fbpx
Wikipedia

Perceived visual angle

In human visual perception, the visual angle, denoted θ, subtended by a viewed object sometimes looks larger or smaller than its actual value. One approach to this phenomenon posits a subjective correlate to the visual angle: the perceived visual angle or perceived angular size. An optical illusion where the physical and subjective angles differ is then called a visual angle illusion or angular size illusion.

Angular size illusions are most obvious as relative angular size illusions, in which two objects that subtend the same visual angle appear to have different angular sizes; it is as if their equal-sized images on the retina were of different sizes. Angular size illusions are contrasted with linear size illusions, in which two objects that are the same physical size do not appear so. An angular size illusion may be accompanied by (or cause) a linear size illusion at the same time.

The perceived visual angle paradigm begins with a rejection of the classical size–distance invariance hypothesis (SDIH), which states that the ratio of perceived linear size to perceived distance is a simple function of the visual angle. The SDIH does not explain some illusions, such as the Moon illusion, in which the Moon appears larger when it is near the horizon. It is replaced by a perceptual SDIH, in which the visual angle is replaced by the perceived visual angle. This new formulation avoids some of the paradoxes of the SDIH, but it remains difficult to explain why a given illusion occurs.

This paradigm is not universally accepted; many textbook explanations of size and distance perception do not refer to the perceived visual angle, and some researchers deny that it exists. Some recent evidence supporting the idea, reported by Murray, Boyaci and Kersten (2006), suggests a direct relationship between the perceived angular size of an object and the size of the neural activity pattern it excites in the primary visual cortex.

A relatively new idea edit

Visual angle illusions have been explicitly described by many vision researchers, including Joynson (1949), (McCready 1963, 1965, 1985, 1999), Rock & McDermott (1964), Baird (1970), Ono (1970), Roscoe (1985, 1989), Hershenson (1982, 1989), Reed (1984, 1989), Enright (1989), Plug & Ross (1989, 1994), Higashiyama & Shimono (1994), Gogel, & Eby (1997), Ross & Plug (2002), and Murray, Boyaci & Kersten (2006). Specifically, these researchers cited have advocated a relatively new idea: that many of the best-known size illusions demonstrate that for most observers the (subjective) perceived visual angle, θ′, can change for a viewed target that subtends a constant (physical) visual angle θ.

Indeed, various experiments have revealed most of the factors responsible for these visual angle illusions, and a few different explanations for them have been published (Baird, Wagner, & Fuld, 1990, Enright, 1987, 1989, Hershenson, 1982, 1989, Komoda & Ono, 1974, McCready, 1965, 1985, 1986, 1994, Ono, 1970, Oyama, 1977, Reed, 1984, 1989, Restle, 1970, Roscoe, 1985, 1989).

On the other hand, nearly all discussions (and explanations) of those classic size illusions found in textbooks, the popular media, and on the internet use, instead, an older hypothesis that the visual angle is not perceivable (Gregory, 2008, Kaufman & Kaufman, 2002). They can describe and explain only a linear size illusion, which is why they do not properly describe or explain the illusions that most people experience.

In order to clarify the new paradigm which replaces the old one, it helps to keep in mind that an angle is the difference between two directions from a common point (the vertex). Accordingly, as described below, the visual angle θ is the difference between two real (optical) directions in the field of view, while the perceived visual angle θ′, is the difference by which the directions of two viewed points from oneself appear to differ in the visual field.

Physical measures S, D, R, and θ edit

 
Figure 1: Physical measures

Figure 1 illustrates an observer's eye looking at a frontal extent AB that has a linear size S (also called its "metric size" or "tape-measure size"). The extent's lower endpoint at B lies at a distance D from point O, which for present purposes can represent the center of the eye's entrance pupil.

The line from B through O indicates the chief ray of the bundle of light rays that form the optical image of B on the retina at point b, let's say, on the fovea. Likewise, endpoint A is imaged at point a.

The optical (physical) angle between those chief rays is the visual angle θ which can be calculated:

 

The retinal images at b and a are separated by the distance R, given by the equation

 

in which n is the eye's nodal distance that averages about 17 mm. That is, a viewed object's retinal image size is approximately given by R = 17 S/D mm.

