fbpx
Wikipedia

Self-concept

In the psychology of self, one's self-concept (also called self-construction, self-identity, self-perspective or self-structure) is a collection of beliefs about oneself.[1][2] Generally, self-concept embodies the answer to the question "Who am I?".[3]

The self-concept is distinguishable from self-awareness, which is the extent to which self-knowledge is defined, consistent, and currently applicable to one's attitudes and dispositions.[4] Self-concept also differs from self-esteem: self-concept is a cognitive or descriptive component of one's self (e.g. "I am a fast runner"), while self-esteem is evaluative and opinionated (e.g. "I feel good about being a fast runner").

Self-concept is made up of one's self-schemas, and interacts with self-esteem, self-knowledge, and the social self to form the self as a whole. It includes the past, present, and future selves, where future selves (or possible selves) represent individuals' ideas of what they might become, what they would like to become, or what they are afraid of becoming. Possible selves may function as incentives for certain behaviour.[3][5]

The perception people have about their past or future selves relates to their perception of their current selves. The temporal self-appraisal theory[6] argues that people have a tendency to maintain a positive self-evaluation by distancing themselves from their negative self and paying more attention to their positive one. In addition, people have a tendency to perceive the past self less favourably[7] (e.g. "I'm better than I used to be") and the future self more positively[8] (e.g. "I will be better than I am now").

History edit

Psychologists Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow had major influence in popularizing the idea of self-concept in the west. According to Rogers, everyone strives to reach an "ideal self." He believed that a person gets to self-actualize when they prove to themself that they are capable enough to achieve their goals and desires, but in order to attain their fullest potential, the person must have been raised in healthy surroundings which consist of "genuineness, acceptance, and empathy", however, the lack of relationships with people that have healthy personalities will stop the person from growing "like a tree without sunlight and water" and affect the individual's process to accomplish self- actualization.[9] Rogers also hypothesized that psychologically healthy people actively move away from roles created by others' expectations, and instead look within themselves for validation. On the other hand, neurotic people have "self-concepts that do not match their experiences. They are afraid to accept their own experiences as valid, so they distort them, either to protect themselves or to win approval from others."[10]

According to Carl Rogers, the self-concept has three different components:[11][12]

Abraham Maslow applied his concept of self-actualization in his hierarchy of needs theory. In this theory, he explained the process it takes for a person to achieve self-actualization. He argues that for an individual to get to the "higher level growth needs", he must first accomplish "lower deficit needs". Once the "deficiency needs" have been achieved, the person's goal is to accomplish the next step, which is the "being needs". Maslow noticed that once individuals reach this level, they tend to "grow as a person" and reach self-actualization. However, individuals who experienced negative events while being in the lower deficit needs level prevents them from ascending in the hierarchy of needs.[13]

The self-categorization theory developed by John Turner states that the self-concept consists of at least two "levels": a personal identity and a social one. In other words, one's self-evaluation relies on self-perceptions and how others perceive them. Self-concept can alternate rapidly between one's personal and social identity.[14] Children and adolescents begin integrating social identity into their own self-concept in elementary school by assessing their position among peers.[15] By age five, acceptance from peers significantly affects children's self-concept, affecting their behaviour and academic success.[16]

Model edit

 
One's self-perception is defined by one's self-concept, self-knowledge, self-esteem, and social self.

The self-concept is an internal model that uses self-assessments in order to define one's self-schemas.[17] Changes in self-concept can be measured by spontaneous self-report where a person is prompted by a question like "Who are you?".[18] Often when measuring changes to the self self-evaluation, whether a person has a positive or negative opinion of oneself, is measured instead of self-concept.[18]

Features such as personality, skills and abilities, occupation and hobbies, physical characteristics, gender, etc. are assessed and applied to self-schemas, which are ideas of oneself in a particular dimension (e.g., someone that considers themselves a geek will associate "geek-like" qualities to themselves). A collection of self-schemas makes up one's overall self-concept. For example, the statement "I am lazy" is a self-assessment that contributes to self-concept. Statements such as "I am tired", however, would not be part of someone's self-concept, since being tired is a temporary state and therefore cannot become a part of a self-schema. A person's self-concept may change with time as reassessment occurs, which in extreme cases can lead to identity crises.

Parts edit

Various theories identify different parts of the self include:

Development edit

Researchers debate over when self-concept development begins. Some assert that gender stereotypes and expectations set by parents for their children affect children's understanding of themselves by approximately age three.[21] However, at this developmental stage, children have a very broad sense of self; typically, they use words such as big or nice to describe themselves to others.[22] While this represents the beginnings of self-concept, others suggest that self-concept develops later, in middle childhood, alongside the development of self-control.[23] At this point, children are developmentally prepared to interpret their own feelings and abilities, as well as receive and consider feedback from peers, teachers, and family.[1] In adolescence, the self-concept undergoes a significant time of change. Generally, self-concept changes more gradually, and instead, existing concepts are refined and solidified.[24] However, the development of self-concept during adolescence shows a "U"-shaped curve, in which general self-concept decreases in early adolescence, followed by an increase in later adolescence.[25]

Romantic relationships can affect people's self-concept throughout a relationship.[26]: 89  Self-expansion describes the addition of information to an individual's concept of self.[27]: 30  Self-expansion can occur during relationships. Expansion of self-concept can occur during relationships, during new challenging experiences.[27]

Additionally, teens begin to evaluate their abilities on a continuum, as opposed to the "yes/no" evaluation of children. For example, while children might evaluate themselves "smart", teens might evaluate themselves as "not the smartest, but smarter than average."[28] Despite differing opinions about the onset of self-concept development, researchers agree on the importance of one's self-concept, which influences people's behaviors and cognitive and emotional outcomes including (but not limited to) academic achievement, levels of happiness, anxiety, social integration, self-esteem, and life-satisfaction.[15][29][30][31]

Academic edit

Academic self-concept refers to the personal beliefs about their academic abilities or skills.[15] Some research suggests that it begins developing from ages three to five due to influence from parents and early educators.[21] By age ten or eleven, children assess their academic abilities by comparing themselves to their peers.[32] These social comparisons are also referred to as self-estimates.[33] Self-estimates of cognitive ability are most accurate when evaluating subjects that deal with numbers, such as math.[33] Self-estimates were more likely to be poor in other areas, such as reasoning speed.[33][clarification needed]

Some researchers suggest that to raise academic self-concept, parents and teachers need to provide children with specific feedback that focuses on their particular skills or abilities.[34] Others also state that learning opportunities should be conducted in groups (both mixed-ability and like-ability) that downplay social comparison, as too much of either type of grouping can have adverse effects on children's academic self-concept and the way they view themselves in relation to their peers.[15][29]

Physical edit

Physical self-concept is the individual's perception of themselves in areas of physical ability and appearance. Physical ability includes concepts such as physical strength and endurance, while appearance refers to attractiveness and body image.[35] Adolescents experience significant changes in general physical self-concept at the onset of puberty, about eleven years old for girls and about 15 years old for boys. The bodily changes during puberty, in conjunction with the various psychological changes of this period, makes adolescence especially significant for the development of physical self-concept.[36] An important factor of physical self-concept development is participation in physical activities. It has even been suggested that adolescent involvement in competitive sports increases physical self-concept.[37]

Gender identity edit

A person's gender identity is a sense of one's own gender. These ideas typically form in young children.[38][39] According to the International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family, gender identity is developed at an early age when the child starts to communicate; by the age of eighteen months to two years is when the child begins to identify as a girl or a boy.[40] After this stage, some consider gender identity already formed, although some consider non-gendered identities more salient during that young of an age. Kohlberg noted gender constancy occurs by the ages of five to six, a child becomes well-aware of their gender identity.[41][39] Both biological and social factors may influence identities such as a sense of individuality, identities of place as well as gendered identities. As part of environmental attitudes, some suggest women more than men care about the environment.[42] Forms of gender stereotyping is also important to consider in clinical settings. For example, a study at Kuwait University with a small sample of 102 men with gender dysphoria examined self-concept, masculinity and femininity.[43] Findings were that children who grew up on lower family bonds had lower self-concept.[43] Clearly, it is important to consider the context of social and political attitudes and beliefs before drawing any conclusions about gender identities in relation to personality, particularly about mental health and issues around acceptable behaviours.[43]

Measures edit

Motivational properties edit

Self-concept can have motivational properties. There are four types of motives in particular that are most related to self-concept:[44]

  • Self-assessment: desire to receive information about the self that is accurate
  • Self-enhancement: desire to receive feedback that informs the self of positive or desirable characteristics
  • Self-verification: desire to confirm what one already knows about the self
  • Self-improvement: desire to learn things that will help to improve the self

Some of these motives may be more prominent depending on the situation. In Western societies, the most automatic is the self-enhancement motive, and may be dominant in some situations where motives contradict one another.[44] For example, the self-enhancement motive may contradict and dominate the self-assessment motive if one seeks out inaccurate compliments rather than honest feedback. Additionally, self-concept can motivate behavior because people tend to act in ways that reaffirm their self-concept,[45] which is consistent with the idea of the self-verification motive. In particular, if people perceive the self a certain way and receive feedback contrary to this perception, a tension is produced that motivates them to reestablish consistency between environmental feedback and self-concept.[45] For example, if someone believes herself to be outgoing, but someone tells her she is shy, she may be motivated to avoid that person or the environment in which she met that person because it is inconsistent with her self-concept of being an outgoing person. Further, another major motivational property of self-concept comes from the desire to eliminate the discrepancy between one's current self-concept and his or her ideal possible self.[45] This is parallel with the idea of the self-improvement motive. For example, if one's current self-concept is that she is a novice at piano playing, though she wants to become a concert pianist, this discrepancy will generate motivation to engage in behaviors (like practicing playing piano) that will bring her closer to her ideal possible self (being a concert pianist).

