fbpx
Wikipedia

Bayesian inference in motor learning

Bayesian inference is a statistical tool that can be applied to motor learning, specifically to adaptation. Adaptation is a short-term learning process involving gradual improvement in performance in response to a change in sensory information. Bayesian inference is used to describe the way the nervous system combines this sensory information with prior knowledge to estimate the position or other characteristics of something in the environment. Bayesian inference can also be used to show how information from multiple senses (e.g. visual and proprioception) can be combined for the same purpose. In either case, Bayesian inference dictates that the estimate is most influenced by whichever information is most certain.

Example: integrating prior knowledge with sensory information in tennis edit

 
Using Bayesian inference to combine prior and sensory information to estimate the position of a tennis ball

A person uses Bayesian inference to create an estimate that is a weighted combination of his current sensory information and his previous knowledge, or prior. This can be illustrated by decisions made in a tennis match.[1] If someone plays against a familiar opponent who likes to serve such that the ball strikes on the sideline, one's prior would lead one to place the racket above the sideline to return the serve. However, when one sees the ball moving, it may appear that it will land closer to the middle of the court. Rather than completely following this sensory information or completely following the prior, one would move the racket to a location between the sideline (suggested by the prior) and the point where her eyes indicate the ball will land.

Another key part of Bayesian inference is that the estimate will be closer to the physical state suggested by sensory information if the senses are more accurate and will be closer to the state of the prior if the sensory information is more uncertain than the prior. Extending this to the tennis example, a player facing an opponent for the first time would have little certainty in his/her previous knowledge of the opponent and would therefore have an estimate weighted more heavily on visual information concerning ball position. Alternatively, if one were familiar with one's opponent but were playing in foggy or dark conditions that would hamper sight, sensory information would be less certain and one's estimate would rely more heavily on previous knowledge.

Statistical overview edit

Bayes' theorem states

 

In the language of Bayesian statistics,  , or probability of A given B, is called the posterior, while   and   are the likelihood and the prior probabilities, respectively.[2]   is a constant scaling factor which allows the posterior to be between zero and one. Translating this into the language of motor learning, the prior represents previous knowledge about the physical state of the thing being observed, the likelihood is sensory information used to update the prior, and the posterior is the nervous system's estimate of the physical state. Therefore, for adaptation, Bayes' Theorem can be expressed as

 estimate = (previous knowledge × sensory information)/scaling factor  

The 3 terms in the equation above are all probability distributions. To find the estimate in non-probabilistic terms, a weighted sum can be used.

 

where   is the estimate,   is sensory information,   is previous knowledge, and the weighting factors   and   are the variances of   and  , respectively. Variance is a measure of uncertainty in a variable, so the above equation indicates that higher uncertainty in sensory information causes previous knowledge to have more influence on the estimate and vice versa.

More rigorous mathematical Bayesian descriptions are available here and here.

Reaching edit

Many motor tasks exhibit adaptation to new sensory information. Bayesian inference has been most commonly studied in reaching.

Integrating a prior with current sensory information edit

 
(A) The cursor is represented by one dot with an exact location. (B) The location of the cursor is less certain, because it is somewhere within the cloud of dots.

Adaptation studies often involve a person reaching for a target without seeing either the target or his/her hand. Instead, the hand is represented by a cursor on a computer screen, which they must move over the target on the screen. In some cases, the cursor is shifted a small distance away from the actual hand position to test how the person responds to changes in visual feedback.[3][4] A person learns to counteract this shifts by moving his/her hand an equal and opposite distance from the shift and still moves the cursor to the target, meaning he has developed a prior for this specific shift. When the cursor is then shifted a new, different distance from the same person's reaching hand, the person reaction is consistent with Bayesian inference; the hand moves a distance that is between the old shift (prior) and the new shift (sensory information).[3]

If, for the new shift, the cursor is a large cloud of dots instead of one dot (as shown in the figure), the person's sensory information is less clear and will have less influence on how he reacts than the prior will. This supports the Bayesian idea that sensory information with more certainty will have greater influence on a person's adaptation to shifted sensory feedback.

This form of adaptation holds true only when the shift is small compared to the distance the person has to reach to hit the target. A person reaching for a target 15 cm away would adapt to a 2 cm shift of the cursor in a Bayesian way.[4] However, if the target were only 5 cm away, a 2 cm shift cursor position (visual information) would be recognized and the person would realize the visual information does not accurately show hand position. Instead, the person would rely on proprioception and prior knowledge to move the hand to the target.

Humans also adapt to changing forces when reaching.[5] When a force field a person reaches through changes slightly, he modifies his force to maintain reaching in a straight line based partly on a prior force which had been applied earlier. He relies more on the prior if the prior shift is less variable (more certain).