The line from point O outward through object point B specifies the optical direction, dB, of the object's base from the eye, let's say toward the horizon. The line from point O through point A specifies that endpoint's optical direction, dA, toward some specific elevation value (say, 18 degrees). The difference between those real directions (dAdB) is, again, the visual angle θ.

Perceived measures edit

Figure 2 diagrams the perceived (subjective) values for a viewed object.

 
Figure 2: Subjective values

Point O′ represents the place from which the observer feels that he or she is viewing the world. For present purposes, O′ can represent the cyclopean eye (Ono, 1970, Ono, Mapp & Howard, 2002).[1]

Perceived linear values D′ and S′ edit

In Figure 2, D′ is the perceived distance of the subjective point B′ from O′. The observer might simply say how far away point B′ looks, in inches or meters or miles.

Similarly, S′ is the perceived linear extent by which the subjective point A′ appears directly above point B′. The observer could simply say how many inches or meters that vertical distance looks. For a viewed object, S′ thus is its perceived linear size in meters, (or apparent linear size).

Perceived visual angle θ′ edit

The perceived endpoint at B′ has the perceived direction, d′B, and the observer might simply say "it looks straight ahead and toward the horizon."

This concept of the (subjective) visual direction is very old.[2] However, as Wade, Ono & Mapp (2006) noted, it unfortunately has been ignored in many current theories of size perception, and size illusions.

The object's other perceived endpoint, A′, has a perceived direction d′A;, about which the observer might say "it appears toward a higher elevation than point B′." The difference between the two perceived directions (d′Ad′B) is the perceived visual angle θ′, also called the perceived angular size or apparent angular size.

It is not easy to quantify θ′. For instance, a well-trained observer might say that point A′ "looks about 25 degrees higher" than B′, but most cannot reliably say how large a direction difference looks. That skill is not practiced because it is easier to use pointing gestures (Ono, 1970): For example, one often tells another person about the change in the directions seen for two viewed points by pointing something, say a finger or the eyes from one point to the other.

Therefore, in some experiments the observers aimed a pointer from one viewed point to the other, so the angle through which the pointer rotated was the measure of θ′, (Komodo, 1970, Komodo & Ono, 1974, Ono, Muter, & Mitson, 1974, Gogel & Eby, 1997).

Also, because θ′, specifies the amount by which one should rotate one's eye to quickly look from one seen point to another eye tracking, saccade, observers in other experiments shifted their gaze from one object endpoint to the other, and the angle the eye rotated through was measured as θ′ for that object (Yarbus (1967).

Difference between θ′ and S′ edit

It is important to understand how θ′ differs from S′. Consider an example illustrated by the sketch at the right.

 

Suppose one is looking through a window at a 30-foot-wide (9.1 m) house 240 feet away, so it subtends a visual angle of about 7 degrees. The 30-inch-wide (760 mm) window opening is 10 feet away, so it subtends a visual angle of 14 degrees.

It can be said that the house "looks larger and farther away" than the window, meaning that the perceived linear size S′ for the house's width is much larger than S′ for the window; for instance a person might say the house "looks about 40 feet wide" and the window "looks about 3 feet wide."

One can also say that the house "looks smaller and farther away" than the window, and that does not contradict the other statement because now we mean that the amount (θ′) by which directions of the house's edges appear to differ is, say, about half the apparent direction difference for the window edges.

Notice that humans experience both the linear size and the angular size comparisons at the same time, along with the distance comparison (Joynson, 1949). Thus any report merely that one object "looks larger" than another object is ambiguous. It needs to specify whether "looks larger" refers to the perceived angular size (θ′) or to the perceived linear size (S′) or to both of those qualitatively different "size" experiences (Joynson, 1949, McCready, 1965, 1985, Ono, 1970). Notice that in everyday conversations "looks larger" often refers to an angular size comparison rather than a linear size comparison.

Additional confusion has resulted from widespread use of the ambiguous terms "apparent size" and "perceived size", because they sometimes have referred to θ′ and sometimes to S′ without clarification, so the reader must try to ascertain what they mean. Also, in astronomy, "apparent size" refers to the physical angle θ rather than to the subjective apparent visual angle θ′.

