fbpx
Wikipedia

False confession

A false confession is an admission of guilt for a crime which the individual did not commit. Although such confessions seem counterintuitive, they can be made voluntarily, perhaps to protect a third party, or induced through coercive interrogation techniques. When some degree of coercion is involved, studies have found that subjects with highly sophisticated intelligence or manipulated by their so called "friends" are more likely to make such confessions.[1] Young people are particularly vulnerable to confessing, especially when stressed, tired, or traumatized, and have a significantly higher rate of false confessions than adults. Hundreds of innocent people have been convicted, imprisoned, and sometimes sentenced to death after confessing to crimes they did not commit—but years later, have been exonerated.[2] It was not until several shocking false confession cases were publicized in the late 1980s, combined with the introduction of DNA evidence, that the extent of wrongful convictions began to emerge—and how often false confessions played a role in these.[3]

False confessions are distinguished from forced confessions where the use of torture or the threat of physical harm is used to induce the confession.

Types

False confessions can be categorized into three general types, as outlined by American Saul Kassin in an article for Current Directions in Psychological Science:[4][5]

Voluntary false confessions

These confessions are given freely, without police prompting. Sometimes people incriminate themselves to divert attention from the actual person who committed the crime. For instance, a parent might confess to save their child from jail. Alternatively, people sometimes confess to a notorious crime because of the attention they receive from such a confession. About 250 people confessed to the 1932 kidnapping of the Lindbergh baby which received headlines around the world. Approximately 500 people confessed to the murder of Elizabeth Short (known as the "Black Dahlia") in 1947 which also received enormous media attention—some of those who confessed were not even born when she died.[6]

A more recent example of a voluntary confession occurred in 2006, when John Mark Karr confessed to the murder of six-year-old JonBenét Ramsey in the United States. Karr had become obsessed with every detail of the murder and, ten years after her death, he was extradited from Thailand based on his confession. But his account did not match details of the case, and his DNA did not match that found at the crime scene. His wife and brother also said he was home in another state at the time of the murder, and had never been to Colorado, where the murder occurred. His confession was so clearly false that prosecutors never charged him with the crime.[7]

Coerced compliant confessions

These confessions are the result of coercive interrogation techniques used by the police. Suspects may be interviewed for hours on end, sometimes without a lawyer or family member present. Even when the suspect is innocent, this creates stress and eventually leads to mental exhaustion. Sometimes, police offer inducements to suspects, telling them they will be treated more leniently if they confess. Material rewards such as coffee or the cessation of the interrogation are used to the same effect. Suspects may be told they will feel better by confessing, thereby getting the truth out in the open. After enduring this pressure, often for hours on end, vulnerable suspects may confess just to bring the process to an end.

The Reid technique codifies these strategies and is still used by many police forces in the United States. People may also confess to a crime they did not commit as a form of plea bargaining in order to avoid the risk of a harsher sentence after trial. Teenagers and young adults, individuals with mental health problems or low intelligence and those who achieve high scores high on the Gudjonsson suggestibility scale are more vulnerable to making false confessions.[8]

Coerced internalized confessions

These confessions are those in which the person is so affected by the interrogation process, they come to believe they have actually committed the crime, even though they have no memory of doing so. This seems to occur when the suspect lacks self-confidence, especially in their own memory about a particular event. Research suggests "An interrogator can take advantage of this weakness, sometimes unwittingly, through highly suggestive questioning and proffered explanations for the suspect's alleged lack of memory." The suspect is unable to detect that they are being manipulated into agreeing with something that is not true and begins to agree with the interrogator "until he or she finally comes to accept guilt".[9]

Factors involved

To the average person, the possibility that someone would confess to a crime they did not commit seems highly unlikely and makes little sense.[10] The following factors have been found to contribute to false confessions.

Police mindset

Police use persuasive manipulation techniques when conducting interrogations in hopes of obtaining a confession. These can include lying about evidence, making suspects believe they are there to help them or pretending to be the suspect's friends. After enough time and persuasion, suspects are likely to conform to the investigators' demands for a confession, even if it was to a crime they did not commit. One of the most important findings in guilt manipulation research is that once guilt is induced in the subject, it can be directed into greater compliance with requests that are completely unrelated to the original source of guilt. This has important implications for police interrogation because guilt induction is recommended in manuals on police interrogation.[11]

A 2010 study conducted by Fisher and Geiselman showed the lack of instruction given to entry-level police officers regarding the interview process. They stated in their research that, "We were discouraged to find that police often receive only minimal, and sometimes no, formal training to interview cooperative witnesses, and, not surprisingly, their actual interview practices are quite poor." While many officers may develop their own interview techniques, the lack of formal training could lead to interviewing with the purpose of simply completing the investigation, regardless of the truth. The easiest way to complete an investigation would be a confession. Fisher and Geiselman concur, saying, "It seems to be more on interrogating suspects (to elicit confessions) rather than on interviewing cooperative witnesses and victims". This study suggests that more training could prevent false confessions, and give police a new mindset while in the interview room.[12][13][14]

Reid Technique

The Reid Technique of interrogating suspects was first introduced in the United States in the 1940s and 50s by former police officer, John Reid. It was intended to replace the beatings that police frequently used to elicit information.[15] The Technique involves a nine-step process. The first step involves directly confronting the suspect with a statement that it is known that he or she committed the crime. This would usually involve frequent interruptions when the suspect tried to speak. Researchers have found that police interrogators only allowed people to speak for an average 5.8 seconds before they interrupted.[16] Often, the police lie and describe non-existent evidence that points to the suspect as the offender. In the second step, the police present a hypothesis about why the suspect committed the crime. This explanation "minimizes the moral implications of the alleged offense or allows a suspect to save face by having a morally acceptable excuse for committing the crime."[17]

The Reid Technique went on to become the leading interrogation method used by law enforcement throughout the United States and led to countless confessions. In recent years, justice researchers found that not all of those confessions were legitimate and determined that the technique primarily relies on deception, coercion and aggressive confrontation to secure confessions. Despite this, in 2014, it was still popular with police interrogators even though subjects provide less information, and the strategy provides fewer true confessions and more false confessions than less confrontational interviewing techniques.[18]

In 2017, Wicklander-Zulawski & Associates, one of the biggest consulting groups responsible for training law enforcement officers throughout the United States announced that because of its coercive methods, it would no longer use the Reid Technique.[19]

Individual vulnerability

In the Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, Richard Leo wrote: "Even though psychological coercion is the primary cause of police-induced false confessions, individuals differ in their ability to withstand interrogation pressure and thus in their susceptibility to making false confessions. All other things being equal, those who are highly suggestible or compliant are more likely to confess falsely. Individuals who are highly suggestible tend to have poor memories, high levels of anxiety, low self-esteem, and low assertiveness, personality factors that also make them more vulnerable to the pressures of interrogation and thus more likely to confess falsely. Interrogative suggestibility tends to be heightened by sleep deprivation, fatigue, and drug or alcohol withdrawal. Individuals who are highly compliant tend to be conflict avoidant, acquiescent, and eager to please others, especially authority figures."[20] In particular, this tends to apply to individuals who are intellectually impaired or suffer from mental health issues.[21]

Intellectual impairment

According to Richard Leo, the developmentally disabled are more likely to confess for a number of reasons. "First, because of their subnormal intellectual functioning, low intelligence, short attention span, poor memory, and poor conceptual and communication skills, they do not always understand statements made to them or the implications of their answers. They often lack the ability to think in a causal way about the consequences of their actions." This also affects their social intelligence. Leo says: "They are not, for example, likely to understand that the police detective who appears to be friendly is really their adversary or to grasp the long-term consequences of making an incriminating statement. They are thus highly suggestible and easy to manipulate ... (they are also) eager to please. They tend to have a high need for approval and thus are prone to being acquiescent."[22]

The case of Canadian Simon Marshall is an example and was one of Quebec's most notorious miscarriages of justice. Marshall was mentally disabled, and accused of a series of rapes in 1997. He confessed to 13 charges, and was convicted and imprisoned for five years. While in prison he was beaten, sodomized, and scalded with boiling water by other prisoners.[23] Eventually DNA testing established that Marshall was not involved in the crimes.[24] Despite being released, Marshall continues to live in a state of "semi-detention"; he is being held in a psychiatric hospital due to the psychological damage caused during his incarceration. He was awarded $2.3-million in compensation. An inquiry into the case made the point that not only had DNA testing not been done at the time of his trial, Marshall's mental handicap was entirely overlooked throughout his prosecution.[25]

Mental illness

Individuals who are mentally ill tend to have a number of symptoms which predispose them to agreeing with or confabulating false and misleading information. In The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, Richard Leo wrote that these "include faulty reality monitoring, distorted perceptions and beliefs, an inability to distinguish fact from fantasy, proneness to feelings of guilt, heightened anxiety, mood disturbances, and a lack of self control. In addition, the mentally ill may suffer from deficits in executive functioning, attention, and memory; become easily confused; and lack social skills such as assertiveness. These traits also increase the risk of falsely confessing."[26]

Youth and immaturity

Saul Kassin, a leading expert on false confessions, says that young people are also particularly vulnerable to confessing, especially when stressed, tired, or traumatized.[27] In the Central Park Jogger case, for example, five teenagers aged from 14 to 16 falsely confessed to assault and rape of a white woman in Manhattan's Central Park on April 19, 1989.[28] The police ignored the fact that none of the suspects' DNA matched two semen samples found on the victim. Both samples belonged to a single source, Matias Reyes, a violent serial rapist and murderer who finally confessed to the Central Park rape in 2002.[29]

Incidence

The incidence of false confession, and its causes, are likely to vary from one country to another. The rate also varies depending on the methodology used to measure it. Some studies only use confirmed cases where DNA proved the person who confessed was in fact innocent and has been exonerated by a court. This mostly applies to murder and rape cases. For instance, in the United States, the Innocence Project reports that since 1989, 375 offenders have been exonerated by DNA. Twenty-nine percent of them confessed to the crime for which they were convicted, but were then exonerated. As of July 2020, twenty-three of the 104 people whose cases involved false confessions had exculpatory DNA evidence available at the time of trial—but were still wrongfully convicted.[30] According to the National Registry of Exonerations in the United States, 27% of those on the registry who were accused of homicide, but were later exonerated, gave false confessions. However, 81% of people with mental illness or intellectual disabilities also confessed when accused of homicide.[31]

Offenders may also be exonerated by means other than DNA evidence. In the US, 2,750 people have been exonerated in the last three decades—9% of whom were women. Nearly 73% of women exonerated in the last three decades were convicted of crimes that never took place at all, according to data from the National Registry of Exonerations. Their alleged "crimes" included events determined to be accidents, crimes that were fabricated and deaths by suicide. About 40% of female exonerees were wrongly convicted of harming their children or other loved ones in their care.[32]

