  • Sperling, George; Novak, Stanley (1963). "Visual thresholds near a continuously visible or briefly presented light-dark boundary". Optica Acta. 10 (2): 187–191. Bibcode:1963AcOpt..10..187.. doi:10.1080/713817782. PMID 14051029.
  • Sperling, George (1967). "Successive approximations to a model for short term memory". Acta Psychologica. 27: 285–292. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(67)90070-4. PMID 6062221.
  • Sperling, George; Novak, Stanley (1967). "Increment Thresholds". Journal of the Optical Society of America (Letter). 57 (4): 542–3. doi:10.1364/JOSA.57.000542. PMID 6027834 – via OSA Publishing, The Optical Society.
  • 1968 Sperling, G., & Sondhi, M. M. (1968). Model for visual luminance discrimination and flicker detection. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 58, 1133–1145.
  • 1984 van Santen, J. P. H., & Sperling, G. (1984). Temporal covariance model of human motion perception. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics and Image Science, 1, 451–473.
  • 1985 van Santen, J. P. H., & Sperling, G. (1985). Elaborated Reichardt detectors. Journal of the Optical Society of America A: Optics and Image Science, 2, 300–321.
  • Sperling, George; Chubb, Charles (1988). "Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception". Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 5 (11): 1986–2007. Bibcode:1988JOSAA...5.1986C. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.324.3078. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.5.001986. PMID 3210090 – via OSA Publishing, The Optical Society.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Kujawski Taylor, Annette, ed. (2013). Encyclopedia of Human Memory. Vol. 3. Greenwood, ABC-CLIO LLC. p. 1069. ISBN 978-1440800269 – via Google Books.
  2. ^ a b Sperling, George (1980). "Bandwidth Requirements for Video Transmission of American Sign Language and Finger Spelling". Science. 210 (4471): 797–799. Bibcode:1980Sci...210..797S. doi:10.1126/science.7433998. PMID 7433998.
  3. ^ a b c d e f No Authorship Indicated (1989). "Awards for distinguished scientific contributions: George Sperling". American Psychologist. 44 (4): 626–628. doi:10.1037/h0092099.
  4. ^ George Sperling's Personal Statement. School of Social Sciences, retrieved December 1, 2011
  5. ^ a b Schacter, Gilbert, Wegner (2011) Psychology (2nd Edition), page 225, Worth Publishers
  6. ^ "Book of Members, 1780-2010: Chapter S" (PDF). American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved 5 May 2011.

External links edit


george, sperling, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, contain, citations, that, verify, text, please, check, citation, inaccuracies, december. This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article may contain citations that do not verify the text Please check for citation inaccuracies December 2011 Learn how and when to remove this message This biography of a living person needs additional citations for verification Please help by adding reliable sources Contentious material about living persons that is unsourced or poorly sourced must be removed immediately from the article and its talk page especially if potentially libelous Find sources George Sperling news newspapers books scholar JSTOR May 2011 Learn how and when to remove this message Learn how and when to remove this message George Sperling born 1934 1 is an American cognitive psychologist researcher and educator Sperling documented the existence of iconic memory one of the sensory memory subtypes Through several experiments he showed support for his hypothesis that human beings store a perfect image of the visual world for a brief moment before it is discarded from memory He was in the forefront in wanting to help the deaf population in terms of speech recognition 2 He argued that the telephone was created originally for the hearing impaired but it became popularized by the hearing community He suggested with a sevenfold reduction in the bandwidth for video transmission it can be useful for the improvement in American Sign Language communication 2 Sperling used a method of partial report to measure the time course of visual persistence sensory memory 3 George SperlingBorn1934 age 89 90 NationalityAmericanAlma materUniversity of Michigan Columbia University Harvard UniversityKnown forResearch in Cognitive Psychology Iconic Memory He is a Distinguished Professor of both Cognitive Science and Neurobiology amp Behavior at the University of California Irvine Contents 1 Education 2 Career 3 Publications 3 1 Select representative publications 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksEducation editIn 1955 George Sperling graduated with a B S degree at the University of Michigan with hopes to become a scientist in one of the major scientific field such as biology chemistry mathematics and physics 1 In 1956 he went on to receive an M A degree in psychology from Columbia University 3 1 His passion for physiological psychology began accidentally in university and caused him to pursue a career in cognitive psychology He received his Ph D from Harvard University in 1959 and his thesis paper was focused on short term memory 4 Career editIn summer 1958 Sperling went to work at Bell Laboratories where numerous experiments were conducted Sperling was originally attracted to psychology because he wanted to apply quantitative methods and theories used by physicists to describe the brain s mental microprocesses 3 In the early 1960s George Sperling proposed a method of measuring visual persistence duration an auditory synchronization method of measuring visual persistence duration This approach had the synchrony of a click and the onset termination of a light this synchrony being judged by the subject Later the method was innovated by with Erich Weichselgartner so that the entire rise and fall of the temporal brightness function was also measured contrasting the initial