Cultural differences edit

Worldviews about one's self in relation to others differ across and within cultures.[46] Western cultures place particular importance on personal independence and on the expression of one's own attributes[47] (i.e. the self is more important than the group). This is not to say those in an independent culture do not identify and support their society or culture, there is simply a different type of relationship.[48] Non-Western cultures[which?] favor an interdependent view of the self:[46] Interpersonal relationships are more important than one's individual accomplishments, and individuals experience a sense of oneness with the group.[46] Such identity fusion can have positive and negative consequences.[46] Identity fusion can give people the sense that their existence is meaningful provided the person feels included within the society (for example, in Japan, the definition of the word for self (jibun) roughly translates to "one's share of the shared life space").[48] Identity fusion can also harm one's self-concept because one's behaviors and thoughts must be able to change to continue to align with those of the overall group.[48] Non-interdependent self-concepts can also differ between cultural traditions.[49]

Additionally, one's social norms and cultural identities have a large effect on self-concept and mental well-being.[48] When a person can clearly define their culture's norms and how those play a part in their life, that person is more likely to have a positive self-identity, leading to better self-concept and psychological welfare.[48] One example of this is in regards to consistency. One of the social norms within a Western, independent culture is consistency, which allows each person to maintain their self-concept over time.[50] The social norm in a non-Western, interdependent culture[which?] has a larger focus on one's ability to be flexible and to change as the group and environment change.[50] If this social norm is not followed in either culture, this can lead to a disconnection with one's social identity, which affects personality, behavior, and overall self-concept.[48] Buddhists emphasize the impermanence of any self-concept.[51]

Anit Somech, an organizational psychologist and professor, who carried a small study in Israel showed that the divide between independent and interdependent self-concepts exists within cultures as well. Researchers compared mid-level merchants in an urban community with those in a kibbutz (collective community).[52] The managers from the urban community followed the independent culture. When asked to describe themselves, they primarily used descriptions of their own personal traits without comparison to others within their group.[52] When the independent, urban managers gave interdependent-type responses, most were focused on work or school, due to these being the two biggest groups identified within an independent culture.[52] The kibbutz managers followed the interdependent culture. They used hobbies and preferences to describe their traits, which is more frequently seen in interdependent cultures as these serve as a means of comparison with others in their society. There was also a large focus on residence, lending to the fact they share resources and living space with the others from the kibbutz. These types of differences were also seen in a study done with Swedish and Japanese adolescents.[53] Typically, these would both be considered[by whom?] non-Western cultures, but the Swedish showed more independent traits, while the Japanese followed the expected interdependent traits.[52]

Along with viewing one's identity as part of a group, another factor that coincides with self-concept is stereotype threat. Many working names have been used for this term: stigmatization, stigma pressure, stigma vulnerability and stereotype vulnerability. The terminology that was settled upon Claude Steele and Joshua Aronson to describe this "situational predicament was 'stereotype threat.' This term captures the idea of a situational predicament as a contingency of their [marginalized] group identity, a real threat of judgment or treatment in the person's environment that went beyond any limitations within."[54] Steele and Aronson described the idea of stereotype threat in their study of how this socio‐psychological notion affected the intellectual performance of African Americans.[55] Steele and Aronson tested a hypothesis by administering a diagnostic exam between two different groups: African American and White students. For one group a stereotype threat was introduced while the other served as a control. The findings were that academic performance of the African American students was significantly lower than their White counterparts when a stereotype threat was perceived after controlling for intellectual ability. Since the inception by Steele and Aronson of stereotype threat, other research has demonstrated the applicability of this idea to other groups.[55]

When one's actions could negatively influence general assumptions of a stereotype, those actions are consciously emphasized. Instead of one's individual characteristics, one's categorization into a social group is what society views objectively - which could be perceived as a negative stereotype, thus creating a threat. "The notion that stereotypes held about a particular group may create psychologically threatening situations associated with fears of confirming judgment about one's group, and in turn, inhibit learning and performance."[56]

The presence of stereotype threat perpetuates a "hidden curriculum" that further marginalized minority groups. Hidden curriculum refers to a covert expression of prejudice where one standard is accepted as the "set and right way to do things". More specifically, the hidden curriculum is an unintended transmission of social constructs that operate in the social environment of an educational setting or classroom. In the United States' educational system, this caters to dominant culture groups in American society.[57]"A primary source of stereotyping is often the teachers education program itself. It is in these programs that teachers learn that poor students and students of color should be expected to achieve less than their 'mainstream' counterparts."[58] These child-deficit assumptions that are built into the program that instructs teachers and lead to inadvertently testing all students on a "mainstream" standard that is not necessarily academic and that does not account for the social values and norms of non-"mainstream" students.[clarification needed]

For example, the model of "teacher as the formal authority" is the orthodox teaching role that has been perpetuated[by whom?] for many years until the 21st-century teaching model landed on the scene. As part of the 5 main teaching style proposed by Anthony Grasha, a cognitive and social psychologist until his death in 2003, the authoritarian style is described as believing that there are "correct, acceptable, and standard ways to do things".[59][relevant?]

Gender issues edit

Some say, girls tend to prefer one-on-one (dyadic) interaction, forming tight, intimate bonds, while boys prefer group activities.[60] One study in particular found that boys performed almost twice as well in groups than in pairs, whereas girls did not show such a difference.[61] In early adolescence, the variations in physical self-concepts appear slightly stronger for boys than girls. This includes self-concepts about movement, body, appearance and other physical attributes. Yet during periods of physical change such as infancy, adolescence and ageing, it is particularly useful to compare these self-concepts with measured skills before drawing broad conclusions [62]

Some studies suggest self-concept of social behaviours are substantially similar with specific variations for girls and boys. For instance, girls are more likely than boys to wait their turn to speak, agree with others, and acknowledge the contributions of others. It seems boys see themselves as building larger group relationships based on shared interests, threaten, boast, and call names.[60] In mixed-sex pairs of children aged 33 months, girls were more likely to passively watch a boy play, and boys were more likely to be unresponsive to what the girls were saying.[63] In some cultures, such stereotypical traits are sustained from childhood to adulthood suggesting a strong influence of expectations by other people in these cultures.[60] The key impacts of social self-concepts on social behaviours and of social behaviours on social self-concepts is a vital area of ongoing research.

In contrast, research suggest overall similarities for gender groups in self-concepts about academic work. In general, any variations are systematically gender-based yet small in terms of effect sizes. Any variations suggest overall academic self-concept are slightly stronger for men than women in mathematics, science and technology and slightly stronger for women than men about language related skills. It is important to observe there is no link between self concepts and skills [i.e., correlations about r = 0.19 are rather weak if statistically significant with large samples]. Clearly, even small variations in perceived self-concepts tend to reflect gender stereotypes evident in some cultures .[64] In recent years, more women have been entering into the STEM field, working in predominantly mathematics, technology and science related careers. Many factors play a role in variations in gender effects on self-concept to accumulate as attitudes to mathematics and science; in particular, the impact other people's expectations rather than role-models on our self-concepts .[65]

Media edit

A commonly-asked question is "why do people choose one form of media over another?" According to the Galileo Model, there are different forms of media spread throughout three-dimensional space.[66] The closer one form of media is to another the more similar the source of media is to each other. The farther away from each form of media is in space, the least similar the source of media is. For example, mobile and cell phone are located closest in space where as newspaper and texting are farthest apart in space. The study further explained the relationship between self-concept and the use of different forms of media. The more hours per day an individual uses a form of media, the closer that form of media is to their self-concept.