Other studies use self-report surveys where offenders are asked if they have ever falsely confessed to a crime, although there may be no way of checking the validity of such claims. These surveys apply to confessions to any kind of crime, not just rape and murder. Two Icelandic studies based on self-report conducted ten years apart found the rates of false confession to be 12.2% and 24.4% respectively. A more recent Scottish study found the rate of self-reported false confessions was 33.4%.[33]

Impact on judicial process

Leo noted that "most people assume that a confession, especially a detailed confession, is, by its very nature, true. Confession evidence therefore tends to define the case against a defendant, usually overriding any contradictory information or evidence of innocence. A suspect's confession sets in motion a seemingly irrefutable presumption of guilt among justice officials, the media, the public, and lay jurors. This chain of events in effect leads each part of the system to be stacked against the individual who confesses, and as a result he is treated more harshly at every stage of the investigative and trial process. He is significantly more likely to be incarcerated before trial, charged, pressured to plead guilty, and convicted".[34]

As Justice Brennan noted in his dissent in Colorado v. Connelly: "Reliance on confessions is due, in part, to their decisive impact upon the adversarial process. Triers of fact accord confessions such heavyweight in their determinations that 'the introduction of a confession makes the other aspects of a trial in court superfluous, and the real trial, for all practical purposes, occurs when the confession is obtained.' No other class of evidence is so profoundly prejudicial. 'Thus the decision to confess before trial amounts in effect to a waiver of the right to require the state at trial to meet its heavy burden of proof'."[35]

Leo argued that false confessions gather collective force as the judicial process proceeds, and become almost impossible to overcome. He noted that "this chain reaction starts with the police. Once they obtain a confession, they typically close their investigation, clear the case as solved, and make no effort to pursue any exculpatory evidence or other possible leads, even if the confession is internally inconsistent, contradicted by external evidence, or the result of coercive interrogation. Even when other case evidence subsequently emerges suggesting or demonstrating that the suspect’s confession is false, police almost always continue to believe in the suspect’s guilt and the underlying accuracy of the confession."[36]

Remedial strategies

Better police training

Researchers argue that the police need to be trained better at identifying the circumstances which contribute to false confessions and the type of suspects inclined to make them.[37] In the early 1990s, British psychologists collaborated with law enforcement to develop a more conversational approach to obtain information from suspects. This approach, which is more ethical and less confrontational, became known as the PEACE method of interrogation.[38]

The method has five stages: Preparation and Planning; Engage and Explain; Account, Clarification, Challenge; Closure; and Evaluation. Using this approach, investigators are not supposed to interrupt suspects while they tell their story; use open‐ended questions; and challenge any inconsistencies or contradictions after the subject has told their story. Also interviewers are not allowed to deceive or pretend they have incriminating evidence they do not actually have.[39][40]

Taping interrogations and confessions

In response to the prevalence of false confessions induced by aggressive police interrogation methods, one suggested solution has been to videotape all interrogations so that what occurred can be monitored by the legal defense team and by jury members.[41][42] This solution stems from the perception that videotaped interrogations and confessions allow for a more complete and objective record of the police–suspect interaction.[42] Those who advocate for the videotaping of interrogations argue that the presence of the camera will deter the use of coercive methods to induce confessions and will provide a visual and auditory record which can be used to evaluate the voluntariness and potential veracity of any confession.

However, a study in The Journal of Psychiatry & Law notes that videotaping on its own "will not solve the problem of false confessions occurring nor will it ensure that false confessions will be detected before an innocent life is ruined". The authors argue that "More needs to be done with regard to reforming how police go about interviewing/interrogating suspects in the first place".[43]

In the US

Until the 1980s, most confession evidence was recorded by police and later presented at trial in either a written or an audiotaped format.[44] Electronic recording of interrogations was first mandated in the United States in Alaska in 1985 by the Supreme Court of Alaska in Stephan v. State—based on the state constitution's due process clause.[43] In 2019, 21 states plus the District of Columbia require recording by law in serious cases. Many other cities have voluntarily implemented electronic recording as best practice, including Philadelphia, Boston, San Diego, San Francisco, Denver, Portland, and Austin. Electronic recording of interrogations has become mandatory in approximately 1,000 law enforcement agencies across the country.[45]

In the United Kingdom

In England and Wales, the Police and Criminal Evidence Act of 1984 built certain protections into the questioning process, including the requirement that all suspect interviews be taped.[42][46]

 
A police interrogation room

Concerns about videotaping

Camera perspective bias

Psychological research suggests that evaluations of videotaped confessions can be affected by the camera perspective used at the initial recording.[46][47][48] Extensive empirical data has been collected in this area by manipulating the position of the camera: to a suspect-focus (looking at the front of the suspect from waist up and the back of the detective's head and shoulders), detective-focus (looking at the front of the detective and the back of the suspect), and equal-focus (where the profiles of both the detective and the suspect were equally visible) perspective.[46][47][48][49] The research indicates that the camera perspective influences assessments of voluntariness, the level of coercion on the part of the detective, and even the dichotomy of guilt.[46][47][48]

Changes in camera perspective lead to changes in the visual content available to the observer.[50] Using eye-tracking as a measure and monitor of visual attention, researchers deduced that visual attention mediates the camera perspective bias.[50] That is, the correlation between camera perspective and the resulting bias is caused by the viewer's visual attention, which is decided by the focus of the camera.[50]

In the United States and in many other countries, interrogations are typically recorded with the camera positioned behind the interrogator and focused directly on the suspect.[42][44] These suspect-focus videotapes lead to the perception that the subject is participating voluntarily, when compared to both audiotapes and transcripts, which are assumed to be bias-free. In other words, the manner in which videotaping is implemented holds the potential for bias. This bias can be avoided by using an equal-focus perspective. This finding has been replicated numerous times, reflecting the growing uses of videotaped confessions in trial proceedings.[44][48]

Racial salience bias

Psychological research has explored camera perspective bias with African American and Chinese American suspects.[51] African Americans are victims to strong stereotypes linking them with criminal behavior, but these stereotypes are not prevalent towards Chinese Americans, making the two ethnicities ideal for comparison.[51] Participants were randomly assigned to view mock police interrogations developed using a male Caucasian detective questioning a Caucasian, Chinese American, or African American male suspect regarding his whereabouts at a given time and date. All interrogations were taped in an equal-focus perspective.[51] Voluntariness judgments varied as a function of the race of the suspect.[51] More participants viewing the Chinese American suspect and the African American suspect versions of the interrogation judged the suspect's statements to be voluntary than did those viewing the Caucasian suspect version.[51] Both the African American suspect and the Chinese American suspect were judged to have a higher likelihood of guilt than the Caucasian suspect.[51] Racial salience bias in videotaped interrogations is a genuine phenomenon that has been proven through empirical data.[51]

Policy recommendations

Research indicates that an equal focus perspective produces relatively unbiased assessments of videotaped interrogations.[46][48][49][52] A variation of the equal focus perspective is the dual camera approach where the subject's and the interviewer's faces are presented side-by-side. One study into this approach suggests it eliminates the usual camera perspective bias on voluntariness and guilt judgments, but was no better than the infamous suspect-focus condition in terms of its impact on the ability to accurately distinguish between true and false confession.[53]

To aid criminal-justice practitioners and legal policy makers to achieve sound and fair policy, a study in Behavioral Sciences and the Law presented the following recommendations based on the body of research:[41][53]

  • Custodial interrogations recorded in their entirety with the camera positioned so that the resultant videotape displays an equal-focus or detective-focus perspective.[41][53]
  • If an interrogation has already been videotaped from a suspect-focus perspective, it should not be used. Rather, the use of an audio track or a transcript derived from the videotape should serve in its place.[41][53]
  • The dual-camera approach is not advised because it does nothing to moderate actual accuracy of judgments.[41][53]

Cases by country

Japan

In 2007, thirteen men and women, ranging in age from their early 50s to mid-70s, were arrested and indicted in Japan for buying votes in an election. Six confessed to buying votes with liquor, cash and catered parties. All were acquitted in 2007 in a local district court, which found that the confessions had been entirely fabricated. The presiding judge said the defendants had "made confessions in despair while going through marathon questioning."[54]

Sweden

Sture Bergwall (1990)

Sture Bergwall, also known as Thomas Quick, confessed to more than 30 murders in Sweden, Norway, Denmark, and Finland while incarcerated in a mental institution for personality disorders. He had been committed after being convicted of less serious crimes. Between 1994 and 2001, Bergwall was convicted of eight murders, based on his confessions. All of these convictions have now been overturned on appeal as he was found to have made false confessions and been incompetent to stand trial.[55]

United Kingdom

Robert Hubert (1666)

In 1666, Robert Hubert confessed to starting the Great Fire of London by throwing a fire bomb through a bakery window. It was proven during his trial that he had not been in the country until two days after the start of the fire, he was never at any point near the bakery in question, the bakery did not have windows, and he was crippled and unable to throw a bomb. But, as a foreigner (a Frenchman), and a Catholic, Hubert was a perfect scapegoat. Ever-maintaining his guilt, Hubert was brought to trial, found guilty, and duly executed by hanging.[56]

Timothy Evans (1947)

Timothy Evans was accused of murdering his wife and daughter. He was subsequently tried for the murder of the daughter, was convicted and hanged. When informed about their deaths and asked if he was responsible, Evans reportedly replied "Yes".[57] He was later posthumously pardoned in 1966.

Stephen Downing (1974)

Stephen Downing was convicted and spent 27 years in prison. The main piece of evidence used against him was a confession he signed. He had agreed to this after an 8-hour interrogation which left him confused, and his poor literacy skills meant he did not fully understand what he was signing.