Integrating information from multiple senses edit

Bayesian inference can also be applied to the way humans combine information about changes in their environment from multiple senses without any consideration of prior knowledge. The two senses that have the strongest influence on how humans' reaching adaptations are vision and proprioception. Typically, proprioception has more weight than vision for adapting hand position in depth – the direction moving toward or away from the person reaching – and vision has more weight in the vertical and horizontal directions.[6] However, changing conditions can alter the relative influence of these two senses. For example, the influence of vision on adapting hand depth is increased when the hand is passive, while proprioception has more influence when the hand is moving.[6] Moreover, when vision is reduced (e.g. in darkness) proprioception has more influence on determining hand position.[7] This result is consistent with Bayesian inference; when one sense becomes more uncertain, humans increase their reliance on another sense.

Posture edit

Additionally, Bayesian inference has been found to play a part in adaptation of postural control. In one study, for example, subjects use a Wii Balance Board to do a surfing task in which they must move a cursor representing their center of pressure (COP) on a screen.[8] The Wii surfer got visual information about his/her COP from clouds of dots similar to the one shown in the reaching section. With larger clouds, the surfers were more uncertain and less able to move the COP to the target on the screen. While this result is consistent with Bayesian inference, Bayesian mathematical models did not provide the best predictions of COP motion, perhaps because moving the COP accurately is more mechanically difficult than reaching. Therefore, the extent to which postural movement can be described by Bayesian inference is not yet clear.

Gait edit

Adaptation to shifted feedback also occurs during walking and running. People walking with each foot on a different treadmill belt can adapt their step length when one belt begins moving faster than the other.[9] Additionally, runners are able to alter their maximum ground reaction force and leg acceleration when they see a graph of peak leg acceleration.[10] However, to date, no studies have determined if humans adapt their gates using Bayesian inference or not.

Possible contradictions to Bayesian inference edit

Some adaptation studies do not support the application of Bayesian inference to motor learning. One study of reaching in a force field found that, rather than being influenced by a prior developed over hundreds of previous reaches, adaptation to subsequent reaches is only influenced by recent memories.[11] People reaching in the force field adapted to shifts in the amount of force exerted on the arm, but this adaptation was only affected by the shift in force of the immediately previous reach, not by a well-developed prior knowledge of shifts that had occurred throughout the previous trials of the experiment. This appears to conflict with the application of Bayesian inference to adaptation, but Bayesian adaptation proponents have argued that this particular study required each participant to do only 600 reaches, which is not enough to develop a prior.[5] In reaching studies that show evidence of Bayesian inference, participants typically perform 900 reaches or more.[3][4] This indicates that, while Bayesian inference is used in adaptation, it is limited in that much previous experience is necessary to develop an influential prior.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, Daniel M. (2006). Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences (Vol. 10, pp. 319–326).
  2. ^ Lee, PM. (2004). Bayesian Statistics: An Introduction. Oxford University Press: London.
  3. ^ a b c Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, Daniel M. (2004). Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:244-7.
  4. ^ a b c Wei, K., Kording, K. (2009). Relevance of error: what drives motor adaptation? J Neurophysiol 101:655-64.
  5. ^ a b Kording, K.P., Ku, S., Wolpert, D.M. (2004). Bayesian Integration in force estimation. J Neurophysiol 92:3161-5.
  6. ^ a b Van Beers, R.J., Wolpert, D.M., Haggard, P. (2002). When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation. Current Biology 12:834-7.
  7. ^ Plooy, A., Tresilian, J.R., Mon-Williams, M., Wann, J.P. (1998). The contribution of vision and proprioception to judgements of finger proximity. Experimental Brain Research 118(3):415-20.
  8. ^ Stevenson, I.H., Fernades, H.L., Vilares, I., Wei, K., Kording, K.P. (2009). Bayesian integration and non-linear feedback control in a full-body motor task. PLoS Comp Biol 5(12):1-9.
  9. ^ Vasudevan, E.V., Bastian, A.J. (2010). Split-belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking. J Neurophysiol 103:183-191.
  10. ^ Crowell, H.P., Milner, C.E., Hamill, J., Davis, I.S. (2010). J Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy 40(4):206-13.
  11. ^ Scheidt, R.A., Dingwell, J.B., Mussa-Ivaldi, F.A. (2001). Learning to move amid uncertainty. J Neurophysiol 86:971-85.