Self-concept is related to the form of media most used.[66] If one considers oneself tech savvy, then one will use mobile phones more often than one would use a newspaper. If one considers oneself old fashioned, then one will use a magazine more often than one would instant message.

In this day and age, social media is where people experience most of their communication. With developing a sense of self on a psychological level, feeling as part of a greater body such as social, emotional, political bodies can affect how one feels about themselves.[67] If a person is included or excluded from a group, that can affect how they form their identities.[68] Growing social media is a place for not only expressing an already formed identity, but to explore and experiment with developing identities. In the United Kingdom, a study about changing identities revealed that some people believe that partaking in online social media is the first time they have felt like themselves, and they have achieved their true identities. They also revealed that these online identities transferred to their offline identities.[68]

A 2007 study was done on adolescents aged 12 to 18 to view the ways in which social media affects the formation of an identity. The study found that it affected the formation in three different ways: risk taking, communication of personal views, and perceptions of influences.[69] In this particular study, risk taking behavior was engaging with strangers. When it came to communication about personal views, half of the participants reported that it was easier to express these opinions online, because they felt an enhanced ability to be creative and meaningful. When it came to other's opinions, one subject reported finding out more about themselves, like openness to experience, because of receiving differing opinions on things such as relationships.[69]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Leflot, Geertje; Onghena, Patrick; Colpin, Hilde (2010). "Teacher–child interactions: relations with children's self-concept in second grade". Infant and Child Development. 19 (4): 385–405. doi:10.1002/icd.672. ISSN 1522-7219.
  2. ^ Flook, Lisa; Repetti, Rena L; Ullman, Jodie B (March 2005). "Classroom social experiences as predictors of academic performance". Developmental Psychology. 41 (2): 31–327. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.590.5750. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.319. ISSN 0012-1649. PMID 15769188.
  3. ^ a b Myers, David G. (2009). Social psychology (10th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. ISBN 978-0073370668.
  4. ^ Ayduk, Ozlem; Gyurak, Anett; Luerssen, Anna (November 2009). "Rejection sensitivity moderates the impact of rejection on self-concept clarity". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 35 (11): 1467–1478. doi:10.1177/0146167209343969. ISSN 1552-7433. PMC 4184908. PMID 19713567.
  5. ^ Markus, H.; Nurius, P. (1986). "Possible selves". American Psychologist. 41 (9): 954–969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954. S2CID 550525.
  6. ^ Wilson, AE; Ross, M (April 2001). "From chump to champ: people's appraisals of their earlier and present selves". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (4): 572–584. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 11316222.
  7. ^ Ross, Michael; Wilson, Anne E (May 2002). "It feels like yesterday: self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (5): 792–803. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.792. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 12003478.
  8. ^ Wilson, Anne E.; Buehler, Roger; Lawford, Heather; Schmidt, Colin; Yong, An Gie (2012). "Basking in projected glory: The role of subjective temporal distance in future self-appraisal". European Journal of Social Psychology. 42 (3): 342–353. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1863. ISSN 1099-0992.
  9. ^ Ahmad, Nik; Ismail, Hisham (2015). "Rediscovering Rogers's Self Theory and Personality". Journal of Educational, Health and Community Psychology. 4 – via ResearchGate.
  10. ^ Aronson, E.; Wilson, T.; Akert, R. (2007). Social Psychology. New York: Pearson Prentice Hall. p. 113. ISBN 9780132382458.
  11. ^ a b c d Rogers, C. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In (ed.) S. Koch, Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 3: Formulations of the person and the social context. New York: McGraw Hill.
  12. ^ a b c d McLeod, S. A. (2008). Self Concept. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/self-concept.html
  13. ^ "Maslow, Abraham H." Psychologists and Their Theories for Students, edited by Kristine Krapp,vol. 2, Gale, 2005, pp. 303–324. Gale eBooks,https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/CX3456300032/GVRL?u=lincclin_pbcc&sid=GVRL&xid=123255e1
  14. ^ Guimond, Serge; Chatard, Armand; Martinot, Delphine; Crisp, Richard J.; Redersdorff, Sandrine (2006). "Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (2): 221–242. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 16536648.
  15. ^ a b c d Trautwein, Ulrich; Lüdtke, Oliver; Marsh, Herbert W.; Nagy, Gabriel (2009). "Within-school social comparison: How students perceive the standing of their class predicts academic self-concept". Journal of Educational Psychology. 101 (4): 853–866. doi:10.1037/a0016306. ISSN 1939-2176.
  16. ^ Gest, Scott D; Rulison, Kelly L; Davidson, Alice J; Welsh, Janet A (May 2008). "A reputation for success (or failure): the association of peer academic reputations with academic self-concept, effort, and performance across the upper elementary grades". Developmental Psychology. 44 (3): 625–636. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.625. ISSN 0012-1649. PMID 18473632.
  17. ^ Gerrig, Richard J.; Zimbardo, Philip G. (2002). "Glossary of Psychological Terms". Psychology And Life. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Retrieved 14 March 2011.
  18. ^ a b Gore, Jonathan S.; Cross, Susan E. (2011). "Defining and Measuring Self-Concept Change". Psychological Studies. 56 (1): 135–141. doi:10.1007/s12646-011-0067-0. ISSN 0033-2968. S2CID 144222471.
  19. ^ Turner, John; Oakes, Penny (1986). "The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence". British Journal of Social Psychology. 25 (3): 237–252. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1986.tb00732.x.
  20. ^ Social Psychology in Action: Evidence-Based Interventions from Theory to Practice. Springer Link: Springer Nature. 2020. ISBN 978-3-030-13790-8. OCLC 1182516016. The thoughts and feelings that arise when you think about the group you belong to form your social identity.
  21. ^ a b Tiedemann, Joachim (2000). "Parents' gender stereotypes and teachers' beliefs as predictors of children's concept of their mathematical ability in elementary school". Journal of Educational Psychology. 92 (1): 144–151. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.144. ISSN 1939-2176.
  22. ^ C., Broderick, Patricia (6 January 2014). The life span : human development for helping professionals. Blewitt, Pamela. (Fourth ed.). Boston. ISBN 9780132942881. OCLC 858749675.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Markus, Hazel; Nurius, Paula (1984). "Self-Understanding And Self-Regulation In Middle Childhood". In Collins, WA (ed.). Development During Middle Childhood: The Years From Six to Twelve. National Research Council. Retrieved 9 July 2021.
  24. ^ Zafar, Human; Nabeel, Tanzila; Khalily, Muhammad (June 2013). "Adolescents' Self-Concept and Their Attitudes Towards Parents, Teachers, and Police Authority". Pakistan Journal of Psychology. 44: 15–35.
  25. ^ Shapka, Jennifer; Keating, Daniel (April 2005). "Structure and Change in Self-Concept during Adolescence". Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 37 (2): 83–96. doi:10.1037/h0087247.
  26. ^ Interpersonal relationships and the self-concept. Brent A. Mattingly, Kevin P. McIntyre, Gary W. Lewandowski. Cham: Springer. 2020. ISBN 978-3-030-43747-3. OCLC 1156101130.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  27. ^ a b Mattingly, Brent A.; Lewandowski, Gary W. (January 2014). "Broadening Horizons: Self-Expansion in Relational and Non-Relational Contexts: Relational and Non-Relational Self-Expansion". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 8 (1): 30–40. doi:10.1111/spc3.12080.
  28. ^ Gupta, Deepty; Thapliyal, Geeta (Summer 2015). "A Study of Prosocial Behaviour and Self Concept of Adolescents". I-Manager's Journal of Educational Psychology. 9: 38–45.
  29. ^ a b Preckel, Franzis; Brüll, Matthias (October 2010). "The benefit of being a big fish in a big pond: Contrast and assimilation effects on academic self-concept". Learning and Individual Differences. 20 (5): 522–531. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.007. ISSN 1041-6080.
  30. ^ Marsh, Herbert W.; Martin, Andrew J. (2011). "Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 81 (1): 59–77. doi:10.1348/000709910X503501. ISSN 2044-8279. PMID 21391964.
  31. ^ Sadhwani, Indu (Spring 2012). "Effect of Self-Concept on Adolescent Depression". Journal of Psychosocial Research. 7: 147–52.
  32. ^ Rubie-Davies, Christine M. (May 2006). "Teacher Expectations and Student Self-Perceptions: Exploring Relationships". Psychology in the Schools. 43 (5): 537–552. doi:10.1002/pits.20169. ISSN 0033-3085.