Stefan Kiszko (1976)

Stefan Kiszko was convicted of murder in 1976, in what was later described as "one of Britain's most notorious miscarriages of justice".[58] One of the main pieces of prosecution evidence was a confession Kiszko made after three days of police questioning. After almost 16 years in prison, Kiszko was exonerated in 1992.[further explanation needed] When asked why he had confessed to a crime he did not commit, Kiszko replied, "I started to tell these lies and they seemed to please them and the pressure was off as far as I was concerned. I thought if I admitted what I did to the police they would check out what I had said, find it untrue and would then let me go".[citations needed]

United States

Peter Reilly (1973)

In 1973, 18-year-old Peter Reilly of Litchfield County, Connecticut, was convicted of murdering his mother. He had signed a detailed confession after first discovering and informing of the crime, and then being detained and interrogated for many hours with little sleep. During this interrogation, with no lawyer present, he agreed to undergo a polygraph, which he was wrongly told he had failed, and was persuaded that only he could have committed the crime. He was sentenced to six to sixteen years for manslaughter but freed on appeal in 1976.[citation needed]

Pizza Hut murder (1988)

In 1988, Nancy DePriest was raped and murdered at the Pizza Hut where she worked in Austin, Texas. A coworker, Chris Ochoa, pleaded guilty to the murder. His friend and coworker, Richard Danziger, was convicted of the rape. Ochoa confessed to the murder, as well as implicating Danziger in the rape. The only forensic evidence linking Danziger to the crime scene was a single pubic hair found in the restaurant, which was said to be consistent with his pubic hair type. Although semen evidence had been collected, a DNA analysis of only one gene was performed at the time; even though Ochoa had this gene, it was known also to be present in 10–16% of individuals.[59] Both men received life sentences with no possibility of parole.[59]

Years later a man named Achim Marino (who was in prison serving his three-life sentences for robbery, rape and murder respectively) began writing letters from prison claiming he was the actual murderer in the Pizza Hut case and that Ocha and Danziger were innocent. He said that he had converted to Christianity while in prison and wanted to tell the truth to free Ochoa and Danziger from prison. The DNA from the crime scene was tested, and it matched that of Marino.[when?] The DNA of Ochoa and Danziger was excluded from matching that evidence. Ochoa later said that he was coerced by the police to confess and implicate his friend in the rape and murder.

In 2001, Ochoa and Danziger were exonerated and released from prison after 12 years of incarceration. While in prison, Danziger had been severely beaten by other inmates in 1991 and suffered permanent brain damage. He requires all day medical care for the rest of his life.[60] Marino was later convicted of the murder (he couldn't be charged with the rape due to the statute of limitations) and was given an additional life sentence.

Jeffrey Mark Deskovic (1990)

Jeffrey Mark Deskovic was convicted in 1990, at the age of 16, of raping, beating and strangling a high school classmate. He had confessed to the crime after hours of interrogation by police without being given an opportunity to seek legal counsel. Court testimony noted that the DNA evidence in the case did not point to him. He was incarcerated for 15 years.[further explanation needed]

Juan Rivera (1992)

Juan Rivera, from Waukegan, Illinois, was wrongfully convicted of the 1992 rape and murder of 11-year-old Holly Staker. Although his DNA was excluded from that tested in the rape kit, and the report from the electronic ankle monitor he was wearing at the time (while awaiting trial for a non-violent burglary) established that he was not in the vicinity of the murder, he confessed to the crimes. Rivera had been interrogated for several days by police using the Reid technique. His conviction was overturned in 2011, and the appellate court took the unusual step of barring prosecutors from retrying him.[61]

Rivera filed a lawsuit against a number of parties, including John E. Reid & Associates, who developed the Reid technique. Reid contended that Rivera's false confession was the result of the Reid technique being used incorrectly. Rivera was taken to Reid headquarters in Chicago twice during his interrogation for polygraph tests. These were inconclusive, but a Reid employee, Michael Masokas, told Rivera that he failed. The case was settled out of court with John E. Reid & Associates paying $2 million.[61]

Gary Gauger (1993)

Gary Gauger was sentenced to death for the murders of his parents, Morris, 74, and Ruth, 70, at their McHenry County, Illinois farm in April 1993. He was interrogated for more than 21 hours. He gave the police a hypothetical statement, which they took as a confession. His conviction was overturned in 1996, and Gauger was freed. He was pardoned by the Illinois governor in 2002. Two motorcycle gang members were later convicted of Morris and Ruth Gauger's murders.[citation needed]

West Memphis Three (1993)

The West Memphis Three (Damien Echols, Jason Baldwin, and Jessie Misskelley) were convicted for the 1993 murders of three 8-year-old boys. At the time of the alleged crime, they were 16, 17, and 18 years old. One month after the murders, police interrogated Misskelley, who has an IQ of 72, for five hours. He confessed to the murders and implicated both Echols and Baldwin.

Misskelley immediately recanted and said he was coerced to confess. Although his confession contained massive internal inconsistencies and differed significantly from the facts of the physical evidence revealed, the prosecution continued. Misskelley and Baldwin were convicted of murder and sentenced to life in prison without parole; Echols was convicted and sentenced to death.

For the next 17 years, the three men maintained their innocence. In August 2011, testing of DNA evidence was found to be inconclusive; it included DNA from an unknown contributor.

Prosecutors offered the three men a deal if they pleaded guilty: to release them for time served. They accepted the Alford plea but said that they would continue to work to clear their names and find the real murderer(s). They were released after eighteen years in prison.[citation needed]

Norfolk Four (1997)

Danial Williams, Joseph J. Dick Jr., Derek Tice, and Eric C. Wilson are four of five men convicted in the 1997 rape and murder of Michelle Moore-Bosko in Norfolk, Virginia. The convictions of the four were based largely on their confessions, which they have since maintained were coerced after hours of interrogation, during which the men were played off against each other over time. The Mid-Atlantic Innocence Project considers this a miscarriage of justice.[62] Moore-Bosko's parents continue to believe that all those convicted were participants in the crime.[63]

Williams and Dick pleaded guilty to murder, as they had been threatened by the potential of being sentenced to death at a jury trial. They were sentenced to one or more life sentences in prison without the possibility of parole. Tice was convicted of rape and murder and sentenced to death. Wilson was convicted of rape and sentenced to 8½ years in prison. Three other men, Geoffrey A. Farris, John E. Danser and Richard D. Pauley Jr., were also initially indicted with the crime through accusations by others, but their charges were later dropped before trial as Tice would not testify against them. The supporters of the Norfolk Four have offered evidence that purports to prove that the four men are innocent, with no known involvement or connections to the incident. No physical evidence supported their cases.[64] Tice's conviction was overturned, and Williams and Dick received governmental pardons, clearing their names. The four received a settlement from the city of Norfolk and state in 2018.

The fifth man, Omar Ballard, was indicted in 2005 after his DNA was found to match that at the crime scene. He had informally confessed in 1997 but withdrew his statement after being pressured to implicate the four other men. He pleaded guilty to the crime in 2009 in order to avoid the death penalty. A serial rapist and murderer, he was apprehended and sentenced to prison after he pleaded guilty to other crimes of violence against women, and confessed to acting alone. He was sentenced to 100 years in prison, 59 of which were suspended. He is the only man whose DNA matched that found at the scene. He confessed to committing the crime by himself, and said none of the other men indicted and tried were involved. Forensic evidence is consistent with his story that there were no other participants.

Michael Crowe (1998)

Michael Crowe confessed to the murder of his younger sister Stephanie Crowe in 1998. Michael, 14 at the time, was targeted by the police when he seemed "distant and preoccupied" after Stephanie's body was discovered, and the rest of the family grieved. After two days of intense questioning, Michael admitted to killing Stephanie. His confession was vague and lacked detail; he said he could not remember committing the crime but believed he must have done so based on what the police were telling him. The confession was videotaped by police and showed Michael making statements to the effect of, "I'm only saying this because it's what you want to hear". His admission has been cited as a classic example of a coerced false confession during police interrogation.[65]

Joshua Treadway, a friend of Michael's, was questioned and gave a detailed confession after many hours of interrogation. Aaron Houser, a mutual friend of the boys, was questioned and did not confess but presented a "hypothetical" and incriminating account of the crime under prompting by police interrogators using the Reid Technique. All three boys subsequently recanted their statements, claiming coercion.

Crowe's confession and Houser's statements to police were later thrown out as coerced by a judge; part of Treadway's confession was also ruled inadmissible. Later all charges were dropped against each of the three boys. Prosecutors later charged an unrelated party with the crime. His defense team argued that the three boys who were first charged had been responsible.

The charges against the three boys were dismissed without prejudice (which would allow charges to be reinstated at a later date) after DNA testing linked a neighborhood transient, Richard Tuite, to Stephanie's blood. Embarrassed by the reversal, the Escondido police and the San Diego County District Attorney let the case languish without charges for two years. In 2001 the District Attorney and San Diego County Sheriff's Department asked that the case be taken over by the California Department of Justice.[66]

Tuite was convicted of the murder in 2004, but the conviction was overturned.[why?] At the second trial in 2013, the jury found him not guilty. The murder of Stephanie Crowe remains unsolved.[67] In 2012, Superior Court Judge Kenneth So made the rare ruling that Michael Crowe, Treadway, and Houser were factually innocent of the charges, permanently dismissing the City of Escondido case against them.[68]

A TV movie was made about the case called The Interrogation of Michael Crowe (2002).[69]

Corethian Bell (2000)

In 2000, Corethian Bell, who has a diagnosis of mental retardation, was accused of murdering his mother, Netta Bell, after he had found her body and called police in Cook County, Illinois. Police questioned him for more than 50 hours. He said he eventually confessed to the murder of his mother because police hit him so hard he was knocked off his chair, and because he thought that if he confessed, the interrogations would stop. He believed that he would be able to explain himself to a judge and be set free. His confession was videotaped, but his interrogation was not. At the time Cook County prosecutors were required to videotape murder confessions, but not the preceding interrogations. With his confession on tape, Bell was tried, convicted, and sentenced to jail.

When the DNA at the crime scene was finally tested a year later, it matched that of a serial rapist named DeShawn Boyd. He was already in prison after having been convicted of three other violent sexual assaults, all in the same neighborhood as the Netta Bell murder. Bell filed a civil lawsuit with the help of Herschella Conyers and her University of Chicago Law School students, which was settled by the city in 2006 for $1 million.[70]

Kevin Fox (2004)

Kevin Fox was interrogated for 14 hours by Will County, Illinois, police before he confessed to the 2004 murder of his 3-year-old daughter, Riley. He was convicted and sentenced to jail. His confession was later ruled to have been coerced. Because of DNA testing, police later identified Scott Eby as the killer. He was a neighbor living a few miles from the Fox family at the time of Riley's murder. Police identified him as the killer while he was serving a 14-year sentence for sex crimes. After questioning and confrontation with the DNA results, Eby confessed and later pleaded guilty.