External links edit

  • The Computational and Biological Learning Lab at Cambridge

bayesian, inference, motor, learning, bayesian, inference, statistical, tool, that, applied, motor, learning, specifically, adaptation, adaptation, short, term, learning, process, involving, gradual, improvement, performance, response, change, sensory, informa. Bayesian inference is a statistical tool that can be applied to motor learning specifically to adaptation Adaptation is a short term learning process involving gradual improvement in performance in response to a change in sensory information Bayesian inference is used to describe the way the nervous system combines this sensory information with prior knowledge to estimate the position or other characteristics of something in the environment Bayesian inference can also be used to show how information from multiple senses e g visual and proprioception can be combined for the same purpose In either case Bayesian inference dictates that the estimate is most influenced by whichever information is most certain Contents 1 Example integrating prior knowledge with sensory information in tennis 2 Statistical overview 3 Reaching 3 1 Integrating a prior with current sensory information 3 2 Integrating information from multiple senses 4 Posture 5 Gait 6 Possible contradictions to Bayesian inference 7 See also 8 References 9 External linksExample integrating prior knowledge with sensory information in tennis edit nbsp Using Bayesian inference to combine prior and sensory information to estimate the position of a tennis ballA person uses Bayesian inference to create an estimate that is a weighted combination of his current sensory information and his previous knowledge or prior This can be illustrated by decisions made in a tennis match 1 If someone plays against a familiar opponent who likes to serve such that the ball strikes on the sideline one s prior would lead one to place the racket above the sideline to return the serve However when one sees the ball moving it may appear that it will land closer to the middle of the court Rather than completely following this sensory information or completely following the prior one would move the racket to a location between the sideline suggested by the prior and the point where her eyes indicate the ball will land Another key part of Bayesian inference is that the estimate will be closer to the physical state suggested by sensory information if the senses are more accurate and will be closer to the state of the prior if the sensory information is more uncertain than the prior Extending this to the tennis example a player facing an opponent for the first time would have little certainty in his her previous knowledge of the opponent and would therefore have an estimate weighted more heavily on visual information concerning ball position Alternatively if one were familiar with one s opponent but were playing in foggy or dark conditions that would hamper sight sensory information would be less certain and one s estimate would rely more heavily on previous knowledge Statistical overview editBayes theorem states P A B P B A P A P B displaystyle P A B frac P B A P A P B nbsp In the language of Bayesian statistics P A B displaystyle P A B nbsp or probability of A given B is called the posterior while P B A displaystyle P B A nbsp and P A displaystyle P A nbsp are the likelihood and the prior probabilities respectively 2 P B displaystyle P B nbsp is a constant scaling factor which allows the posterior to be between zero and one Translating this into the language of motor learning the prior represents previous knowledge about the physical state of the thing being observed the likelihood is sensory information used to update the prior and the posterior is the nervous system s estimate of the physical state Therefore for adaptation Bayes Theorem can be expressed as estimate previous knowledge sensory information scaling factor The 3 terms in the equation above are all probability distributions To find the estimate in non probabilistic terms a weighted sum can be used E WpS WsPWp Ws displaystyle E frac W p S W s P W p W s nbsp where E displaystyle E nbsp is the estimate S displaystyle S nbsp is sensory information P displaystyle P nbsp is previous knowledge and the weighting factors Wp displaystyle Wp nbsp and Ws displaystyle Ws nbsp are the variances of S displaystyle S nbsp and P displaystyle P nbsp respectively Variance is a measure of uncertainty in a variable so the above equation indicates that higher uncertainty in sensory information causes previous knowledge to have more influence on the estimate and vice versa More rigorous mathematical Bayesian descriptions are available here and here Reaching editMany motor tasks exhibit adaptation to new sensory information Bayesian inference has been most commonly studied in reaching Integrating a prior with current sensory information edit nbsp A The cursor is represented by one dot with an exact location B The location of the cursor is less certain because it is somewhere within the cloud of dots Adaptation studies often involve a person reaching for a target without seeing either the target or his her hand Instead the hand is represented by a cursor on a computer screen which they must move over the target on the screen In some cases the cursor is shifted a small distance away from the actual hand position to test how the person responds to changes in visual feedback 3 4 A person learns to counteract this shifts by moving his her hand an equal and opposite distance from the shift and still moves the cursor to the target meaning he has developed a prior for this specific shift When the cursor is then shifted a new different distance from the same person s reaching hand the person reaction is consistent with Bayesian inference the hand moves a distance that is between the old shift prior and the new shift sensory information 3 If for the new shift the cursor is a large cloud of dots instead of one dot as shown in the figure the person s sensory information is less clear and will have less influence on how he reacts than the prior will This supports the Bayesian idea that sensory information with more certainty will have greater influence