The perceptual size–distance invariance hypothesis edit

How the three perceived values θ′, S′, and D′ would be expected to relate to each other for a given object is illustrated by Figure 2 and stated by the following equation (McCready, 1965, 1985, Ono, 1970, Komoda and Ono, 1974, Reed, 1989, Kaneko & Uchikawa, 1997).

 

Ross & Plug (2002, Page 31) dubbed this new rule the "perceptual size–distance invariance hypothesis".

Retinal size, "cortical size" and θ′ edit

As already noted, the magnitude of an object's visual angle θ determines the size R of its retinal image. And, the size of the retinal image normally determines the extent of the neural activity pattern the retina's neural activity eventually generates in the primary visual cortex, area V1 or Brodmann area 17. This cortical area harbors a distorted but spatially isomorphic "map" of the retina (see Retinotopy). This neurological relationship recently was confirmed by Murray, Boyaci, & Kersten (2006) using functional magnetic resonance imaging.

The retinal image is not perceived or sensed. That is, experimental psychologists long ago rejected any idea that people "sense" a proximal stimulus such as the retinal image. As Gogel (1969, 1997) has repeatedly emphasized, there is no "sensation" which could be called the "perceived retinal image size", R′.

Also rejected is a popular idea that an object's "perceived size" results from a "scaling of retinal size"; an illogical process that somehow "magnifies" the very small "retinal size" to yield the viewed object's much larger perceived linear size S′.

Instead, the physical retinal extent R normally determines the magnitude of the perceived visual angle θ′. But, as already noted, "other factors" can intervene to slightly change θ′ for a target forming a constant sized retinal image (and thereby create a visual angle illusion). Indeed, the major discovery by Murray et al. (2006) concerns this flexible relationship between R and θ′, as described below.

Visual angle illusions and area V1 edit

The Murray, et al. (2006) observers viewed a flat picture with two disks that subtended the same visual angle θ and formed retinal images of the same size (R), but the perceived angular size, θ′, for one disk was larger than θ′ for the other (say, 17% larger) due to differences in their background patterns. And, in cortical Area V1, the sizes of the activity patterns related to the disks were unequal, despite the fact that the retinal images were the same size. The difference between these "cortical sizes" in Area V1 for the illusion disks was essentially the same as the difference produced by two non-illusory disks whose retinal image sizes differed by, say, 17%.

The researchers pointed out that their findings dramatically disagree with the hypothetical models of neural events being proposed in nearly all current theories of visual spatial perception.

Murray, et al. (2006) also noted that the flat illusion pattern they used can represent other classic "size" illusions, such as the Ponzo illusion and, as well, the moon illusion which is a visual angle illusion for most observers, (McCready, 1965, 1986, Restle 1970, Plug & Ross, 1989, p. 21, Ross & Plug, 2002).

A detailed meta-analysis of the Murray et al. (2006) results is available in McCready (2007, Appendix B).

The size–distance paradox edit

The classical size–distance invariance hypothesis edit

Conventional "textbook" theories of "size" and distance perception do not refer to the perceived visual angle (e.g., Gregory, 1963, 1970, 1998, 2008) and some researchers even deny that it exists (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2002). This idea that one does not see the different directions in which objects lie from oneself is a basis of the so-called "size–distance invariance hypothesis" (SDIH).

That old SDIH logic (geometry) is typically illustrated using a diagram that resembles Figure 2, but has the physical visual angle θ substituted for the perceived visual angle θ′. The equation for the SDIH thus is

 

Here, S′ is typically called the "perceived size" or "apparent size"; more precisely it is the perceived linear size, measured in meters.

When rearranged as S′ = D′ tan θ, the equation expresses Emmert's law.

However, at least since 1962, researchers have pointed out that many classic "size" and distance illusions can be neither described nor explained using the SDIH, so a new hypothesis is needed (Boring 1962, Gruber, 1956, McCready, 1965, Baird, 1970, Ono 1970). For instance, consider the simple Ebbinghaus illusion.

Example: the Ebbinghaus illusion edit

 
The two orange circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the left seems smaller.

The two central circles are the same linear size S and the same viewing distance D, so they subtend the same visual angle θ and form equal-sized retinal images. But the lower one "looks larger" than the upper one.