Kevin Fox was released after serving eight months in jail. The Fox family eventually won an $8 million civil judgment against the county government.[71]

Laverne Pavlinac (1990)

Laverne Pavlinac confessed that she and her boyfriend murdered a woman in Oregon in 1990. They were convicted and sentenced to prison. Five years later, Keith Hunter Jesperson confessed to a series of murders, including that of the woman. Pavlinac had become obsessed with details of the crime during interrogation by police. She later said she confessed to get out of the abusive relationship with the boyfriend. Her boyfriend purportedly confessed in order to avoid the death penalty.[citation needed]

See also

References

  1. ^ Cooley, M., Craig, and Turvey E. Brent. Miscarriages of Justice: Actual Innocence, Forensic Evidence, and the Law. 1st ed. Academic Press, 2014. p116
  2. ^ Kassin SM. False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications for Reform. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2014;1(1):112–121. doi:10.1177/2372732214548678
  3. ^ This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn’t commit, Science, 13 June 2019
  4. ^ Kassin, Saul M. (2008). "False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications for Reform" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (4): 249–253. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00584.x. S2CID 3789032.
  5. ^ Douglas Starr (13 June 2019). "This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn't commit". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aay3537. S2CID 197717147.
  6. ^ False Confessions and Tips Still Flow in Simpson Case, Los Angeles Times, 25 March 1996
  7. ^ The Psychology of False Confessions, Cale Law Office, 18 April 2018
  8. ^ False confessions: How can psychology so basic be so counterintuitive? Paul Kassin, American Psychologist, 2017, vol. 72, no. 9, p. 953
  9. ^ Frances Chapman, Coerced Internalized False Confessions And Police Interrogations: The Power Of Coercion,
  10. ^ Kassin, False Confessions: How Can Psychology So Basic Be So Counterintuitive? American Psychologist 2017, vol. 72, no. 9, 951–964
  11. ^ Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions. p.7.
  12. ^ Fisher, R.P.; Geiselman, E.R. (2010). "The Cognitive Interview method of conducting police interviews: Eliciting extensive information and promoting Therapeutic Jurisprudence". International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 33 (5–6): 5–6. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2010.09.004. PMID 20875685.
  13. ^ Kassin, S. M.; Drizin, S. A.; Grisso, T.; Gudjonsson, G. H.; Leo, R. A.; Redlich, A. D. (2010). "Police-induced confessions, risk factors, and recommendations: Looking ahead". Law and Human Behavior. 34 (1): 49–52. doi:10.1007/s10979-010-9217-5. PMID 20112057. S2CID 189389.
  14. ^ Gudjonsson, G. H.; Pearse, J. (2011). "Suspect Interviews and False Confessions". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20: 33–37. doi:10.1177/0963721410396824. S2CID 145624273.
  15. ^ The Seismic Change in Police Interrogations, Marshall Project, 3 July 2017.
  16. ^ Jackie Adams, Investigative Interviewing, Slideshow presentation for the New Zealand Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment.
  17. ^ Reid Technique for Interrogations, Psychology
  18. ^ Aldert Vrij (2019). Deception and truth detection when analyzing nonverbal and verbal cues, Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 33, pp. 160-167
  19. ^ Leading Police Consulting Group Will No Longer Teach the Reid Technique, Innocence Project, 3 August 2017
  20. ^ False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, September 2009, vol. 37(3), pp. 332–343;
  21. ^ False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, September 2009, vol. 37(3), pp. 332–343;
  22. ^ Richard Leo, False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 37:332– 43, 2009.
  23. ^ Mentally handicapped Quebec man receives millions for injustice, The Globe and Mail, 21 December 2006
  24. ^ . Canada.com. 29 April 2008. Archived from the original on 23 September 2015.
  25. ^ Mentally handicapped Quebec man receives millions for injustice, The Globe and Mail, 21 December 2006
  26. ^ Richard Leo, False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 37:332–343, 2009.
  27. ^ This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn’t commit, Science 13 June 2019
  28. ^ False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online September 2009, 37 (3) 332–343
  29. ^ Donald Trump and the Central Park Five: the racially charged rise of a demagogue, The Guardian, 17 February 2016
  30. ^ DNA Exonerations in the United States, Innocence Project
  31. ^ Examining why false confessions occur in the U.S. criminal justice system, Washington Post, 23 June 2019
  32. ^ 8 Facts About Incarcerated and Wrongfully Convicted Women You Should Know, Innocence Project
  33. ^ The Risk of Making False Confessions: The Role of Developmental Disorders, Conduct Disorder, Psychiatric Symptoms, and Compliance, Gisli Hannes Gudjonsson, Rafael A. Gonzalez, & Susan Young. Journal of Attention Disorders 2021, vol. 25(5) 715–723
  34. ^ False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, September 2009, vol. 37(3), pp. 332-343;
  35. ^ Colorado v. Connelly, 49 U.S. 157 (1986)
  36. ^ False Confessions: Causes, Consequences, and Implications, Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, September 2009, vol. 37(3), pp. 332–343;
  37. ^ Strategies for Preventing False Confessions and Their Consequences (2006), Mark R. Kebbell and Graham M. Davies, eds., Wiley & Sons. Univ. of San Francisco Law Research Paper No. 2011-20
  38. ^ P.E.A.C.E. A Different Approach to Investigative Interviewing, Forensic Interview Solutions
  39. ^ James Orlando, Interrogation techniques
  40. ^ C.A. Meissner, A.D. Redlich, S, Bhatt & S. Brandon. Interview and interrogation methods and their effects on true and false confessions, 01 September 2012
  41. ^ a b c d e Lassiter, G. Daniel; Lindberg, M. (2010). "Video recording custodial interrogations: Implications of psychological science for policy and practice". The Journal of Psychiatry & Law. 38 (1–2): 177–192. doi:10.1177/009318531003800108. S2CID 146850991.
  42. ^ a b c d Kassin, Saul M. (1 January 1997). "The psychology of confession evidence". American Psychologist. 52 (3): 221–233. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.3.221.
  43. ^ a b Video recording custodial interrogations: Implications of psychological science for policy and practice, The Journal of Psychiatry & Law, vol. 38/Spring–Summer 2010.
  44. ^ a b c Geller, W.A. (1992). "Police videotaping of suspect interrogations and confessions: A preliminary explanation of issues and practices". (a Report to the National Institute of Justice). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
  45. ^ National Organizations - Recording Custodial Interrogations February 25, 2019
  46. ^ a b c d e Lassiter, G. Daniel; Geers, Andrew L.; Handley, Ian M.; Weiland, Paul E.; Munhall, Patrick J. (1 January 2002). "Videotaped interrogations and confessions: A simple change in camera perspective alters verdicts in simulated trials". Journal of Applied Psychology. 87 (5): 867–874. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.5.867. PMID 12395811.
  47. ^ a b c Daniel Lassiter, G.; Diamond, S. S.; Schmidt, H. C.; Elek, J. K. (1 March 2007). "Evaluating Videotaped Confessions: Expertise Provides No Defense Against the Camera-Perspective Effect". Psychological Science. 18 (3): 224–226. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01879.x. PMID 17444917. S2CID 722865.
  48. ^ a b c d e Lassiter, G. Daniel; Slaw, R. David; Briggs, Michael A.; Scanlan, Carla R. (1 December 1992). "The Potential for Bias in Videotaped Confessions1". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 22 (23): 1838–1851. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00980.x.
  49. ^ a b Lassiter, G. Daniel; Ware, Lezlee J.; Ratcliff, Jennifer J.; Irvin, Clinton R. (1 February 2009). "Evidence of the camera perspective bias in authentic videotaped interrogations: Implications for emerging reform in the criminal justice system". Legal and Criminological Psychology. 14 (1): 157–170. doi:10.1348/135532508X284293.
  50. ^ a b c Ware, Lezlee J.; Lassiter, G. Daniel; Patterson, Stephen M.; Ransom, Michael R. (1 January 2008). "Camera perspective bias in videotaped confessions: Evidence that visual attention is a mediator". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 14 (2): 192–200. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.14.2.192. PMID 18590374.
  51. ^ a b c d e f g Ratcliff, Jennifer J.; Lassiter, G. Daniel; Jager, Victoria M.; Lindberg, Matthew J.; Elek, Jennifer K.; Hasinski, Adam E. (1 January 2010). "The hidden consequences of racial salience in videotaped interrogations and confessions". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 16 (2): 200–218. doi:10.1037/a0018482.
  52. ^ Ratcliff, Jennifer J.; Lassiter, G. Daniel; Schmidt, Heather C.; Snyder, Celeste J. (1 January 2006). "Camera perspective bias in videotaped confessions: Experimental evidence of its perceptual basis". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 12 (4): 197–206. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.12.4.197. PMID 17154769.
  53. ^ a b c d e Snyder, Celeste J.; Lassiter, G. Daniel; Lindberg, Matthew J.; Pinegar, Shannon K. (1 May 2009). "Videotaped interrogations and confessions: does a dual-camera approach yield unbiased and accurate evaluations?". Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 27 (3): 451–466. doi:10.1002/bsl.875. PMID 19387972.
  54. ^ Onishi, Norimitsu (11 May 2007). "Pressed by Police, Even Innocent Confess in Japan". The New York Times. Retrieved 11 May 2007.
  55. ^ Stridbeck, Ulf (28 February 2020). "Coerced-Reactive Confessions: The Case of Thomas Quick". Journal of Forensic Psychology Research and Practice. 20, 2020 (4): 305–322. doi:10.1080/24732850.2020.1732758.
  56. ^ Juan, Dr Stephen (1 September 2006). "What is a Confessing Sam?". The Register. Situation Publishing. Retrieved 14 December 2018.
  57. ^ Kennedy, Ludovich (1961). Ten Rillingtom Place. Berkley Books. p. 103. ISBN 978-0586034286.
  58. ^ "Second victim of Molseed inquiry". BBC News. 12 November 2007. Retrieved 29 April 2021.
  59. ^ a b Forensic Files, "Forever Hold Your Peace"
  60. ^ "Burden of Innocence: Richard Danziger". Frontline. PBS. Retrieved 13 December 2013.
  61. ^ a b Starr, Douglas (22 May 2015). "Juan Rivera and the Dangers of Coercive Interrogation". The New Yorker.
  62. ^ "Mid-Atlantic Innocence Project " Blog Archive " Norfolk Four". Retrieved 3 May 2015.
  63. ^ Jackman, Tom (15 December 2008). "Clemency Campaign Renews Misery". The Washington Post. Retrieved 25 May 2010.
  64. ^ "Dereck Tice". Retrieved 3 May 2015.
  65. ^ Bell, Rachael. . Crime Library. Archived from the original on 12 November 2013. Retrieved 12 November 2013.
  66. ^ "Attorney General's Office to Review Stephanie Crowe Murder Investigation". Office of the Attorney General. State of California Department of Justice. 29 June 2001. Retrieved 12 November 2013.
  67. ^ Sauer, Mark (6 December 2013). "San Diego Jury Finds Richard Tuite Not Guilty in Retrial for the Murder of Stephanie Crowe". KPBS. Retrieved 6 December 2013.
  68. ^ Sauer, Mark (22 May 2012). "Michael Crowe Found 'Factually Innocent' In Sister's Murder". KPBS. Retrieved 20 October 2012.
  69. ^ "The Interrogation of Michael Crowe". IMDb. Retrieved 12 November 2013.
  70. ^ "Corethian Bell v. Chicago Police Department". MacArthur Justice Center. Northwestern University Law School. 11 October 2006. Retrieved 13 December 2013.
  71. ^ Schmadeke, Steve (3 February 2012). "A belated reward for Riley Fox murder lead". Chicago Tribune. Retrieved 12 December 2013.