  33. ^ a b c Freund, Philipp Alexander; Kasten, Nadine (1 January 2012). "How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (2): 296–321. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.473.5659. doi:10.1037/a0026556. PMID 22181852.
  34. ^ Craven, Rhonda G.; Marsh, Herbert W. Marsh (1991). "Effects of internally focused feedback and attributional feedback on enhancement of academic self-concept". Journal of Educational Psychology. 83 (1): 17–27. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.1.17. ISSN 0022-0663.
  35. ^ Çaglar, Emine (Summer 2009). "Similarities and Differences in Physical Self-Concept of Males and Females during Late Adolescence and Early Adulthood". Adolescence. 44 (174): 407–19. PMID 19764275.
  36. ^ Klomsten, Anne; Skaalvik, Einar; Espnes, Geir (January 2004). "Physical Self-Concept and Sports: Do Gender Differences Still Exist?". Sex Roles. 50: 119–27. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000011077.10040.9a. S2CID 144257373.
  37. ^ Findlay, Leanne; Bowker, Anne (January 2009). "The Link between Competitive Sport Participation and Self-Concept in Early Adolescence: A Consideration of Gender and Sport Orientation". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 38 (1): 29–40. doi:10.1007/s10964-007-9244-9. PMID 19636789. S2CID 23442548.
  38. ^ Bukatko, Danuta; Daehler, Marvin W. (2004). Child Development: A Thematic Approach. Houghton Mifflin. p. 495. ISBN 978-0-618-33338-7.
  39. ^ a b Hine, F. R.; Carson, R. C.; Maddox, G. L.; Thompson, R. J. Jr; Williams, R. B. (2012). Introduction to Behavioral Science in Medicine. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 106. ISBN 978-1-4612-5452-2.
  40. ^ LEHNE, GREGORY K. "Gender Identity." International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family,edited by James J. Ponzetti Jr., Gale, 2nd edition, 2003. Credo Reference, https://go.openathens.net/redirector/palmbeachstate.edu?url=https%3A%2F%2Fsearch.credoreference.com%2Fcontent%2Fentry%i%2Fgender_identity%2F0%3FinstitutionId%3D6086.
  41. ^ "Lawrence Kohlberg." The Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology, edited by Jacqueline L. Longe, 3rded., vol. 1, Gale, 2016, pp. 631–632. Gale eBooks, https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/CX36310004ndi9/GVRL?u=lincclin_pbcc&sid=GVRL&xid=179efc26.
  42. ^ Meek, William R.; Sullivan, Diane M. (September 2018). "The Influence of Gender, Self-Identity and Organizational Tenure on Environmental Sustainability Orientation". Journal of Developmental Entrepreneurship. 23 (3): 1850018. doi:10.1142/S1084946718500188. ISSN 1084-9467. S2CID 158670007.
  43. ^ a b c Taher, Naeema S. (1 January 2007). "Self-Concept and Masculinity/Femininity Among Normal Male Individuals and Males with Gender Identity Disorder". Social Behavior and Personality. 35 (4): 469–478. doi:10.2224/sbp.2007.35.4.469.
  44. ^ a b Koole, S. (2007). Self-Concept. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 797-798). SAGE Publications.
  45. ^ a b c Reeve, J. (2018). The Self and Its Strivings. In Understanding Motivation and Emotion (7th ed., pp. 255–282). essay, Wiley.
  46. ^ a b c d Swann, William B.; Jetten, Jolanda; Gómez, Ángel; Whitehouse, Harvey; Bastian, Brock (1 January 2012). (PDF). Psychological Review. 119 (3): 441–456. doi:10.1037/a0028589. PMID 22642548. Archived from the original (PDF) on 13 April 2015.
  47. ^ Markus, Hazel R.; Kitayama, Shinobu (1991). "Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation". Psychological Review. 98 (2): 224–253. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.1159. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224. ISSN 1939-1471. S2CID 13606371.
  48. ^ a b c d e f Usborne, Esther; Taylor, Donald M (2010). "The Role of Cultural Identity Clarity for Self-Concept Clarity, Self-Esteem, and Subjective Well-Being". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (7): 883–897. doi:10.1177/0146167210372215. PMID 20519575. S2CID 45197124.
  49. ^ Shin, Kiseong (2017). The Concept of Self in Hinduism, Buddhism, and Christianity and Its Implication for Interfaith Relations. Eugene, Oregon: Wipf and Stock Publishers. ISBN 9781532600951. Retrieved 15 October 2018.
  50. ^ a b English, Tammy; Chen, Serena (2011). "Self-Concept Consistency and Culture: The Differential Impact of Two Forms of Consistency". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (6): 838–849. doi:10.1177/0146167211400621. PMID 21398583. S2CID 9708510.
  51. ^ Hoffer, Axel, ed. (2015). Freud and the Buddha: The Couch and the Cushion. Abingdon: Routledge (published 2018). ISBN 9780429913969. Retrieved 15 October 2018. Impermanence is also the basis for the Buddhist view that the self has no substantial existence. It does not exist except in one's mind and what goes on in the mind, along with everything else, is impermanent. Life itself is in constant motion. Thus, there is no self. This view of the 'not self,' or 'no self,' is the most difficult concept for non-Buddhists to understand, particularly because the self, individuality, and autonomy are so basic to Western sensibilities.
  52. ^ a b c d Somech, Anit (1 March 2000). "The independent and the interdependent selves: different meanings in different cultures". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 24 (2): 161–172. doi:10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00030-9. ISSN 0147-1767.
  53. ^ Nishikawa, Saori; Norlander, Torsten (2007). "A Cross-Cultural Validation of Adolescent Self-Concept in Two Cultures: Japan and Sweden". Social Behavior and Personality. 35 (2): 269–286. doi:10.2224/sbp.2007.35.2.269.
  54. ^ Steele, Claude (2010). "Whistling Vivaldi: and Other Clues to How Stereotypes Affect Us". Issues of Our Time: 59–60.
  55. ^ a b Meador, Audrey (3 January 2018). "Examining Recruitment and Retention Factors for Minority STEM Majors Through a Stereotype Threat Lens". School Science and Mathematics. 118 (1–2): 61–69. doi:10.1111/ssm.12260. ISSN 0036-6803.
  56. ^ Sunny, Cijy (April 2017). "Stereotype Threat and Gender Differences in Chemistry". Instructional Science. 45 (2): 157–175. doi:10.1007/s11251-016-9395-8. S2CID 151919352.
  57. ^ Gofton, Wade; Regehr, Glenn (2006). "What We Don't Know We Are Teaching". Clinical Orthopaedics & Related Research. 449: 20–27. doi:10.1097/01.blo.0000224024.96034.b2. ISSN 0009-921X.
  58. ^ Delpit, Lisa (2006). "Education in a Multicultural Society: Our Future's Greatest Challenge". Other People's Children: Cultural Conflict in the Classroom: 167–183.
  59. ^ Grasha, Anthony (2010). "A Matter of Style: The Teacher as Expert, Formal Authority, Personal Model, Facilitator, and Delegator, College Teaching". College Teaching. 42 (4): 142–149. doi:10.1080/87567555.1994.9926845.
  60. ^ a b c Maccoby, EE (April 1990). "Gender and relationships. A developmental account". The American Psychologist. 45 (4): 513–520. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.513. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 2186679.
  61. ^ Benenson, Joyce F; Heath, Anna (March 2006). "Boys withdraw more in one-on-one interactions, whereas girls withdraw more in groups". Developmental Psychology. 42 (2): 272–282. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.272. ISSN 0012-1649. PMID 16569166. S2CID 15754277.
  62. ^ Klomsten, Anne Torhild; Skaalvik, Einar M.; Espnes, Geir Arild (1 January 2004). "Physical Self-Concept and Sports: Do Gender Differences Still Exist?". Sex Roles. 50 (1–2): 119–127. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000011077.10040.9a. ISSN 0360-0025. S2CID 144257373.
  63. ^ Jacklin, Carol Nagy; Maccoby, Eleanor E. (September 1978). "Social Behavior at Thirty-Three Months in Same-Sex and Mixed-Sex Dyads". Child Development. 49 (3): 557–569. doi:10.2307/1128222. ISSN 0009-3920. JSTOR 1128222.
  64. ^ Skaalvik, Sidsel; Skaalvik, Einar M. (1 February 2004). "Gender Differences in Math and Verbal Self-Concept, Performance Expectations, and Motivation". Sex Roles. 50 (3–4): 241–252. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000015555.40976.e6. ISSN 0360-0025. S2CID 144393272.
  65. ^ Ertl, Bernhard; Luttenberger, Silke; Paechter, Manuela (17 May 2017). "The Impact of Gender Stereotypes on the Self-Concept of Female Students in STEM Subjects with an Under-Representation of Females". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 703. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00703. PMC 5434750. PMID 28567022.
  66. ^ a b Cheong, P., Hwang, J., Elbirt, B., Chen, H., Evans, C., & Woelfel, J. (2010). Media use as a function of identity: The role of the self concept in media usage. In v. M. Hinner (Ed.), The role of communication in business transactions and relationships, Vol. 6: Freiberger Beiträge zur interkulturellen und Wirtschaftskommunikation: A Forum for General and Intercultural Business Communication (pp. 365 - 381). Berlin: Peter Lang.
  67. ^ Gündüz, Uğur (Fall 2017). "The Effect of Social Media on Identity Construction". Mediterranean Journal of Social Sciences. 8 (5): 85–92. doi:10.1515/mjss-2017-0026.
  68. ^ a b Beddington, Sir John (2013). "Future Identities Changing Identities in the UK: the next 10 years". Foresight Government Office for Science.
  69. ^ a b Long, Chen, J, M (2007). "The Impact of Internet Usage on Adolescent Self-Identity Development". China Media Research. 3: 99–109.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)