According to the SDIH, "looks larger" can mean only that S′ is greater, and with the physical angle θ the same for both, the SDIH requires that D′ be greater for the lower one than for the upper one. However, for most observers, both circles appear unequal while also appearing at the same distance (on the same page).

This commonly found disagreement between published data and the SDIH is known as the "size–distance paradox" (Gruber, 1956, Ono, et al. 1974).

The "paradox" completely vanishes, however, when the illusion is described, instead, as basically a visual angle illusion: That is, the perceived visual angle θ′ is larger for the lower circle than for the upper circle: It is as if its retinal image were larger. So. according to the "new" perceptual invariance hypothesis, (S′ / D′ = tan θ′), with θ′ larger for the lower circle, and with D′ correctly the same for both circles, then S′ becomes larger for the lower one by the same ratio that θ′ is larger. That is, the reason the lower one looks a larger linear size on the page is because it looks a larger angular size than the upper one.

Explaining visual angle illusions remains difficult edit

The new hypothesis that includes θ′ along with S′ describes the Ebbinghaus illusion and many other classic "size" illusions more completely and more logically than does the popular SDIH. What still needs to be explained, however, is why the basic visual angle illusion occurs in each example.

Describing the few existing explanations for visual angle illusions is beyond the scope of this present entry. The most recent theories have been presented mostly in articles concerning the moon illusion (Baird et al., 1990, Enright, 1989a, 1989b, Hershenson, 1982, 1989b, Higashiyama, 1992, McCready 1986, 1999–2007, Plug & Ross, 1989, Reed, 1989, Roscoe, 1989, and especially in two "moon illusion" books (Hershenson, 1989; Ross & Plug, 2002) which make it quite clear that vision scientists have not yet agreed upon any particular theory of visual angle illusions.

There also is the lesser-known, but evidently the largest visual angle illusion of oculomotor micropsia (convergence micropsia) for which a few different explanations are being considered (McCready, 1965, 2007, Ono, 1970, Komoda & Ono, 1974, Ono, et al. 1974, Enright, 1987b, 1989a, 1989b).

This is a partial list of "size and distance" illusions that begin as visual angle illusions (angular size illusions) for most observers.

Notes edit

  1. ^ In some theories the cyclopean eye is, in effect, approximately midway between where one feels one's eye are located in one's body image of one's head (Ono, 1970, Ono, Mapp, & Howard, 2002). Some other theories define the place from which one feels one is viewing the world as the visual egocenter (Roelofs, 19xx, McCready, 1964, 1965, Sakuma & Pfaff, 1979) which, among observers, ranges, in effect, from about midway between the eyes to at least as far back as the center of the head, about 4 inches behind the eyes, approximately midway between the two ears, on the axis for horizontal head rotations.
  2. ^ The subjective experiences of visual directions were fully researched by Ewald Hering (1942/1879) and by Hermann von Helmholtz (1962/1910) who distinguished between the perceived oculocentric directions and the perceived egocentric directions. They, and other theorists, have pointed out that a viewed point's egocentric direction (d'B and d'A here) is determined by a process that necessarily combines the position of the point's image on the retina with information about the position of the eye with respect to the head (and body).