External links

  • The Confessions at Frontline
  • NPR: False Confession
  • Slate: Why make a false confession?
  • Psychology Today: False Confession
  • Chicago Tribune: Cops urged to tape their interrogations
  • The Register: What is a Confessing Sam?
  • The Truth About False Confessions
  • Justice Denied: False Confessions Are Alive and Well in the U.S.
  • The radio drama "Forced Confession", from Destination Freedom, depicts an American case

false, confession, also, forced, confession, false, confession, admission, guilt, crime, which, individual, commit, although, such, confessions, seem, counterintuitive, they, made, voluntarily, perhaps, protect, third, party, induced, through, coercive, interr. See also Forced confession A false confession is an admission of guilt for a crime which the individual did not commit Although such confessions seem counterintuitive they can be made voluntarily perhaps to protect a third party or induced through coercive interrogation techniques When some degree of coercion is involved studies have found that subjects with highly sophisticated intelligence or manipulated by their so called friends are more likely to make such confessions 1 Young people are particularly vulnerable to confessing especially when stressed tired or traumatized and have a significantly higher rate of false confessions than adults Hundreds of innocent people have been convicted imprisoned and sometimes sentenced to death after confessing to crimes they did not commit but years later have been exonerated 2 It was not until several shocking false confession cases were publicized in the late 1980s combined with the introduction of DNA evidence that the extent of wrongful convictions began to emerge and how often false confessions played a role in these 3 False confessions are distinguished from forced confessions where the use of torture or the threat of physical harm is used to induce the confession Contents 1 Types 1 1 Voluntary false confessions 1 2 Coerced compliant confessions 1 3 Coerced internalized confessions 2 Factors involved 2 1 Police mindset 2 2 Reid Technique 2 3 Individual vulnerability 2 3 1 Intellectual impairment 2 3 2 Mental illness 2 3 3 Youth and immaturity 3 Incidence 4 Impact on judicial process 5 Remedial strategies 5 1 Better police training 5 2 Taping interrogations and confessions 5 2 1 In the US 5 2 2 In the United Kingdom 5 3 Concerns about videotaping 5 3 1 Camera perspective bias 5 3 2 Racial salience bias 5 4 Policy recommendations 6 Cases by country 6 1 Japan 6 2 Sweden 6 2 1 Sture Bergwall 1990 6 3 United Kingdom 6 3 1 Robert Hubert 1666 6 3 2 Timothy Evans 1947 6 3 3 Stephen Downing 1974 6 3 4 Stefan Kiszko 1976 6 4 United States 6 4 1 Peter Reilly 1973 6 4 2 Pizza Hut murder 1988 6 4 3 Jeffrey Mark Deskovic 1990 6 4 4 Juan Rivera 1992 6 4 5 Gary Gauger 1993 6 4 6 West Memphis Three 1993 6 4 7 Norfolk Four 1997 6 4 8 Michael Crowe 1998 6 4 9 Corethian Bell 2000 6 4 10 Kevin Fox 2004 6 4 11 Laverne Pavlinac 1990 7 See also 8 References 9 External linksTypes EditFalse confessions can be categorized into three general types as outlined by American Saul Kassin in an article for Current Directions in Psychological Science 4 5 Voluntary false confessions Edit These confessions are given freely without police prompting Sometimes people incriminate themselves to divert attention from the actual person who committed the crime For instance a parent might confess to save their child from jail Alternatively people sometimes confess to a notorious crime because of the attention they receive from such a confession About 250 people confessed to the 1932 kidnapping of the Lindbergh baby which received headlines around the world Approximately 500 people confessed to the murder of Elizabeth Short known as the Black Dahlia in 1947 which also received enormous media attention some of those who confessed were not even born when she died 6 A more recent example of a voluntary confession occurred in 2006 when John Mark Karr confessed to the murder of six year old JonBenet Ramsey in the United States Karr had become obsessed with every detail of the murder and ten years after her death he was extradited from Thailand based on his confession But his account did not match details of the case and his DNA did not match that found at the crime scene His wife and brother also said he was home in another state at the time of the murder and had never been to Colorado where the murder occurred His confession was so clearly false that prosecutors never charged him with the crime 7 Coerced compliant confessions Edit These confessions are the result of coercive interrogation techniques used by the police Suspects may be interviewed for hours on end sometimes without a lawyer or family member present Even when the suspect is innocent this creates stress and eventually leads to mental exhaustion Sometimes police offer inducements to suspects telling them they will be treated more leniently if they confess Material rewards such as coffee or the cessation of the interrogation are used to the same effect Suspects may be told they will feel better by confessing thereby getting the truth out in the open After enduring this pressure often for hours on end vulnerable suspects may confess just to bring the process to an end The Reid technique codifies these strategies and is still used by many police forces in the United States People may also confess to a crime they did not commit as a form of plea bargaining in order to avoid the risk of a harsher sentence after trial Teenagers and young adults individuals with mental health problems or low intelligence and those who achieve high scores high on the Gudjonsson suggestibility scale are more vulnerable to making false confessions 8 Coerced internalized confessions Edit These confessions are those in which the person is so affected by the interrogation process they come to believe they have actually committed the crime even though they have no memory of doing so This seems to occur when the suspect lacks self confidence especially in their own memory about a particular event Research suggests An interrogator can take advantage of this weakness sometimes unwittingly through highly suggestive questioning and proffered explanations for the suspect s alleged lack of memory The suspect is unable to detect that they are being manipulated into agreeing with something that is not true and begins to agree with the interrogator until he or she finally comes to accept guilt 9 Factors involved EditTo the average person the possibility that someone would confess to a crime they did not commit seems highly unlikely and makes little sense 10 The following factors have been found to contribute to false confessions Police mindset Edit Police use persuasive manipulation techniques when conducting interrogations in hopes of obtaining a confession These can include lying about evidence making suspects believe they are there to help them or pretending to be the suspect s friends After enough time and persuasion suspects are likely to conform to the investigators demands for a confession even if it was to a crime they did not commit One of the most important findings in guilt manipulation research is that once guilt is induced in the subject it can be directed into greater compliance with requests that are completely unrelated to the original source of guilt This has important implications for police interrogation because guilt induction is recommended in manuals on police interrogation 11 A 2010 study conducted by Fisher and Geiselman showed the lack of instruction given to entry level police officers regarding the interview process They stated in their research that We were discouraged to find that police often receive only minimal and sometimes no formal training to interview cooperative witnesses and not surprisingly their actual interview practices are quite poor While many officers may develop their own interview techniques the lack of formal training could lead to interviewing with the purpose of simply completing the investigation regardless of the truth The easiest way to complete an investigation would be a confession Fisher and Geiselman concur saying It seems to be more on interrogating suspects to elicit confessions rather than on interviewing cooperative witnesses and victims This study suggests that more training could prevent false confessions and give police a new mindset while in the interview room 12 13 14 Reid Technique Edit The Reid Technique of interrogating suspects was first introduced in the United States in the 1940s and 50s by former police officer John Reid It was intended to replace the beatings that police frequently used to elicit information 15 The Technique involves a nine step process The first step involves directly confronting the suspect with a statement that it is known that he or she committed the crime This would usually involve frequent interruptions when the suspect tried to speak Researchers have found that police interrogators only allowed people to speak for an average 5 8 seconds before they interrupted 16 Often the police lie and describe non existent evidence that points to the suspect as the offender In the second step the police present a hypothesis about why the suspect committed the crime This explanation minimizes the moral implications of the alleged offense or allows a suspect to save face by having a morally acceptable excuse for committing the crime 17 The Reid Technique went on to become the leading interrogation method used by law enforcement throughout the United States and led to countless confessions In recent years justice researchers found that not all of those confessions were legitimate and determined that the technique primarily relies on deception coercion and aggressive confrontation to secure confessions Despite this in 2014 it was still popular with police interrogators even though subjects provide less information and the strategy provides fewer true confessions and more false confessions than less confrontational interviewing techniques 18 In 2017 Wicklander Zulawski amp Associates one of the biggest consulting groups responsible for training law enforcement officers throughout the United States announced that because of its coercive methods it would no longer use the Reid Technique 19 Individual vulnerability Edit In the Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Richard Leo wrote Even though psychological coercion is the primary cause of police induced false confessions individuals differ in their ability to withstand interrogation pressure and thus in their susceptibility to making false confessions All other things being equal those who are highly suggestible or compliant are more likely to confess falsely Individuals who are highly suggestible tend to have poor memories high levels of anxiety low self esteem and low assertiveness personality factors that also make them more vulnerable to the pressures of interrogation and thus more likely to confess falsely Interrogative suggestibility tends to be heightened by sleep deprivation fatigue and drug or alcohol withdrawal Individuals who are highly compliant tend to be conflict avoidant acquiescent and eager to please others especially authority figures 20 In particular this tends to apply to individuals who are intellectually impaired or suffer from mental health issues 21 Intellectual impairment Edit According to Richard Leo the developmentally disabled are more likely to confess for a number of reasons First because of their subnormal intellectual functioning low intelligence short attention span poor memory and poor conceptual and communication skills they do not always understand statements made to them or the implications of their answers They often lack the ability to think in a causal way about the consequences of their actions This also affects their social intelligence Leo says They are not for example likely to understand that the police detective who appears to be friendly is really their adversary or to grasp the long term consequences of making an incriminating statement They are thus highly suggestible and easy to manipulate they are also eager to please They tend to have a high need for approval and thus are prone to being acquiescent 22 The case of Canadian Simon Marshall is an example and was one of Quebec s most notorious miscarriages of justice Marshall was mentally disabled and accused of a series of rapes in 1997 He confessed to 13 charges and was convicted and imprisoned for five years While in prison he was beaten sodomized and scalded with boiling water by other prisoners 23 Eventually DNA testing established that Marshall was not involved in the crimes 24 Despite being released Marshall continues to live in a state of semi detention he is being held in a psychiatric hospital due to the psychological damage caused during his incarceration He was awarded 2 3 million in compensation An inquiry into the case made the point that not only had DNA testing not been done at the time of his trial Marshall s mental handicap was entirely overlooked throughout his prosecution 25 Mental illness Edit Individuals who are mentally ill tend to have a number of symptoms which predispose them to agreeing with or confabulating