on a person s adaptation to shifted sensory feedback This form of adaptation holds true only when the shift is small compared to the distance the person has to reach to hit the target A person reaching for a target 15 cm away would adapt to a 2 cm shift of the cursor in a Bayesian way 4 However if the target were only 5 cm away a 2 cm shift cursor position visual information would be recognized and the person would realize the visual information does not accurately show hand position Instead the person would rely on proprioception and prior knowledge to move the hand to the target Humans also adapt to changing forces when reaching 5 When a force field a person reaches through changes slightly he modifies his force to maintain reaching in a straight line based partly on a prior force which had been applied earlier He relies more on the prior if the prior shift is less variable more certain Integrating information from multiple senses edit Bayesian inference can also be applied to the way humans combine information about changes in their environment from multiple senses without any consideration of prior knowledge The two senses that have the strongest influence on how humans reaching adaptations are vision and proprioception Typically proprioception has more weight than vision for adapting hand position in depth the direction moving toward or away from the person reaching and vision has more weight in the vertical and horizontal directions 6 However changing conditions can alter the relative influence of these two senses For example the influence of vision on adapting hand depth is increased when the hand is passive while proprioception has more influence when the hand is moving 6 Moreover when vision is reduced e g in darkness proprioception has more influence on determining hand position 7 This result is consistent with Bayesian inference when one sense becomes more uncertain humans increase their reliance on another sense Posture editAdditionally Bayesian inference has been found to play a part in adaptation of postural control In one study for example subjects use a Wii Balance Board to do a surfing task in which they must move a cursor representing their center of pressure COP on a screen 8 The Wii surfer got visual information about his her COP from clouds of dots similar to the one shown in the reaching section With larger clouds the surfers were more uncertain and less able to move the COP to the target on the screen While this result is consistent with Bayesian inference Bayesian mathematical models did not provide the best predictions of COP motion perhaps because moving the COP accurately is more mechanically difficult than reaching Therefore the extent to which postural movement can be described by Bayesian inference is not yet clear Gait editAdaptation to shifted feedback also occurs during walking and running People walking with each foot on a different treadmill belt can adapt their step length when one belt begins moving faster than the other 9 Additionally runners are able to alter their maximum ground reaction force and leg acceleration when they see a graph of peak leg acceleration 10 However to date no studies have determined if humans adapt their gates using Bayesian inference or not Possible contradictions to Bayesian inference editSome adaptation studies do not support the application of Bayesian inference to motor learning One study of reaching in a force field found that rather than being influenced by a prior developed over hundreds of previous reaches adaptation to subsequent reaches is only influenced by recent memories 11 People reaching in the force field adapted to shifts in the amount of force exerted on the arm but this adaptation was only affected by the shift in force of the immediately previous reach not by a well developed prior knowledge of shifts that had occurred throughout the previous trials of the experiment This appears to conflict with the application of Bayesian inference to adaptation but Bayesian adaptation proponents have argued that this particular study required each participant to do only 600 reaches which is not enough to develop a prior 5 In reaching studies that show evidence of Bayesian inference participants typically perform 900 reaches or more 3 4 This indicates that while Bayesian inference is used in adaptation it is limited in that much previous experience is necessary to develop an influential prior See also editBayesian approaches to brain function PerceptionReferences edit Kording K P amp Wolpert Daniel M 2006 Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol 10 pp 319 326 Lee PM 2004 Bayesian Statistics An Introduction Oxford University Press London a b c Kording K P amp Wolpert Daniel M 2004 Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning Nature 427 244 7 a b c Wei K Kording K 2009 Relevance of error what drives motor adaptation J Neurophysiol 101 655 64 a b Kording K P Ku S Wolpert D M 2004 Bayesian Integration in force estimation J Neurophysiol 92 3161 5 a b Van Beers R J Wolpert D M Haggard P 2002 When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation Current Biology 12 834 7 Plooy A Tresilian J R Mon Williams M Wann J P 1998 The contribution of vision and proprioception to judgements of finger proximity Experimental Brain Research 118 3 415 20 Stevenson I H Fernades H L Vilares I Wei K Kording K P 2009 Bayesian integration and non linear feedback control in a full body motor task PLoS Comp Biol 5 12 1 9 Vasudevan E V Bastian A J 2010 Split belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking J Neurophysiol 103 183 191 Crowell H P Milner C E Hamill J Davis I S 2010 J Orthopaedic amp Sports Physical Therapy 40 4 206 13 Scheidt R A Dingwell J B Mussa Ivaldi F A 2001 Learning to move amid uncertainty J Neurophysiol 86 971 85 External links editThe Computational and Biological Learning Lab at Cambridge Konrad Kording s lab web site Research for this Wikipedia entry was conducted as a part of a Locomotion Neuromechanics course APPH 6232 in the School of Applied Physiology at Georgia Tech Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Bayesian inference in motor learning amp oldid 1156414835, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.