References edit

  • Baird, J.C. (1970), Psychophysical analysis of visual space, Oxford, London: Pergamon Press
  • Baird, J.C.; Wagner, M.; Fuld, K. (1990), "A simple but powerful theory of the moon illusion", Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16 (3): 675–677, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.675, PMID 2144580
  • Barbeito, R.; Ono, H (1979), "Four methods of locating the egocenter: a comparison of their predictive validities and reliabilities", Behav Res Methods Instrum, 11: 31–36, doi:10.3758/bf03205428
  • Enright, J.T. (1987a), "Art and the oculomotor system: Perspective illustrations evoke vergence changes", Perception, 16 (6): 731–746, doi:10.1068/p160731, PMID 3454431, S2CID 34545108
  • Enright, J.T. (1987b), "Perspective vergence: Oculomotor responses to line drawings", Vision Research, 27 (9): 1513–1526, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.211.4341, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90160-X, PMID 3445485, S2CID 9183517
  • Enright, J.T. (1989a), "Manipulating stereopsis and vergence in an outdoor setting: Moon, sky and horizon", Vision Research, 29 (12): 1815–1824, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(89)90162-4, PMID 2631401, S2CID 26009881
  • Enright, J.T. (1989b), "4. The eye, the brain and the size of the moon: Toward a unified oculomotor hypothesis for the moon illusion", in Hershenson, M. (ed.), The Moon Illusion, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Earlbaum
  • Gogel, W.C. (1969), "The sensing of retinal size", Vision Research, 9 (9): 1079–94, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(69)90049-2, PMID 5350376
  • Gogel, W.C.; Eby, D.W. (1997), "Measures of perceived linear size, sagittal motion, and visual angle from optical expansions and contractions", Perception & Psychophysics, 59 (5): 783–806, doi:10.3758/BF03206024, PMID 9259645
  • Gregory, R.L. (1963), "Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling", Nature, 199 (4894): 678–680, doi:10.1038/199678a0, PMID 14074555, S2CID 4153670
  • Gregory, R.L. (1970), The intelligent eye, New York: McGraw-Hill
  • Gregory, R.L. (1998), Eye and brain (5th ed.), Oxford: Oxford University Press
  • Gregory, R.L. (2008), "Emmert's law and the moon illusion", Spatial Vision, 21 (3–5): 407–420 n, doi:10.1163/156856808784532509, PMID 18534112
  • Gruber, H.E. (1956), "The size-distance paradox: A reply to Gilinsky", American Journal of Psychology, 69 (3): 469–476, doi:10.2307/1419056, JSTOR 1419056, PMID 13354816
  • Helmholtz, H. von. (1962) [1910], translated by Southall, J.P.C. (ed.), Treatise on physiological optics, vol. 3, New York: Dover
  • Hering, E. (1977) [1879], The Theory of Binocular Vision, New York: Plenum Press (translation)
  • Hershenson, M. (1982), "Moon illusion and spiral aftereffect: Illusions due to the loom-zoom system?", Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111 (4): 423–440, doi:10.1037/0096-3445.111.4.423, PMID 6219186
  • Hershenson, M. (1989), "5. Moon illusion as anomaly", in Hershenson, M. (ed.), The Moon Illusion, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Earlbaum
  • Higashiyama, A. (1992), "Anisotropic perception of visual angle: Implications for the horizontal-vertical illusion, overconstancy of size, and the moon illusion", Perception & Psychophysics, 51 (3): 218–230, doi:10.3758/BF03212248, PMID 1561047
  • Higashiyama, A.; Shimono, K. (1994), "How accurate is size and distance perception for very far terrestrial objects?", Perception & Psychophysics, 55 (4): 429–442, doi:10.3758/BF03205300, PMID 8036122
  • Joynson, R.B. (1949), "The problem of size and distance", Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1 (3): 119–135, doi:10.1080/17470214908416754, S2CID 144334816
  • Kaneko, H.; Uchikawa, K. (1997), "Perceived angular size and linear size: the role of binocular disparity and visual surround", Perception, 26 (1): 17–27, doi:10.1068/p260017, PMID 9196687, S2CID 41489059
  • Kaufman, L.; Kaufman, J.H. (2000), "Explaining the moon illusion", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97 (1): 500–505, doi:10.1073/pnas.97.1.500, PMC 26692, PMID 10618447
  • Komoda, M.K.; Ono, H. (1974), "Oculomotor adjustments and size-distance perception", Perception & Psychophysics, 15 (2): 353–360, doi:10.3758/BF03213958