false and misleading information In The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Richard Leo wrote that these include faulty reality monitoring distorted perceptions and beliefs an inability to distinguish fact from fantasy proneness to feelings of guilt heightened anxiety mood disturbances and a lack of self control In addition the mentally ill may suffer from deficits in executive functioning attention and memory become easily confused and lack social skills such as assertiveness These traits also increase the risk of falsely confessing 26 Youth and immaturity Edit Saul Kassin a leading expert on false confessions says that young people are also particularly vulnerable to confessing especially when stressed tired or traumatized 27 In the Central Park Jogger case for example five teenagers aged from 14 to 16 falsely confessed to assault and rape of a white woman in Manhattan s Central Park on April 19 1989 28 The police ignored the fact that none of the suspects DNA matched two semen samples found on the victim Both samples belonged to a single source Matias Reyes a violent serial rapist and murderer who finally confessed to the Central Park rape in 2002 29 Incidence EditThe incidence of false confession and its causes are likely to vary from one country to another The rate also varies depending on the methodology used to measure it Some studies only use confirmed cases where DNA proved the person who confessed was in fact innocent and has been exonerated by a court This mostly applies to murder and rape cases For instance in the United States the Innocence Project reports that since 1989 375 offenders have been exonerated by DNA Twenty nine percent of them confessed to the crime for which they were convicted but were then exonerated As of July 2020 twenty three of the 104 people whose cases involved false confessions had exculpatory DNA evidence available at the time of trial but were still wrongfully convicted 30 According to the National Registry of Exonerations in the United States 27 of those on the registry who were accused of homicide but were later exonerated gave false confessions However 81 of people with mental illness or intellectual disabilities also confessed when accused of homicide 31 Offenders may also be exonerated by means other than DNA evidence In the US 2 750 people have been exonerated in the last three decades 9 of whom were women Nearly 73 of women exonerated in the last three decades were convicted of crimes that never took place at all according to data from the National Registry of Exonerations Their alleged crimes included events determined to be accidents crimes that were fabricated and deaths by suicide About 40 of female exonerees were wrongly convicted of harming their children or other loved ones in their care 32 Other studies use self report surveys where offenders are asked if they have ever falsely confessed to a crime although there may be no way of checking the validity of such claims These surveys apply to confessions to any kind of crime not just rape and murder Two Icelandic studies based on self report conducted ten years apart found the rates of false confession to be 12 2 and 24 4 respectively A more recent Scottish study found the rate of self reported false confessions was 33 4 33 Impact on judicial process EditLeo noted that most people assume that a confession especially a detailed confession is by its very nature true Confession evidence therefore tends to define the case against a defendant usually overriding any contradictory information or evidence of innocence A suspect s confession sets in motion a seemingly irrefutable presumption of guilt among justice officials the media the public and lay jurors This chain of events in effect leads each part of the system to be stacked against the individual who confesses and as a result he is treated more harshly at every stage of the investigative and trial process He is significantly more likely to be incarcerated before trial charged pressured to plead guilty and convicted 34 As Justice Brennan noted in his dissent in Colorado v Connelly Reliance on confessions is due in part to their decisive impact upon the adversarial process Triers of fact accord confessions such heavyweight in their determinations that the introduction of a confession makes the other aspects of a trial in court superfluous and the real trial for all practical purposes occurs when the confession is obtained No other class of evidence is so profoundly prejudicial Thus the decision to confess before trial amounts in effect to a waiver of the right to require the state at trial to meet its heavy burden of proof 35 Leo argued that false confessions gather collective force as the judicial process proceeds and become almost impossible to overcome He noted that this chain reaction starts with the police Once they obtain a confession they typically close their investigation clear the case as solved and make no effort to pursue any exculpatory evidence or other possible leads even if the confession is internally inconsistent contradicted by external evidence or the result of coercive interrogation Even when other case evidence subsequently emerges suggesting or demonstrating that the suspect s confession is false police almost always continue to believe in the suspect s guilt and the underlying accuracy of the confession 36 Remedial strategies EditBetter police training Edit Researchers argue that the police need to be trained better at identifying the circumstances which contribute to false confessions and the type of suspects inclined to make them 37 In the early 1990s British psychologists collaborated with law enforcement to develop a more conversational approach to obtain information from suspects This approach which is more ethical and less confrontational became known as the PEACE method of interrogation 38 The method has five stages Preparation and Planning Engage and Explain Account Clarification Challenge Closure and Evaluation Using this approach investigators are not supposed to interrupt suspects while they tell their story use open ended questions and challenge any inconsistencies or contradictions after the subject has told their story Also interviewers are not allowed to deceive or pretend they have incriminating evidence they do not actually have 39 40 Taping interrogations and confessions Edit In response to the prevalence of false confessions induced by aggressive police interrogation methods one suggested solution has been to videotape all interrogations so that what occurred can be monitored by the legal defense team and by jury members 41 42 This solution stems from the perception that videotaped interrogations and confessions allow for a more complete and objective record of the police suspect interaction 42 Those who advocate for the videotaping of interrogations argue that the presence of the camera will deter the use of coercive methods to induce confessions and will provide a visual and auditory record which can be used to evaluate the voluntariness and potential veracity of any confession However a study in The Journal of Psychiatry amp Law notes that videotaping on its own will not solve the problem of false confessions occurring nor will it ensure that false confessions will be detected before an innocent life is ruined The authors argue that More needs to be done with regard to reforming how police go about interviewing interrogating suspects in the first place 43 In the US Edit Until the 1980s most confession evidence was recorded by police and later presented at trial in either a written or an audiotaped format 44 Electronic recording of interrogations was first mandated in the United States in Alaska in 1985 by the Supreme Court of Alaska in Stephan v State based on the state constitution s due process clause 43 In 2019 21 states plus the District of Columbia require recording by law in serious cases Many other cities have voluntarily implemented electronic recording as best practice including Philadelphia Boston San Diego San Francisco Denver Portland and Austin Electronic recording of interrogations has become mandatory in approximately 1 000 law enforcement agencies across the country 45 In the United Kingdom Edit In England and Wales the Police and Criminal Evidence Act of 1984 built certain protections into the questioning process including the requirement that all suspect interviews be taped 42 46 A police interrogation room Concerns about videotaping Edit Camera perspective bias Edit Psychological research suggests that evaluations of videotaped confessions can be affected by the camera perspective used at the initial recording 46 47 48 Extensive empirical data has been collected in this area by manipulating the position of the camera to a suspect focus looking at the front of the suspect from waist up and the back of the detective s head and shoulders detective focus looking at the front of the detective and the back of the suspect and equal focus where the profiles of both the detective and the suspect were equally visible perspective 46 47 48 49 The research indicates that the camera perspective influences assessments of voluntariness the level of coercion on the part of the detective and even the dichotomy of guilt 46 47 48 Changes in camera perspective lead to changes in the visual content available to the observer 50 Using eye tracking as a measure and monitor of visual attention researchers deduced that visual attention mediates the camera perspective bias 50 That is the correlation between camera perspective and the resulting bias is caused by the viewer s visual attention which is decided by the focus of the camera 50 In the United States and in many other countries interrogations are typically recorded with the camera positioned behind the interrogator and focused directly on the suspect 42 44 These suspect focus videotapes lead to the perception that the subject is participating voluntarily when compared to both audiotapes and transcripts which are assumed to be bias free In other words the manner in which videotaping is implemented holds the potential for bias This bias can be avoided by using an equal focus perspective This finding has been replicated numerous times reflecting the growing uses of videotaped confessions in trial proceedings 44 48 Racial salience bias Edit Psychological research has explored camera perspective bias with African American and Chinese American suspects 51 African Americans are victims to strong stereotypes linking them with criminal behavior but these stereotypes are not prevalent towards Chinese Americans making the two ethnicities ideal for comparison 51 Participants were randomly assigned to view mock police interrogations developed using a male Caucasian detective questioning a Caucasian Chinese American or African American male suspect regarding his whereabouts at a given time and date All interrogations were taped in an equal focus perspective 51 Voluntariness judgments varied as a function of the race of the suspect 51 More participants viewing the Chinese American suspect and the African American suspect versions of the interrogation judged the suspect s statements to be voluntary than did those viewing the Caucasian suspect version 51 Both the African American suspect and the Chinese American suspect were judged to have a higher likelihood of guilt than the Caucasian suspect 51 Racial salience bias in videotaped interrogations is a genuine phenomenon that has been proven through empirical data 51 Policy recommendations Edit Research indicates that an equal focus perspective produces relatively unbiased assessments of videotaped interrogations 46 48 49 52 A variation of the equal focus perspective is the dual camera approach where the subject s and the interviewer s faces are presented side by side One study into this approach suggests it eliminates the usual camera perspective bias on voluntariness and guilt judgments but was no better than the infamous suspect focus condition in terms of its impact on the ability to accurately distinguish between true and false confession 53 To aid criminal justice practitioners and legal policy makers to achieve sound and fair policy a study in Behavioral Sciences and the Law presented the following recommendations based on the body of research 41 53 Custodial interrogations recorded in their entirety with the camera positioned so that the resultant videotape displays an equal focus or detective focus perspective 41 53 If an interrogation has already been videotaped from a suspect focus perspective it should not be used Rather the use of an audio track or a transcript derived from the videotape should serve in its place 41 53 The dual camera approach is not advised because it does nothing to moderate actual accuracy of judgments 41 53 Cases by country EditJapan Edit In 2007 thirteen men and women ranging in age from their early 50s to mid 70s were arrested and indicted in Japan for buying votes in an election Six confessed to buying votes with liquor cash and catered parties All were acquitted in 2007 in a local district court which found that the confessions had been entirely fabricated The presiding judge said the defendants had made confessions in despair while going through marathon questioning 54 