Further reading edit

  • Huitt, W. (2011). "Self and self-views". Educational Psychology Interactive. Valdosta, GA: Valdosta State University. (on self-concept versus self-esteem)
  • Maruscsak, Lance. "What Are The Effects of the Self-Concept Theory in High School Students?". Western Connecticut State University.

self, concept, self, construction, redirects, here, other, uses, self, construction, disambiguation, confused, with, self, awareness, self, consciousness, self, esteem, self, image, self, perception, self, assessment, self, control, self, efficacy, self, refle. Self construction redirects here For other uses see Self construction disambiguation Not to be confused with Self awareness Self consciousness Self esteem Self image Self perception Self assessment Self control Self efficacy Self reflection Self knowledge psychology Self schema Self categorization theory Ideal self or Personal identity In the psychology of self one s self concept also called self construction self identity self perspective or self structure is a collection of beliefs about oneself 1 2 Generally self concept embodies the answer to the question Who am I 3 The self concept is distinguishable from self awareness which is the extent to which self knowledge is defined consistent and currently applicable to one s attitudes and dispositions 4 Self concept also differs from self esteem self concept is a cognitive or descriptive component of one s self e g I am a fast runner while self esteem is evaluative and opinionated e g I feel good about being a fast runner Self concept is made up of one s self schemas and interacts with self esteem self knowledge and the social self to form the self as a whole It includes the past present and future selves where future selves or possible selves represent individuals ideas of what they might become what they would like to become or what they are afraid of becoming Possible selves may function as incentives for certain behaviour 3 5 The perception people have about their past or future selves relates to their perception of their current selves The temporal self appraisal theory 6 argues that people have a tendency to maintain a positive self evaluation by distancing themselves from their negative self and paying more attention to their positive one In addition people have a tendency to perceive the past self less favourably 7 e g I m better than I used to be and the future self more positively 8 e g I will be better than I am now Contents 1 History 2 Model 2 1 Parts 3 Development 3 1 Academic 3 2 Physical 3 3 Gender identity 4 Measures 5 Motivational properties 6 Cultural differences 7 Gender issues 8 Media 9 See also 10 References 11 Further readingHistory editPsychologists Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow had major influence in popularizing the idea of self concept in the west According to Rogers everyone strives to reach an ideal self He believed that a person gets to self actualize when they prove to themself that they are capable enough to achieve their goals and desires but in order to attain their fullest potential the person must have been raised in healthy surroundings which consist of genuineness acceptance and empathy however the lack of relationships with people that have healthy personalities will stop the person from growing like a tree without sunlight and water and affect the individual s process to accomplish self actualization 9 Rogers also hypothesized that psychologically healthy people actively move away from roles created by others expectations and instead look within themselves for validation On the other hand neurotic people have self concepts that do not match their experiences They are afraid to accept their own experiences as valid so they distort them either to protect themselves or to win approval from others 10 According to Carl Rogers the self concept has three different components 11 12 The view one has of oneself self image How much value one places on oneself self esteem or self worth What one wishes one were really like ideal self Abraham Maslow applied his concept of self actualization in his hierarchy of needs theory In this theory he explained the process it takes for a person to achieve self actualization He argues that for an individual to get to the higher level growth needs he must first accomplish lower deficit needs Once the deficiency needs have been achieved the person s goal is to accomplish the next step which is the being needs Maslow noticed that once individuals reach this level they tend to grow as a person and reach self actualization However individuals who experienced negative events while being in the lower deficit needs level prevents them from ascending in the hierarchy of needs 13 The self categorization theory developed by John Turner states that the self concept consists of at least two levels a personal identity and a social one In other words one s self evaluation relies on self perceptions and how others perceive them Self concept can alternate rapidly between one s personal and social identity 14 Children and adolescents begin integrating social identity into their own self concept in elementary school by assessing their position among peers 15 By age five acceptance from peers significantly affects children s self concept affecting their behaviour and academic success 16 Model edit nbsp One s self perception is defined by one s self concept self knowledge self esteem and social self The self concept is an internal model that uses self assessments in order to define one s self schemas 17 Changes in self concept can be measured by spontaneous self report where a person is prompted by a question like Who are you 18 Often when measuring changes to the self self evaluation whether a person has a positive or negative opinion of oneself is measured instead of self concept 18 Features such as personality skills and abilities occupation and hobbies physical characteristics gender etc are assessed and applied to self schemas which are ideas of oneself in a particular dimension e g someone that considers themselves a geek will associate geek like qualities to themselves A collection of self schemas makes up one s overall self concept For example the statement I am lazy is a self assessment that contributes to self concept Statements such as I am tired however would not be part of someone s self concept since being tired is a temporary state and therefore cannot become a part of a self schema A person s self concept may change with time as reassessment occurs which in extreme cases can lead to identity crises Parts edit Various theories identify different parts of the self include Self image The view one has of oneself 11 12 Self esteem How much you value yourself 11 12 Ideal self What you wish to be 11 12 Social identity The part of the self that is determined by members in social groups 19 20 Development editSee also Social emotional development Researchers debate over when self concept development begins Some assert that gender stereotypes and expectations set by parents for their children affect children s understanding of themselves by approximately age three 21 However at this developmental stage children have a very broad sense of self typically they use words such as big or nice to describe themselves to others 22 While this represents the beginnings of self concept others suggest that self concept develops later in middle childhood alongside the development of self control 23 At this point children are developmentally prepared to interpret their own feelings and abilities as well as receive and consider feedback from peers teachers and family 1 In adolescence the self concept undergoes a significant time of change Generally self concept changes more gradually and instead existing concepts are refined and solidified 24 However the development of self concept during adolescence shows a U shaped curve in which general self concept decreases in early adolescence followed by an increase in later adolescence 25 Romantic relationships can affect people s self concept throughout a relationship 26 89 Self expansion describes the addition of information to an individual s concept of self 27 30 Self expansion can occur during relationships Expansion of self concept can occur during relationships during new challenging experiences 27 Additionally teens begin to evaluate their abilities on a continuum as opposed to the yes no evaluation of children For example while children might evaluate themselves smart teens might evaluate themselves as not the smartest but smarter than average 28 Despite differing opinions about the onset of self concept development researchers agree on the importance of one s self concept which influences people s behaviors and cognitive and emotional outcomes including but not limited to academic achievement levels of happiness anxiety social integration self esteem and life satisfaction 15 29 30 31 Academic edit Academic self concept refers to the personal beliefs about their academic abilities or skills 15 Some research suggests that it begins developing from ages three to five due to influence from parents and early educators 21 By age ten or eleven children assess their academic abilities by comparing themselves to their peers 32 These social comparisons are also referred to as self estimates 33 Self estimates of cognitive ability are most accurate when evaluating subjects that deal with numbers such as math 33 Self estimates were more likely to be poor in other areas such as reasoning speed 33 clarification needed Some researchers suggest that to raise academic self concept parents and teachers need to provide children with specific feedback that focuses on their particular skills or abilities 34 Others also state that learning opportunities should be conducted in groups both mixed ability and like ability that downplay social comparison as too much of either type of grouping can have adverse effects on children s academic self concept and the way they view themselves in relation to their peers 15 29 Physical edit Physical self concept is the individual s perception of themselves in areas of physical ability and appearance Physical ability includes concepts such as physical strength and endurance while appearance refers to attractiveness and body image 35 Adolescents experience significant changes in general physical self concept at the onset of puberty about eleven years old for girls and about 15 years old for boys The bodily changes during puberty in conjunction with the various psychological changes of this period makes adolescence especially significant for the development of physical self concept 36 An important factor of