  • McCready, D. (1963), Visual acuity under conditions that induce size illusions, Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan (See Dissertation Abstracts International, 1964, 24, 5573.)
  • McCready, D. (1964), Location of the Visual Egocenter Paper presented at meeting of the Midwestern Section of the Association for Research in Ophthalmology, Rochester MN. (May, 1964).
  • McCready, D. (1965), "Size-distance perception and accommodation-convergence micropsia: A critique", Vision Research, 5 (3): 189–206, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(65)90065-9, PMID 5862949
  • McCready, D. (1983), Moon Illusions and Other Visual Illusions Redefined, Psychology Department Report, University of Wisconsin–Whitewater, p. 86
  • McCready, D. (1985), "On size, distance and visual angle perception", Perception & Psychophysics, 37 (4): 323–334, doi:10.3758/BF03211355, PMID 4034350
  • McCready, D. (1986), "Moon illusions redescribed", Perception & Psychophysics, 39 (1): 64–72, doi:10.3758/BF03207585, PMID 3703663
  • McCready, D. (1994), Toward the Distance-Cue Theory of Visual Angle Illusions, Psychology Department Report, University of Wisconsin–Whitewater, p. 40
  • McCready, D. (1999–2007), The moon illusion explained (PDF)
  • Murray, S.O.; Boyaci, H.; Kersten, D. (March 1, 2006), (PDF), Nature Neuroscience, 9 (3): 429–434, doi:10.1038/nn1641, PMID 16462737, S2CID 8391410, archived from the original (PDF) on March 18, 2015
  • Ono, H. (1970), "Some thoughts on different perceptual tasks related to size and distance", in Baird, J. C. (ed.), Human space perception: Proceedings of the Dartmouth conference, Psychonomic Monograph Supplement, vol. 3 (13, Whole No. 45)
  • Ono, H.; Mapp, A.P.; Howard, I.P. (2002), "The cyclopean eye in vision: The new and old data continue to hit you right between the eyes", Vision Research, 42 (10): 1307–1324, doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00281-4, PMID 12044760, S2CID 8467032
  • Ono, H.; Muter, P.; Mitson, L. (1974), "Size-distance paradox with accommodative micropsia", Perception & Psychophysics, 15 (2): 301–307, doi:10.3758/BF03213948
  • Oyama, T. (1977), "Feature analysers, optical illusions, and figural aftereffects", Perception, 6 (4): 401–406, doi:10.1068/p060401, PMID 917729, S2CID 21941416
  • Plug, C.; Ross, H.E. (1989), "2. Historical Review", in Hershenson, M. (ed.), The Moon Illusion, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Earlbaum
  • Plug, C.; Ross, H.E. (1994), "The natural moon illusion: A multifactor angular account", Perception, 23 (3): 321–333, doi:10.1068/p230321, PMID 7971109, S2CID 41311235
  • Reed, C.F. (1984), "Terrestrial passage theory of the moon illusion", Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113 (4): 489–500, doi:10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.489, PMID 6240520
  • Reed, C.F. (1989), "11. Terrestrial and celestial passage", in Hershenson, M. (ed.), The Moon Illusion, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Earlbaum
  • Restle, F. (1970), "Moon illusion explained on the basis of relative size", Science, 167 (3921): 1092–1096, doi:10.1126/science.167.3921.1092, PMID 17829398
  • Rock, I.; McDermott, W. (1964), "The perception of visual angle", Acta Psychologica, 22: 119–134, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(64)90011-3
  • Roelofs, C.O. (1959), "Considerations on the visual egocenter", Acta Psychologica, 16: 226–234, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(59)90096-4
  • Roscoe, S.N. (1985), "Bigness is in the eye of the beholder", Human Factors, 27 (6): 615–636, doi:10.1177/001872088502700601, PMID 3914446, S2CID 34272970
  • Roscoe, S.N. (1989), "3. The zoom-lens hypothesis", in Hershenson, M. (ed.), The Moon Illusion, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Earlbaum
  • Ross, H.E.; Plug, C. (2002), The mystery of the moon illusion: Exploring size perception, Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-850862-5
  • Sakuma, Y.; Pfaff, W. (1979), "Considerations on the visual egocentre", Acta Psychologica, 16: 226–234, doi:10.1016/0001-6918(59)90096-4
  • Wade, N.J.; Ono, H.; Mapp, A.P. (2006), "The lost direction in binocular vision: The neglected signs posted by Walls, Towne, and Leconte", Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 42 (1): 61–86, doi:10.1002/jhbs.20135, PMID 16345004
  • Yarbus, A.L. (1967), Eye Movements and Vision, New York: Plenum