Sweden Edit Sture Bergwall 1990 Edit Main article Sture Bergwall Sture Bergwall also known as Thomas Quick confessed to more than 30 murders in Sweden Norway Denmark and Finland while incarcerated in a mental institution for personality disorders He had been committed after being convicted of less serious crimes Between 1994 and 2001 Bergwall was convicted of eight murders based on his confessions All of these convictions have now been overturned on appeal as he was found to have made false confessions and been incompetent to stand trial 55 United Kingdom Edit Robert Hubert 1666 Edit Main article Robert Hubert In 1666 Robert Hubert confessed to starting the Great Fire of London by throwing a fire bomb through a bakery window It was proven during his trial that he had not been in the country until two days after the start of the fire he was never at any point near the bakery in question the bakery did not have windows and he was crippled and unable to throw a bomb But as a foreigner a Frenchman and a Catholic Hubert was a perfect scapegoat Ever maintaining his guilt Hubert was brought to trial found guilty and duly executed by hanging 56 Timothy Evans 1947 Edit Main article Timothy Evans Timothy Evans was accused of murdering his wife and daughter He was subsequently tried for the murder of the daughter was convicted and hanged When informed about their deaths and asked if he was responsible Evans reportedly replied Yes 57 He was later posthumously pardoned in 1966 Stephen Downing 1974 Edit Main article Stephen Downing case Stephen Downing was convicted and spent 27 years in prison The main piece of evidence used against him was a confession he signed He had agreed to this after an 8 hour interrogation which left him confused and his poor literacy skills meant he did not fully understand what he was signing Stefan Kiszko 1976 Edit Main article Murder of Lesley Molseed Stefan Kiszko was convicted of murder in 1976 in what was later described as one of Britain s most notorious miscarriages of justice 58 One of the main pieces of prosecution evidence was a confession Kiszko made after three days of police questioning After almost 16 years in prison Kiszko was exonerated in 1992 further explanation needed When asked why he had confessed to a crime he did not commit Kiszko replied I started to tell these lies and they seemed to please them and the pressure was off as far as I was concerned I thought if I admitted what I did to the police they would check out what I had said find it untrue and would then let me go citations needed United States Edit Peter Reilly 1973 Edit In 1973 18 year old Peter Reilly of Litchfield County Connecticut was convicted of murdering his mother He had signed a detailed confession after first discovering and informing of the crime and then being detained and interrogated for many hours with little sleep During this interrogation with no lawyer present he agreed to undergo a polygraph which he was wrongly told he had failed and was persuaded that only he could have committed the crime He was sentenced to six to sixteen years for manslaughter but freed on appeal in 1976 citation needed Pizza Hut murder 1988 Edit In 1988 Nancy DePriest was raped and murdered at the Pizza Hut where she worked in Austin Texas A coworker Chris Ochoa pleaded guilty to the murder His friend and coworker Richard Danziger was convicted of the rape Ochoa confessed to the murder as well as implicating Danziger in the rape The only forensic evidence linking Danziger to the crime scene was a single pubic hair found in the restaurant which was said to be consistent with his pubic hair type Although semen evidence had been collected a DNA analysis of only one gene was performed at the time even though Ochoa had this gene it was known also to be present in 10 16 of individuals 59 Both men received life sentences with no possibility of parole 59 Years later a man named Achim Marino who was in prison serving his three life sentences for robbery rape and murder respectively began writing letters from prison claiming he was the actual murderer in the Pizza Hut case and that Ocha and Danziger were innocent He said that he had converted to Christianity while in prison and wanted to tell the truth to free Ochoa and Danziger from prison The DNA from the crime scene was tested and it matched that of Marino when The DNA of Ochoa and Danziger was excluded from matching that evidence Ochoa later said that he was coerced by the police to confess and implicate his friend in the rape and murder In 2001 Ochoa and Danziger were exonerated and released from prison after 12 years of incarceration While in prison Danziger had been severely beaten by other inmates in 1991 and suffered permanent brain damage He requires all day medical care for the rest of his life 60 Marino was later convicted of the murder he couldn t be charged with the rape due to the statute of limitations and was given an additional life sentence Jeffrey Mark Deskovic 1990 Edit Jeffrey Mark Deskovic was convicted in 1990 at the age of 16 of raping beating and strangling a high school classmate He had confessed to the crime after hours of interrogation by police without being given an opportunity to seek legal counsel Court testimony noted that the DNA evidence in the case did not point to him He was incarcerated for 15 years further explanation needed Juan Rivera 1992 Edit Juan Rivera from Waukegan Illinois was wrongfully convicted of the 1992 rape and murder of 11 year old Holly Staker Although his DNA was excluded from that tested in the rape kit and the report from the electronic ankle monitor he was wearing at the time while awaiting trial for a non violent burglary established that he was not in the vicinity of the murder he confessed to the crimes Rivera had been interrogated for several days by police using the Reid technique His conviction was overturned in 2011 and the appellate court took the unusual step of barring prosecutors from retrying him 61 Rivera filed a lawsuit against a number of parties including John E Reid amp Associates who developed the Reid technique Reid contended that Rivera s false confession was the result of the Reid technique being used incorrectly Rivera was taken to Reid headquarters in Chicago twice during his interrogation for polygraph tests These were inconclusive but a Reid employee Michael Masokas told Rivera that he failed The case was settled out of court with John E Reid amp Associates paying 2 million 61 Gary Gauger 1993 Edit Gary Gauger was sentenced to death for the murders of his parents Morris 74 and Ruth 70 at their McHenry County Illinois farm in April 1993 He was interrogated for more than 21 hours He gave the police a hypothetical statement which they took as a confession His conviction was overturned in 1996 and Gauger was freed He was pardoned by the Illinois governor in 2002 Two motorcycle gang members were later convicted of Morris and Ruth Gauger s murders citation needed West Memphis Three 1993 Edit The West Memphis Three Damien Echols Jason Baldwin and Jessie Misskelley were convicted for the 1993 murders of three 8 year old boys At the time of the alleged crime they were 16 17 and 18 years old One month after the murders police interrogated Misskelley who has an IQ of 72 for five hours He confessed to the murders and implicated both Echols and Baldwin Misskelley immediately recanted and said he was coerced to confess Although his confession contained massive internal inconsistencies and differed significantly from the facts of the physical evidence revealed the prosecution continued Misskelley and Baldwin were convicted of murder and sentenced to life in prison without parole Echols was convicted and sentenced to death For the next 17 years the three men maintained their innocence In August 2011 testing of DNA evidence was found to be inconclusive it included DNA from an unknown contributor Prosecutors offered the three men a deal if they pleaded guilty to release them for time served They accepted the Alford plea but said that they would continue to work to clear their names and find the real murderer s They were released after eighteen years in prison citation needed Norfolk Four 1997 Edit Main article Norfolk Four Danial Williams Joseph J Dick Jr Derek Tice and Eric C Wilson are four of five men convicted in the 1997 rape and murder of Michelle Moore Bosko in Norfolk Virginia The convictions of the four were based largely on their confessions which they have since maintained were coerced after hours of interrogation during which the men were played off against each other over time The Mid Atlantic Innocence Project considers this a miscarriage of justice 62 Moore Bosko s parents continue to believe that all those convicted were participants in the crime 63 Williams and Dick pleaded guilty to murder as they had been threatened by the potential of being sentenced to death at a jury trial They were sentenced to one or more life sentences in prison without the possibility of parole Tice was convicted of rape and murder and sentenced to death Wilson was convicted of rape and sentenced to 8 years in prison Three other men Geoffrey A Farris John E Danser and Richard D Pauley Jr were also initially indicted with the crime through accusations by others but their charges were later dropped before trial as Tice would not testify against them The supporters of the Norfolk Four have offered evidence that purports to prove that the four men are innocent with no known involvement or connections to the incident No physical evidence supported their cases 64 Tice s conviction was overturned and Williams and Dick received governmental pardons clearing their names The four received a settlement from the city of Norfolk and state in 2018 The fifth man Omar Ballard was indicted in 2005 after his DNA was found to match that at the crime scene He had informally confessed in 1997 but withdrew his statement after being pressured to implicate the four other men He pleaded guilty to the crime in 2009 in order to avoid the death penalty A serial rapist and murderer he was apprehended and sentenced to prison after he pleaded guilty to other crimes of violence against women and confessed to acting alone He was sentenced to 100 years in prison 59 of which were suspended He is the only man whose DNA matched that found at the scene He confessed to committing the crime by himself and said none of the other men indicted and tried were involved Forensic evidence is consistent with his story that there were no other participants Michael Crowe 1998 Edit Michael Crowe confessed to the murder of his younger sister Stephanie Crowe in 1998 Michael 14 at the time was targeted by the police when he seemed distant and preoccupied after Stephanie s body was discovered and the rest of the family grieved After two days of intense questioning Michael admitted to killing Stephanie His confession was vague and lacked detail he said he could not remember committing the crime but believed he must have done so based on what the police were telling him The confession was videotaped by police and showed Michael making statements to the effect of I m only saying this because it s what you want to hear His admission has been cited as a classic example of a coerced false confession during police interrogation 65 Joshua Treadway a friend of Michael s was questioned and gave a detailed confession after many hours of interrogation Aaron Houser a mutual friend of the boys was questioned and did not confess but presented a hypothetical and incriminating account of the crime under prompting by police interrogators using the Reid Technique All three boys subsequently recanted their statements claiming coercion Crowe s confession and Houser s statements to police were later thrown out as coerced by a judge part of Treadway s confession was also ruled inadmissible Later all charges were dropped against each of the three boys Prosecutors later charged an unrelated party with the crime His defense team argued that the three boys who were first charged had been responsible The charges against the three boys were dismissed without prejudice which would allow charges to be reinstated at a later date after DNA testing linked a neighborhood transient Richard Tuite to Stephanie s blood Embarrassed by the reversal the Escondido police and the San Diego County District Attorney let the case languish without charges for two years In 2001 the District Attorney and San Diego County Sheriff s Department asked that the case be taken over by the California Department of Justice 66 Tuite was convicted of the murder in 2004 but the conviction was overturned why At the second trial in 2013 the jury found him not guilty The murder of Stephanie Crowe remains unsolved 67 In 2012 Superior Court Judge Kenneth So made the rare ruling that Michael Crowe Treadway and Houser were factually innocent of the charges permanently dismissing the City of Escondido case against them 68 A TV movie was made about the case called The Interrogation of Michael Crowe 2002 69 Corethian Bell 2000 Edit In 2000 Corethian Bell who has a diagnosis of mental retardation was accused of murdering his mother Netta Bell after he had found her body and called police in Cook County Illinois Police questioned him for more