physical self concept development is participation in physical activities It has even been suggested that adolescent involvement in competitive sports increases physical self concept 37 Gender identity edit Main article Gender self identification A person s gender identity is a sense of one s own gender These ideas typically form in young children 38 39 According to the International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family gender identity is developed at an early age when the child starts to communicate by the age of eighteen months to two years is when the child begins to identify as a girl or a boy 40 After this stage some consider gender identity already formed although some consider non gendered identities more salient during that young of an age Kohlberg noted gender constancy occurs by the ages of five to six a child becomes well aware of their gender identity 41 39 Both biological and social factors may influence identities such as a sense of individuality identities of place as well as gendered identities As part of environmental attitudes some suggest women more than men care about the environment 42 Forms of gender stereotyping is also important to consider in clinical settings For example a study at Kuwait University with a small sample of 102 men with gender dysphoria examined self concept masculinity and femininity 43 Findings were that children who grew up on lower family bonds had lower self concept 43 Clearly it is important to consider the context of social and political attitudes and beliefs before drawing any conclusions about gender identities in relation to personality particularly about mental health and issues around acceptable behaviours 43 Measures editThis section is empty You can help by adding to it November 2022 Motivational properties editSelf concept can have motivational properties There are four types of motives in particular that are most related to self concept 44 Self assessment desire to receive information about the self that is accurate Self enhancement desire to receive feedback that informs the self of positive or desirable characteristics Self verification desire to confirm what one already knows about the self Self improvement desire to learn things that will help to improve the selfSome of these motives may be more prominent depending on the situation In Western societies the most automatic is the self enhancement motive and may be dominant in some situations where motives contradict one another 44 For example the self enhancement motive may contradict and dominate the self assessment motive if one seeks out inaccurate compliments rather than honest feedback Additionally self concept can motivate behavior because people tend to act in ways that reaffirm their self concept 45 which is consistent with the idea of the self verification motive In particular if people perceive the self a certain way and receive feedback contrary to this perception a tension is produced that motivates them to reestablish consistency between environmental feedback and self concept 45 For example if someone believes herself to be outgoing but someone tells her she is shy she may be motivated to avoid that person or the environment in which she met that person because it is inconsistent with her self concept of being an outgoing person Further another major motivational property of self concept comes from the desire to eliminate the discrepancy between one s current self concept and his or her ideal possible self 45 This is parallel with the idea of the self improvement motive For example if one s current self concept is that she is a novice at piano playing though she wants to become a concert pianist this discrepancy will generate motivation to engage in behaviors like practicing playing piano that will bring her closer to her ideal possible self being a concert pianist Cultural differences editWorldviews about one s self in relation to others differ across and within cultures 46 Western cultures place particular importance on personal independence and on the expression of one s own attributes 47 i e the self is more important than the group This is not to say those in an independent culture do not identify and support their society or culture there is simply a different type of relationship 48 Non Western cultures which favor an interdependent view of the self 46 Interpersonal relationships are more important than one s individual accomplishments and individuals experience a sense of oneness with the group 46 Such identity fusion can have positive and negative consequences 46 Identity fusion can give people the sense that their existence is meaningful provided the person feels included within the society for example in Japan the definition of the word for self jibun roughly translates to one s share of the shared life space 48 Identity fusion can also harm one s self concept because one s behaviors and thoughts must be able to change to continue to align with those of the overall group 48 Non interdependent self concepts can also differ between cultural traditions 49 Additionally one s social norms and cultural identities have a large effect on self concept and mental well being 48 When a person can clearly define their culture s norms and how those play a part in their life that person is more likely to have a positive self identity leading to better self concept and psychological welfare 48 One example of this is in regards to consistency One of the social norms within a Western independent culture is consistency which allows each person to maintain their self concept over time 50 The social norm in a non Western interdependent culture which has a larger focus on one s ability to be flexible and to change as the group and environment change 50 If this social norm is not followed in either culture this can lead to a disconnection with one s social identity which affects personality behavior and overall self concept 48 Buddhists emphasize the impermanence of any self concept 51 Anit Somech an organizational psychologist and professor who carried a small study in Israel showed that the divide between independent and interdependent self concepts exists within cultures as well Researchers compared mid level merchants in an urban community with those in a kibbutz collective community 52 The managers from the urban community followed the independent culture When asked to describe themselves they primarily used descriptions of their own personal traits without comparison to others within their group 52 When the independent urban managers gave interdependent type responses most were focused on work or school due to these being the two biggest groups identified within an independent culture 52 The kibbutz managers followed the interdependent culture They used hobbies and preferences to describe their traits which is more frequently seen in interdependent cultures as these serve as a means of comparison with others in their society There was also a large focus on residence lending to the fact they share resources and living space with the others from the kibbutz These types of differences were also seen in a study done with Swedish and Japanese adolescents 53 Typically these would both be considered by whom non Western cultures but the Swedish showed more independent traits while the Japanese followed the expected interdependent traits 52 Along with viewing one s identity as part of a group another factor that coincides with self concept is stereotype threat Many working names have been used for this term stigmatization stigma pressure stigma vulnerability and stereotype vulnerability The terminology that was settled upon Claude Steele and Joshua Aronson to describe this situational predicament was stereotype threat This term captures the idea of a situational predicament as a contingency of their marginalized group identity a real threat of judgment or treatment in the person s environment that went beyond any limitations within 54 Steele and Aronson described the idea of stereotype threat in their study of how this socio psychological notion affected the intellectual performance of African Americans 55 Steele and Aronson tested a hypothesis by administering a diagnostic exam between two different groups African American and White students For one group a stereotype threat was introduced while the other served as a control The findings were that academic performance of the African American students was significantly lower than their White counterparts when a stereotype threat was perceived after controlling for intellectual ability Since the inception by Steele and Aronson of stereotype threat other research has demonstrated the applicability of this idea to other groups 55 When one s actions could negatively influence general assumptions of a stereotype those actions are consciously emphasized Instead of one s individual characteristics one s categorization into a social group is what society views objectively which could be perceived as a negative stereotype thus creating a threat The notion that stereotypes held about a particular group may create psychologically threatening situations associated with fears of confirming judgment about one s group and in turn inhibit learning and performance 56 The presence of stereotype threat perpetuates a hidden curriculum that further marginalized minority groups Hidden curriculum refers to a covert expression of prejudice where one standard is accepted as the set and right way to do things More specifically the hidden curriculum is an unintended transmission of social constructs that operate in the social environment of an educational setting or classroom In the United States educational system this caters to dominant culture groups in American society 57 A primary source of stereotyping is often the teachers education program itself It is in these programs that teachers learn that poor students and students of color should be expected to achieve less than their mainstream counterparts 58 These child deficit assumptions that are built into the program that instructs teachers and lead to inadvertently testing all students on a mainstream standard that is not necessarily academic and that does not account for the social values and norms of non mainstream students clarification needed For example the model of teacher as the formal authority is the orthodox teaching role that has been perpetuated by whom for many years until the 21st century teaching model landed on the scene As part of the 5 main teaching style proposed by Anthony Grasha a cognitive and social psychologist until his death in 2003 the authoritarian style is described as believing that there are correct