perceived, visual, angle, human, visual, perception, visual, angle, denoted, subtended, viewed, object, sometimes, looks, larger, smaller, than, actual, value, approach, this, phenomenon, posits, subjective, correlate, visual, angle, perceived, visual, angle, . In human visual perception the visual angle denoted 8 subtended by a viewed object sometimes looks larger or smaller than its actual value One approach to this phenomenon posits a subjective correlate to the visual angle the perceived visual angle or perceived angular size An optical illusion where the physical and subjective angles differ is then called a visual angle illusion or angular size illusion Angular size illusions are most obvious as relative angular size illusions in which two objects that subtend the same visual angle appear to have different angular sizes it is as if their equal sized images on the retina were of different sizes Angular size illusions are contrasted with linear size illusions in which two objects that are the same physical size do not appear so An angular size illusion may be accompanied by or cause a linear size illusion at the same time The perceived visual angle paradigm begins with a rejection of the classical size distance invariance hypothesis SDIH which states that the ratio of perceived linear size to perceived distance is a simple function of the visual angle The SDIH does not explain some illusions such as the Moon illusion in which the Moon appears larger when it is near the horizon It is replaced by a perceptual SDIH in which the visual angle is replaced by the perceived visual angle This new formulation avoids some of the paradoxes of the SDIH but it remains difficult to explain why a given illusion occurs This paradigm is not universally accepted many textbook explanations of size and distance perception do not refer to the perceived visual angle and some researchers deny that it exists Some recent evidence supporting the idea reported by Murray Boyaci and Kersten 2006 suggests a direct relationship between the perceived angular size of an object and the size of the neural activity pattern it excites in the primary visual cortex Contents 1 A relatively new idea 2 Physical measures S D R and 8 3 Perceived measures 3 1 Perceived linear values D and S 3 2 Perceived visual angle 8 3 3 Difference between 8 and S 3 4 The perceptual size distance invariance hypothesis 4 Retinal size cortical size and 8 4 1 Visual angle illusions and area V1 5 The size distance paradox 5 1 The classical size distance invariance hypothesis 5 2 Example the Ebbinghaus illusion 5 3 Explaining visual angle illusions remains difficult 6 Notes 7 ReferencesA relatively new idea editVisual angle illusions have been explicitly described by many vision researchers including Joynson 1949 McCready 1963 1965 1985 1999 Rock amp McDermott 1964 Baird 1970 Ono 1970 Roscoe 1985 1989 Hershenson 1982 1989 Reed 1984 1989 Enright 1989 Plug amp Ross 1989 1994 Higashiyama amp Shimono 1994 Gogel amp Eby 1997 Ross amp Plug 2002 and Murray Boyaci amp Kersten 2006 Specifically these researchers cited have advocated a relatively new idea that many of the best known size illusions demonstrate that for most observers the subjective perceived visual angle 8 can change for a viewed target that subtends a constant physical visual angle 8 Indeed various experiments have revealed most of the factors responsible for these visual angle illusions and a few different explanations for them have been published Baird Wagner amp Fuld 1990 Enright 1987 1989 Hershenson 1982 1989 Komoda amp Ono 1974 McCready 1965 1985 1986 1994 Ono 1970 Oyama 1977 Reed 1984 1989 Restle 1970 Roscoe 1985 1989 On the other hand nearly all discussions and explanations of those classic size illusions found in textbooks the popular media and on the internet use instead an older hypothesis that the visual angle is not perceivable Gregory 2008 Kaufman amp Kaufman 2002 They can describe and explain only a linear size illusion which is why they do not properly describe or explain the illusions that most people experience In order to clarify the new paradigm which replaces the old one it helps to keep in mind that an angle is the difference between two directions from a common point the vertex Accordingly as described below the visual angle 8 is the difference between two real optical directions in the field of view while the perceived visual angle 8 is the difference by which the directions of two viewed points from oneself appear to differ in the visual field Physical measures S D R and 8 editMain article Visual angle nbsp Figure 1 Physical measuresFigure 1 illustrates an observer s eye looking at a frontal extent AB that has a linear size S also called its metric size or tape measure size The extent s lower endpoint at B lies at a distance D from point O which for present purposes can represent the center of the eye s entrance pupil The line from B through O indicates the chief ray of the bundle of light rays that form the optical image of B on the retina at point b let s say on the fovea Likewise endpoint A is imaged at point a The optical physical angle between those chief rays is the visual angle 8 which can be calculated tan 8 S