method that only measured the moment and which visual persistence stopped 3 Throughout Sperling s career he has contributed very much to the fields of visual information processing and theory and empirical research 3 In 1960 Sperling performed an experiment using a matrix with three rows of three letters Participants of the study were asked to look at the letters for a brief period of time and then recall them immediately afterwards This technique called free recall showed that participants were able to on average recall 4 5 letters of the 9 they were given This however was already generally accepted in the psychological community because it was understandable that people simply could not retain all the letters in their mind in such a brief period of time Sperling on the other hand felt that they had encoded all of the letters in their mind but had simply forgotten them while trying to recall this information on what they had seen 5 He believed that all nine letters were stored in the viewer s memory for a short period of time but the memory failed leading to only 4 or 5 being recalled Sperling called this iconic memory This was exemplified through Sperling s Iconic Memory Test which involves having a grid of letters being flashed for 1 20 of a second If individuals were prompted to recall a particular row immediately after the grid was shown opposed to being asked to recall the entire grid participants experienced higher accuracy This procedure demonstrated that although iconic memory can store the whole grid information tends to fade away too rapidly for a person to recall all of the information 5 Sperling also showed this with his experiment of cued recall This trial was similar to free recall however instead of allowing participants to recall any of the letters it would allow them to view the same matrix for the same amount of time and then hear a pitch corresponding to a different row in the matrix The viewer was to recall the letters in that corresponding row On average viewers were able to recall more during cued recall trials than free recall Sperling built upon this experiment to then determine the amount of time before information was discarded from a person s memory Using the same matrix allowing viewers to see the matrix for the same amount of time and still giving the pitches to cue the viewer which row to recall Sperling added a twist there would be a 5 millisecond delay after the letters disappeared before the cue would appear The participants were unable to recall as many letters thus showing that visual stimuli that are not added to short term memory are discarded less than 5 milliseconds of initial introduction It was later agreed upon who that most visual icons are eliminated from memory before 250 milliseconds citation needed Sperling has lectured at Stanford University University of Washington University of Western Australia University of London University of California Los Angeles Columbia University Duke University and New York University citation needed He was elected a Fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1992 6 Publications editSperling s first publication Negative Afterimages Without Prior Positive Images was in visual psychophysics He then went on to publish mathematical models for adaptation and flicker contrast detection binocular vision and motion perception 3 Select representative publications edit Sperling George 1963 A model for visual memory tasks Human Factors 5 19 31 doi 10 1177 001872086300500103 PMID 13990068 S2CID 5347138 Sperling George Novak Stanley 1963 Visual thresholds near a continuously visible or briefly presented light dark boundary Optica Acta 10 2 187 191 Bibcode 1963AcOpt 10 187 doi 10 1080 713817782 PMID 14051029 Sperling George 1967 Successive approximations to a model for short term memory Acta Psychologica 27 285 292 doi 10 1016 0001 6918 67 90070 4 PMID 6062221 Sperling George Novak Stanley 1967 Increment Thresholds Journal of the Optical Society of America Letter 57 4 542 3 doi 10 1364 JOSA 57 000542 PMID 6027834 via OSA Publishing The Optical Society 1968 Sperling G amp Sondhi M M 1968 Model for visual luminance discrimination and flicker detection Journal of the Optical Society of America 58 1133 1145 1984 van Santen J P H amp Sperling G 1984 Temporal covariance model of human motion perception Journal of the Optical Society of America A Optics and Image Science 1 451 473 1985 van Santen J P H amp Sperling G 1985 Elaborated Reichardt detectors Journal of the Optical Society of America A Optics and Image Science 2 300 321 Sperling George Chubb Charles 1988 Drift balanced random stimuli a general basis for studying non Fourier motion perception Journal of the Optical Society of America A 5 11 1986 2007 Bibcode 1988JOSAA 5 1986C CiteSeerX 10 1 1 324 3078 doi 10 1364 JOSAA 5 001986 PMID 3210090 via OSA Publishing The Optical Society See also editIconic memoryReferences edit a b c Kujawski Taylor Annette ed 2013 Encyclopedia of Human Memory Vol 3 Greenwood ABC CLIO LLC p 1069 ISBN 978 1440800269 via Google Books a b Sperling George 1980 Bandwidth Requirements for Video Transmission of American Sign Language and Finger Spelling Science 210 4471 797 799 Bibcode 1980Sci 210 797S doi 10 1126 science 7433998 PMID 7433998 a b c d e f No Authorship Indicated 1989 Awards for distinguished scientific contributions George Sperling American Psychologist 44 4 626 628 doi 10 1037 h0092099 George Sperling s Personal Statement School of Social Sciences retrieved December 1 2011 a b Schacter Gilbert Wegner 2011 Psychology 2nd Edition page 225 Worth Publishers Book of Members 1780 2010 Chapter S PDF American Academy of Arts and Sciences Retrieved 5 May 2011 External links editUC Irvine Faculty page with photo Archived 2012 07 21 at the Wayback Machine Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title George Sperling amp oldid 1181216621, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.