acceptable and standard ways to do things 59 relevant Gender issues editSome say girls tend to prefer one on one dyadic interaction forming tight intimate bonds while boys prefer group activities 60 One study in particular found that boys performed almost twice as well in groups than in pairs whereas girls did not show such a difference 61 In early adolescence the variations in physical self concepts appear slightly stronger for boys than girls This includes self concepts about movement body appearance and other physical attributes Yet during periods of physical change such as infancy adolescence and ageing it is particularly useful to compare these self concepts with measured skills before drawing broad conclusions 62 Some studies suggest self concept of social behaviours are substantially similar with specific variations for girls and boys For instance girls are more likely than boys to wait their turn to speak agree with others and acknowledge the contributions of others It seems boys see themselves as building larger group relationships based on shared interests threaten boast and call names 60 In mixed sex pairs of children aged 33 months girls were more likely to passively watch a boy play and boys were more likely to be unresponsive to what the girls were saying 63 In some cultures such stereotypical traits are sustained from childhood to adulthood suggesting a strong influence of expectations by other people in these cultures 60 The key impacts of social self concepts on social behaviours and of social behaviours on social self concepts is a vital area of ongoing research In contrast research suggest overall similarities for gender groups in self concepts about academic work In general any variations are systematically gender based yet small in terms of effect sizes Any variations suggest overall academic self concept are slightly stronger for men than women in mathematics science and technology and slightly stronger for women than men about language related skills It is important to observe there is no link between self concepts and skills i e correlations about r 0 19 are rather weak if statistically significant with large samples Clearly even small variations in perceived self concepts tend to reflect gender stereotypes evident in some cultures 64 In recent years more women have been entering into the STEM field working in predominantly mathematics technology and science related careers Many factors play a role in variations in gender effects on self concept to accumulate as attitudes to mathematics and science in particular the impact other people s expectations rather than role models on our self concepts 65 Media editA commonly asked question is why do people choose one form of media over another According to the Galileo Model there are different forms of media spread throughout three dimensional space 66 The closer one form of media is to another the more similar the source of media is to each other The farther away from each form of media is in space the least similar the source of media is For example mobile and cell phone are located closest in space where as newspaper and texting are farthest apart in space The study further explained the relationship between self concept and the use of different forms of media The more hours per day an individual uses a form of media the closer that form of media is to their self concept Self concept is related to the form of media most used 66 If one considers oneself tech savvy then one will use mobile phones more often than one would use a newspaper If one considers oneself old fashioned then one will use a magazine more often than one would instant message In this day and age social media is where people experience most of their communication With developing a sense of self on a psychological level feeling as part of a greater body such as social emotional political bodies can affect how one feels about themselves 67 If a person is included or excluded from a group that can affect how they form their identities 68 Growing social media is a place for not only expressing an already formed identity but to explore and experiment with developing identities In the United Kingdom a study about changing identities revealed that some people believe that partaking in online social media is the first time they have felt like themselves and they have achieved their true identities They also revealed that these online identities transferred to their offline identities 68 A 2007 study was done on adolescents aged 12 to 18 to view the ways in which social media affects the formation of an identity The study found that it affected the formation in three different ways risk taking communication of personal views and perceptions of influences 69 In this particular study risk taking behavior was engaging with strangers When it came to communication about personal views half of the participants reported that it was easier to express these opinions online because they felt an enhanced ability to be creative and meaningful When it came to other s opinions one subject reported finding out more about themselves like openness to experience because of receiving differing opinions on things such as relationships 69 See also editMain article Outline of self Ego death Identity social science Moral identity Personal boundaries Personal identity Psyche psychology Self psychology Self awareness Self control Self efficacy Self image Self esteem Self reflection Self schema Theory of mind Future selfReferences edit a b Leflot Geertje Onghena Patrick Colpin Hilde 2010 Teacher child interactions relations with children s self concept in second grade Infant and Child Development 19 4 385 405 doi 10 1002 icd 672 ISSN 1522 7219 Flook Lisa Repetti Rena L Ullman Jodie B March 2005 Classroom social experiences as predictors of academic performance Developmental Psychology 41 2 31 327 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 590 5750 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 41 2 319 ISSN 0012 1649 PMID 15769188 a b Myers David G 2009 Social psychology 10th ed New York McGraw Hill Higher Education ISBN 978 0073370668 Ayduk Ozlem Gyurak Anett Luerssen Anna November 2009 Rejection sensitivity moderates the impact of rejection on self concept clarity Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 35 11 1467 1478 doi 10 1177 0146167209343969 ISSN 1552 7433 PMC 4184908 PMID 19713567 Markus H Nurius P 1986 Possible selves American Psychologist 41 9 954 969 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 41 9 954 S2CID 550525 Wilson AE Ross M April 2001 From chump to champ people s appraisals of their earlier and present selves Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 4 572 584 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 80 4 572 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 11316222 Ross Michael Wilson Anne E May 2002 It feels like yesterday self esteem valence of personal past experiences and judgments of subjective distance Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82 5 792 803 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 82 5 792 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 12003478 Wilson Anne E Buehler Roger Lawford Heather Schmidt Colin Yong An Gie 2012 Basking in projected glory The role of subjective temporal distance in future self appraisal European Journal of Social Psychology 42 3 342 353 doi 10 1002 ejsp 1863 ISSN 1099 0992 Ahmad Nik Ismail Hisham 2015 Rediscovering Rogers s Self Theory and Personality Journal of Educational Health and Community Psychology 4 via ResearchGate Aronson E Wilson T Akert R 2007 Social Psychology New York Pearson Prentice Hall p 113 ISBN 9780132382458 a b c d Rogers C 1959 A theory of therapy personality and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client centered framework In ed S Koch Psychology A study of a science Vol 3 Formulations of the person and the social context New York McGraw Hill a b c d McLeod S A 2008 Self Concept Retrieved from www simplypsychology org self concept html Maslow Abraham H Psychologists and Their Theories for Students edited by Kristine Krapp vol 2 Gale 2005 pp 303 324 Gale eBooks https link gale com apps doc CX3456300032 GVRL u lincclin pbcc amp sid GVRL amp xid 123255e1 Guimond Serge Chatard Armand Martinot Delphine Crisp Richard J Redersdorff Sandrine 2006 Social comparison self stereotyping and gender differences in self construals Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90 2 221 242 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 90 2 221 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 16536648 a b c d Trautwein Ulrich Ludtke Oliver Marsh Herbert W Nagy Gabriel 2009 Within school social comparison How students perceive the standing of their class predicts academic self concept Journal of Educational Psychology 101 4 853 866 doi 10 1037 a0016306 ISSN 1939 2176 Gest Scott D Rulison Kelly L Davidson Alice J Welsh Janet A May 2008 A reputation for success or failure the association of peer academic reputations with academic self concept effort and performance across the upper elementary grades Developmental Psychology 44 3 625 636 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 44 3 625 ISSN 0012 1649 PMID 18473632 Gerrig Richard J Zimbardo Philip G 2002 Glossary of Psychological Terms Psychology And Life Boston Allyn and Bacon Retrieved 14 March 2011 a b Gore Jonathan S Cross Susan E 2011 Defining and Measuring Self Concept Change Psychological Studies 56 1 135 141 doi 10 1007 s12646 011 0067 0 ISSN 0033 2968 S2CID 144222471 Turner John Oakes Penny 1986 The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism interactionism and social influence British Journal of Social Psychology 25 3 237 252 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8309 1986 tb00732 x Social Psychology in Action Evidence Based Interventions from Theory to Practice Springer Link Springer Nature 2020 ISBN 978 3 030 13790 8 OCLC 1182516016 The thoughts and feelings that arise when you think about the group you belong to form your social identity a b Tiedemann Joachim 2000 Parents gender stereotypes and teachers beliefs as predictors of children s concept of their mathematical ability in elementary school Journal of Educational Psychology 92 1 144 151 doi 10 1037 0022 0663 92 1 144 ISSN 1939 2176 C Broderick Patricia 6 January 2014 The life span human development for helping professionals Blewitt Pamela Fourth ed Boston ISBN 9780132942881 OCLC 858749675 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Markus Hazel Nurius Paula 1984 Self Understanding And Self Regulation In Middle Childhood In Collins WA ed Development During Middle Childhood