than 50 hours He said he eventually confessed to the murder of his mother because police hit him so hard he was knocked off his chair and because he thought that if he confessed the interrogations would stop He believed that he would be able to explain himself to a judge and be set free His confession was videotaped but his interrogation was not At the time Cook County prosecutors were required to videotape murder confessions but not the preceding interrogations With his confession on tape Bell was tried convicted and sentenced to jail When the DNA at the crime scene was finally tested a year later it matched that of a serial rapist named DeShawn Boyd He was already in prison after having been convicted of three other violent sexual assaults all in the same neighborhood as the Netta Bell murder Bell filed a civil lawsuit with the help of Herschella Conyers and her University of Chicago Law School students which was settled by the city in 2006 for 1 million 70 Kevin Fox 2004 Edit Kevin Fox was interrogated for 14 hours by Will County Illinois police before he confessed to the 2004 murder of his 3 year old daughter Riley He was convicted and sentenced to jail His confession was later ruled to have been coerced Because of DNA testing police later identified Scott Eby as the killer He was a neighbor living a few miles from the Fox family at the time of Riley s murder Police identified him as the killer while he was serving a 14 year sentence for sex crimes After questioning and confrontation with the DNA results Eby confessed and later pleaded guilty Kevin Fox was released after serving eight months in jail The Fox family eventually won an 8 million civil judgment against the county government 71 Laverne Pavlinac 1990 Edit Main article Laverne Pavlinac Laverne Pavlinac confessed that she and her boyfriend murdered a woman in Oregon in 1990 They were convicted and sentenced to prison Five years later Keith Hunter Jesperson confessed to a series of murders including that of the woman Pavlinac had become obsessed with details of the crime during interrogation by police She later said she confessed to get out of the abusive relationship with the boyfriend Her boyfriend purportedly confessed in order to avoid the death penalty citation needed See also EditBritish Post Office scandal False evidence False memory syndrome Fingerprint Forced confession Gudjonsson suggestibility scale Miscarriage of justice Iatrogenesis List of wrongful convictions in the United States Police misconduct Reid technique Timothy Evans Amanda Knox Trisha Meili Martin TankleffReferences Edit Cooley M Craig and Turvey E Brent Miscarriages of Justice Actual Innocence Forensic Evidence and the Law 1st ed Academic Press 2014 p116 Kassin SM False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications for Reform Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2014 1 1 112 121 doi 10 1177 2372732214548678 This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn t commit Science 13 June 2019 Kassin Saul M 2008 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications for Reform PDF Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 4 249 253 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2008 00584 x S2CID 3789032 Douglas Starr 13 June 2019 This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn t commit Science doi 10 1126 science aay3537 S2CID 197717147 False Confessions and Tips Still Flow in Simpson Case Los Angeles Times 25 March 1996 The Psychology of False Confessions Cale Law Office 18 April 2018 False confessions How can psychology so basic be so counterintuitive Paul Kassin American Psychologist 2017 vol 72 no 9 p 953 Frances Chapman Coerced Internalized False Confessions And Police Interrogations The Power Of Coercion Kassin False Confessions How Can Psychology So Basic Be So Counterintuitive American Psychologist 2017 vol 72 no 9 951 964 Gudjonsson G H 2003 The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions p 7 Fisher R P Geiselman E R 2010 The Cognitive Interview method of conducting police interviews Eliciting extensive information and promoting Therapeutic Jurisprudence International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 33 5 6 5 6 doi 10 1016 j ijlp 2010 09 004 PMID 20875685 Kassin S M Drizin S A Grisso T Gudjonsson G H Leo R A Redlich A D 2010 Police induced confessions risk factors and recommendations Looking ahead Law and Human Behavior 34 1 49 52 doi 10 1007 s10979 010 9217 5 PMID 20112057 S2CID 189389 Gudjonsson G H Pearse J 2011 Suspect Interviews and False Confessions Current Directions in Psychological Science 20 33 37 doi 10 1177 0963721410396824 S2CID 145624273 The Seismic Change in Police Interrogations Marshall Project 3 July 2017 Jackie Adams Investigative Interviewing Slideshow presentation for the New Zealand Ministry of Business Innovation and Employment Reid Technique for Interrogations Psychology Aldert Vrij 2019 Deception and truth detection when analyzing nonverbal and verbal cues Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology vol 33 pp 160 167 Leading Police Consulting Group Will No Longer Teach the Reid Technique Innocence Project 3 August 2017 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law September 2009 vol 37 3 pp 332 343 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law September 2009 vol 37 3 pp 332 343 Richard Leo False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 37 332 43 2009 Mentally handicapped Quebec man receives millions for injustice The Globe and Mail 21 December 2006 Victims advocate says justice system to blame for wrongful conviction Canada com 29 April 2008 Archived from the original on 23 September 2015 Mentally handicapped Quebec man receives millions for injustice The Globe and Mail 21 December 2006 Richard Leo False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 37 332 343 2009 This psychologist explains why people confess to crimes they didn t commit Science 13 June 2019 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online September 2009 37 3 332 343 Donald Trump and the Central Park Five the racially charged rise of a demagogue The Guardian 17 February 2016 DNA Exonerations in the United States Innocence Project Examining why false confessions occur in the U S criminal justice system Washington Post 23 June 2019 8 Facts About Incarcerated and Wrongfully Convicted Women You Should Know Innocence Project The Risk of Making False Confessions The Role of Developmental Disorders Conduct Disorder Psychiatric Symptoms and Compliance Gisli Hannes Gudjonsson Rafael A Gonzalez amp Susan Young Journal of Attention Disorders 2021 vol 25 5 715 723 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law September 2009 vol 37 3 pp 332 343 Colorado v Connelly 49 U S 157 1986 False Confessions Causes Consequences and Implications Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law September 2009 vol 37 3 pp 332 343 Strategies for Preventing False Confessions and Their Consequences 2006 Mark R Kebbell and Graham M Davies eds Wiley amp Sons Univ of San Francisco Law Research Paper No 2011 20 P E A C E A Different Approach to Investigative Interviewing Forensic Interview Solutions James Orlando Interrogation techniques C A Meissner A D Redlich S Bhatt amp S Brandon Interview and interrogation methods and their effects on true and false confessions 01 September 2012 a b c d e Lassiter G Daniel Lindberg M 2010 Video recording custodial interrogations Implications of psychological science for policy and practice The Journal of Psychiatry amp Law 38 1 2 177 192 doi 10 1177 009318531003800108 S2CID 146850991 a b c d Kassin Saul M 1 January 1997 The psychology of confession evidence American Psychologist 52 3 221 233 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 52 3 221 a b Video recording custodial interrogations Implications of psychological science for policy and practice The Journal of Psychiatry amp Law vol 38 Spring Summer 2010 a b c Geller W A 1992 Police videotaping of suspect interrogations and confessions A preliminary explanation of issues and practices a Report to the National Institute of Justice Washington DC U S Department of Justice National Organizations Recording Custodial Interrogations February 25 2019 a b c d e Lassiter G Daniel Geers Andrew L Handley Ian M Weiland Paul E Munhall Patrick J 1 January 2002 Videotaped interrogations and confessions A simple change in camera perspective alters verdicts in simulated trials Journal of Applied Psychology 87 5 867 874 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 87 5 867 PMID 12395811 a b c Daniel Lassiter G Diamond S S Schmidt H C Elek J K 1 March 2007 Evaluating Videotaped Confessions Expertise Provides No Defense Against the Camera Perspective Effect Psychological Science 18 3 224 226 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2007 01879 x PMID 17444917 S2CID 722865 a b c d e Lassiter G Daniel Slaw R David Briggs Michael A Scanlan Carla R 1 December 1992 The Potential for Bias in Videotaped Confessions1 Journal of Applied Social Psychology 22 23 1838 1851 doi 10 1111 j 1559 1816 1992 tb00980 x a b Lassiter G Daniel Ware Lezlee J Ratcliff Jennifer J Irvin Clinton R 1 February 2009 Evidence of the camera perspective bias in authentic videotaped interrogations Implications for emerging reform in the criminal justice system Legal and Criminological Psychology 14 1 157 170 doi 10 1348 135532508X284293 a b c Ware Lezlee J Lassiter G Daniel Patterson Stephen M Ransom Michael R 1 January 2008 Camera perspective bias in videotaped confessions Evidence that visual attention is a mediator Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 14 2 192 200 doi 10 1037 1076 898X 14 2 192 PMID 18590374 a b c d e f g Ratcliff Jennifer J Lassiter G Daniel Jager Victoria M Lindberg Matthew J Elek Jennifer K Hasinski Adam E 1 January 2010 The hidden consequences of racial salience in videotaped interrogations and confessions Psychology Public Policy and Law 16 2 200 218 doi 10 1037 a0018482 Ratcliff Jennifer J Lassiter G Daniel Schmidt Heather C Snyder Celeste J 1 January 2006 Camera perspective bias in videotaped confessions Experimental evidence of its perceptual basis Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 12 4 197 206 doi 10 1037 1076 898X 12 4 197 PMID 17154769 a b c d e Snyder Celeste J Lassiter G Daniel Lindberg Matthew J Pinegar Shannon K 1 May 2009 Videotaped interrogations and confessions does a dual camera approach yield unbiased and accurate evaluations Behavioral Sciences amp the Law 27 3 451 466 doi 10 1002 bsl 875 PMID 19387972 Onishi Norimitsu 11 May 2007 Pressed by Police Even Innocent Confess in Japan The New York Times Retrieved 11 May 2007 Stridbeck Ulf 28 February 2020 Coerced Reactive Confessions The Case of Thomas Quick Journal of Forensic Psychology Research and Practice 20 2020 4 305 322 doi 10 1080 24732850 2020 1732758 Juan Dr Stephen 1 September 2006 What is a Confessing Sam The Register Situation Publishing Retrieved 14 December 2018 Kennedy Ludovich 1961 Ten Rillingtom Place Berkley Books p 103 ISBN 978 0586034286 Second victim of Molseed inquiry BBC News 12 November 2007 Retrieved 29 April 2021 a b Forensic Files Forever Hold Your Peace Burden of Innocence Richard Danziger Frontline PBS Retrieved 13 December 2013 a b Starr Douglas 22 May 2015 Juan Rivera and the Dangers of Coercive Interrogation The New Yorker Mid Atlantic Innocence Project Blog Archive Norfolk Four Retrieved 3 May 2015 Jackman Tom 15 December 2008 Clemency Campaign Renews Misery The Washington Post Retrieved 25 May 2010 Dereck Tice Retrieved 3 May 2015 Bell Rachael Coerced False Confessions During Police Interrogations Michael Crowe s Forced Confession Crime Library Archived from the original on 12 November 2013 Retrieved 12 November 2013 Attorney General s Office to Review Stephanie Crowe Murder Investigation Office of the Attorney General State of California Department of Justice 29 June 2001 Retrieved 12 November 2013 Sauer Mark 6 December 2013 San Diego Jury Finds Richard Tuite Not Guilty in Retrial for the Murder of Stephanie Crowe KPBS Retrieved 6 December 2013 Sauer Mark 22 May 2012 Michael Crowe Found Factually Innocent In Sister s Murder KPBS Retrieved 20 October 2012 The Interrogation of Michael Crowe IMDb Retrieved 12 November 2013 Corethian Bell v Chicago Police Department MacArthur Justice Center Northwestern University Law School 11 October 2006 Retrieved 13 December 2013 Schmadeke Steve 3 February 2012 A belated reward for Riley Fox murder lead Chicago Tribune Retrieved 12 December 2013 External links EditThe Confessions at Frontline NPR False Confession Slate Why make a false confession Psychology Today False Confession Innocence Project False Confession Chicago Tribune Cops urged to tape their interrogations Time Telling Untruths The Register What is a Confessing Sam The Truth About False Confessions Sign on San Diego The Stephanie Crowe Murder Case Justice Denied Miranda s Failure To Protect the Innocent Justice Denied False Confessions Are Alive and Well in the U S The radio drama Forced Confession from Destination Freedom depicts an American case Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title False confession amp oldid 1121380769, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.