The Years From Six to Twelve National Research Council Retrieved 9 July 2021 Zafar Human Nabeel Tanzila Khalily Muhammad June 2013 Adolescents Self Concept and Their Attitudes Towards Parents Teachers and Police Authority Pakistan Journal of Psychology 44 15 35 Shapka Jennifer Keating Daniel April 2005 Structure and Change in Self Concept during Adolescence Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 37 2 83 96 doi 10 1037 h0087247 Interpersonal relationships and the self concept Brent A Mattingly Kevin P McIntyre Gary W Lewandowski Cham Springer 2020 ISBN 978 3 030 43747 3 OCLC 1156101130 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint others link a b Mattingly Brent A Lewandowski Gary W January 2014 Broadening Horizons Self Expansion in Relational and Non Relational Contexts Relational and Non Relational Self Expansion Social and Personality Psychology Compass 8 1 30 40 doi 10 1111 spc3 12080 Gupta Deepty Thapliyal Geeta Summer 2015 A Study of Prosocial Behaviour and Self Concept of Adolescents I Manager s Journal of Educational Psychology 9 38 45 a b Preckel Franzis Brull Matthias October 2010 The benefit of being a big fish in a big pond Contrast and assimilation effects on academic self concept Learning and Individual Differences 20 5 522 531 doi 10 1016 j lindif 2009 12 007 ISSN 1041 6080 Marsh Herbert W Martin Andrew J 2011 Academic self concept and academic achievement Relations and causal ordering British Journal of Educational Psychology 81 1 59 77 doi 10 1348 000709910X503501 ISSN 2044 8279 PMID 21391964 Sadhwani Indu Spring 2012 Effect of Self Concept on Adolescent Depression Journal of Psychosocial Research 7 147 52 Rubie Davies Christine M May 2006 Teacher Expectations and Student Self Perceptions Exploring Relationships Psychology in the Schools 43 5 537 552 doi 10 1002 pits 20169 ISSN 0033 3085 a b c Freund Philipp Alexander Kasten Nadine 1 January 2012 How smart do you think you are A meta analysis on the validity of self estimates of cognitive ability Psychological Bulletin 138 2 296 321 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 473 5659 doi 10 1037 a0026556 PMID 22181852 Craven Rhonda G Marsh Herbert W Marsh 1991 Effects of internally focused feedback and attributional feedback on enhancement of academic self concept Journal of Educational Psychology 83 1 17 27 doi 10 1037 0022 0663 83 1 17 ISSN 0022 0663 Caglar Emine Summer 2009 Similarities and Differences in Physical Self Concept of Males and Females during Late Adolescence and Early Adulthood Adolescence 44 174 407 19 PMID 19764275 Klomsten Anne Skaalvik Einar Espnes Geir January 2004 Physical Self Concept and Sports Do Gender Differences Still Exist Sex Roles 50 119 27 doi 10 1023 B SERS 0000011077 10040 9a S2CID 144257373 Findlay Leanne Bowker Anne January 2009 The Link between Competitive Sport Participation and Self Concept in Early Adolescence A Consideration of Gender and Sport Orientation Journal of Youth and Adolescence 38 1 29 40 doi 10 1007 s10964 007 9244 9 PMID 19636789 S2CID 23442548 Bukatko Danuta Daehler Marvin W 2004 Child Development A Thematic Approach Houghton Mifflin p 495 ISBN 978 0 618 33338 7 a b Hine F R Carson R C Maddox G L Thompson R J Jr Williams R B 2012 Introduction to Behavioral Science in Medicine Springer Science amp Business Media p 106 ISBN 978 1 4612 5452 2 LEHNE GREGORY K Gender Identity International Encyclopedia of Marriage and Family edited by James J Ponzetti Jr Gale 2nd edition 2003 Credo Reference https go openathens net redirector palmbeachstate edu url https 3A 2F 2Fsearch credoreference com 2Fcontent 2Fentry i 2Fgender identity 2F0 3FinstitutionId 3D6086 Lawrence Kohlberg The Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology edited by Jacqueline L Longe 3rded vol 1 Gale 2016 pp 631 632 Gale eBooks https link gale com apps doc CX36310004ndi9 GVRL u lincclin pbcc amp sid GVRL amp xid 179efc26 Meek William R Sullivan Diane M September 2018 The Influence of Gender Self Identity and Organizational Tenure on Environmental Sustainability Orientation Journal of Developmental Entrepreneurship 23 3 1850018 doi 10 1142 S1084946718500188 ISSN 1084 9467 S2CID 158670007 a b c Taher Naeema S 1 January 2007 Self Concept and Masculinity Femininity Among Normal Male Individuals and Males with Gender Identity Disorder Social Behavior and Personality 35 4 469 478 doi 10 2224 sbp 2007 35 4 469 a b Koole S 2007 Self Concept In R F Baumeister amp K D Vohs Eds Encyclopedia of Social Psychology Vol 2 pp 797 798 SAGE Publications a b c Reeve J 2018 The Self and Its Strivings In Understanding Motivation and Emotion 7th ed pp 255 282 essay Wiley a b c d Swann William B Jetten Jolanda Gomez Angel Whitehouse Harvey Bastian Brock 1 January 2012 When group membership gets personal A theory of identity fusion PDF Psychological Review 119 3 441 456 doi 10 1037 a0028589 PMID 22642548 Archived from the original PDF on 13 April 2015 Markus Hazel R Kitayama Shinobu 1991 Culture and the self Implications for cognition emotion and motivation Psychological Review 98 2 224 253 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 320 1159 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 98 2 224 ISSN 1939 1471 S2CID 13606371 a b c d e f Usborne Esther Taylor Donald M 2010 The Role of Cultural Identity Clarity for Self Concept Clarity Self Esteem and Subjective Well Being Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36 7 883 897 doi 10 1177 0146167210372215 PMID 20519575 S2CID 45197124 Shin Kiseong 2017 The Concept of Self in Hinduism Buddhism and Christianity and Its Implication for Interfaith Relations Eugene Oregon Wipf and Stock Publishers ISBN 9781532600951 Retrieved 15 October 2018 a b English Tammy Chen Serena 2011 Self Concept Consistency and Culture The Differential Impact of Two Forms of Consistency Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37 6 838 849 doi 10 1177 0146167211400621 PMID 21398583 S2CID 9708510 Hoffer Axel ed 2015 Freud and the Buddha The Couch and the Cushion Abingdon Routledge published 2018 ISBN 9780429913969 Retrieved 15 October 2018 Impermanence is also the basis for the Buddhist view that the self has no substantial existence It does not exist except in one s mind and what goes on in the mind along with everything else is impermanent Life itself is in constant motion Thus there is no self This view of the not self or no self is the most difficult concept for non Buddhists to understand particularly because the self individuality and autonomy are so basic to Western sensibilities a b c d Somech Anit 1 March 2000 The independent and the interdependent selves different meanings in different cultures International Journal of Intercultural Relations 24 2 161 172 doi 10 1016 S0147 1767 99 00030 9 ISSN 0147 1767 Nishikawa Saori Norlander Torsten 2007 A Cross Cultural Validation of Adolescent Self Concept in Two Cultures Japan and Sweden Social Behavior and Personality 35 2 269 286 doi 10 2224 sbp 2007 35 2 269 Steele Claude 2010 Whistling Vivaldi and Other Clues to How Stereotypes Affect Us Issues of Our Time 59 60 a b Meador Audrey 3 January 2018 Examining Recruitment and Retention Factors for Minority STEM Majors Through a Stereotype Threat Lens School Science and Mathematics 118 1 2 61 69 doi 10 1111 ssm 12260 ISSN 0036 6803 Sunny Cijy April 2017 Stereotype Threat and Gender Differences in Chemistry Instructional Science 45 2 157 175 doi 10 1007 s11251 016 9395 8 S2CID 151919352 Gofton Wade Regehr Glenn 2006 What We Don t Know We Are Teaching Clinical Orthopaedics amp Related Research 449 20 27 doi 10 1097 01 blo 0000224024 96034 b2 ISSN 0009 921X Delpit Lisa 2006 Education in a Multicultural Society Our Future s Greatest Challenge Other People s Children Cultural Conflict in the Classroom 167 183 Grasha Anthony 2010 A Matter of Style The Teacher as Expert Formal Authority Personal Model Facilitator and Delegator College Teaching College Teaching 42 4 142 149 doi 10 1080 87567555 1994 9926845 a b c Maccoby EE April 1990 Gender and relationships A developmental account The American Psychologist 45 4 513 520 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 45 4 513 ISSN 0003 066X PMID 2186679 Benenson Joyce F Heath Anna March 2006 Boys withdraw more in one on one interactions whereas girls withdraw more in groups Developmental Psychology 42 2 272 282 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 42 2 272 ISSN 0012 1649 PMID 16569166 S2CID 15754277 Klomsten Anne Torhild Skaalvik Einar M Espnes Geir Arild 1 January 2004 Physical Self Concept and Sports Do Gender Differences Still Exist Sex Roles 50 1 2 119 127 doi 10 1023 B SERS 0000011077 10040 9a ISSN 0360 0025 S2CID 144257373 Jacklin Carol Nagy Maccoby Eleanor E September 1978 Social Behavior at Thirty Three Months in Same Sex and Mixed Sex Dyads Child Development 49 3 557 569 doi 10 2307 1128222 ISSN 0009 3920 JSTOR 1128222 Skaalvik Sidsel Skaalvik Einar M 1 February 2004 Gender Differences in Math and Verbal Self Concept Performance Expectations and Motivation Sex Roles 50 3 4 241 252 doi 10 1023 B SERS 0000015555 40976 e6 ISSN 0360 0025 S2CID 144393272 Ertl Bernhard Luttenberger Silke Paechter Manuela 17 May 2017 The Impact of Gender Stereotypes on the Self Concept of Female Students in STEM Subjects with an Under Representation of Females Frontiers in Psychology 8 703 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2017 00703 PMC 5434750 PMID 28567022 a b Cheong P Hwang J Elbirt B Chen H Evans C amp Woelfel J 2010 Media use as a function of identity The role of the self concept in media usage In v M Hinner Ed The role of communication in business transactions and relationships Vol 6 Freiberger Beitrage zur interkulturellen und Wirtschaftskommunikation A Forum for General and Intercultural Business Communication pp 365 381 Berlin Peter Lang Gunduz Ugur Fall 2017 The Effect of Social Media on Identity Construction Mediterranean Journal of Social Sciences 8 5 85 92 doi 10 1515 mjss 2017 0026 a b Beddington Sir John 2013 Future Identities Changing Identities in the UK the next 10 years Foresight Government Office for Science a b Long Chen J M 2007 The Impact of Internet Usage on Adolescent Self Identity Development China Media Research 3 99 109 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Further reading editHuitt W 2011 Self and self views Educational Psychology Interactive Valdosta GA Valdosta State University on self concept versus self esteem Maruscsak Lance What Are The Effects of the Self Concept Theory in High School Students Western Connecticut State University Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Self concept amp oldid 1186590049, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.