D displaystyle tan theta S D nbsp The retinal images at b and a are separated by the distance R given by the equation R n tan 8 displaystyle R n tan theta nbsp in which n is the eye s nodal distance that averages about 17 mm That is a viewed object s retinal image size is approximately given by R 17 S D mm The line from point O outward through object point B specifies the optical direction dB of the object s base from the eye let s say toward the horizon The line from point O through point A specifies that endpoint s optical direction dA toward some specific elevation value say 18 degrees The difference between those real directions dA dB is again the visual angle 8 Perceived measures editFigure 2 diagrams the perceived subjective values for a viewed object nbsp Figure 2 Subjective valuesPoint O represents the place from which the observer feels that he or she is viewing the world For present purposes O can represent the cyclopean eye Ono 1970 Ono Mapp amp Howard 2002 1 Perceived linear values D and S edit In Figure 2 D is the perceived distance of the subjective point B from O The observer might simply say how far away point B looks in inches or meters or miles Similarly S is the perceived linear extent by which the subjective point A appears directly above point B The observer could simply say how many inches or meters that vertical distance looks For a viewed object S thus is its perceived linear size in meters or apparent linear size Perceived visual angle 8 edit The perceived endpoint at B has the perceived direction d B and the observer might simply say it looks straight ahead and toward the horizon This concept of the subjective visual direction is very old 2 However as Wade Ono amp Mapp 2006 noted it unfortunately has been ignored in many current theories of size perception and size illusions The object s other perceived endpoint A has a perceived direction d A about which the observer might say it appears toward a higher elevation than point B The difference between the two perceived directions d A d B is the perceived visual angle 8 also called the perceived angular size or apparent angular size It is not easy to quantify 8 For instance a well trained observer might say that point A looks about 25 degrees higher than B but most cannot reliably say how large a direction difference looks That skill is not practiced because it is easier to use pointing gestures Ono 1970 For example one often tells another person about the change in the directions seen for two viewed points by pointing something say a finger or the eyes from one point to the other Therefore in some experiments the observers aimed a pointer from one viewed point to the other so the angle through which the pointer rotated was the measure of 8 Komodo 1970 Komodo amp Ono 1974 Ono Muter amp Mitson 1974 Gogel amp Eby 1997 Also because 8 specifies the amount by which one should rotate one s eye to quickly look from one seen point to another eye tracking saccade observers in other experiments shifted their gaze from one object endpoint to the other and the angle the eye rotated through was measured as 8 for that object Yarbus 1967 Difference between 8 and S edit It is important to understand how 8 differs from S Consider an example illustrated by the sketch at the right nbsp Suppose one is looking through a window at a 30 foot wide 9 1 m house 240 feet away so it subtends a visual angle of about 7 degrees The 30 inch wide 760 mm window opening is 10 feet away so it subtends a visual angle of 14 degrees It can be said that the house looks larger and farther away than the window meaning that the perceived linear size S for the house s width is much larger than S for the window for instance a person might say the house looks about 40 feet wide and the window looks about 3 feet wide One can also say that the house looks smaller and farther away than the window and that does not contradict the other statement because now we mean that the amount 8 by which directions of the house s edges appear to differ is say about half the apparent direction difference for the window edges Notice that humans experience both the linear size and the angular size comparisons at the same time along with the distance comparison Joynson 1949 Thus any report merely that one object looks larger than another object is ambiguous It needs to specify whether looks larger refers to the perceived angular size 8 or to the perceived linear size S or to both of those qualitatively different size experiences Joynson 1949 McCready 1965 1985 Ono 1970 Notice that in everyday conversations looks larger often refers to an angular size comparison rather than a linear size comparison Additional confusion has resulted from widespread use of the ambiguous terms apparent size and perceived size because they sometimes have referred to 8 and sometimes to S without clarification so the reader must try to ascertain what they mean Also in astronomy apparent size refers to the physical angle 8 rather than to the subjective apparent visual angle 8 The perceptual size distance invariance hypothesis edit How the three perceived values 8 S and D would be expected to relate to each other for a given object is illustrated by Figure 2 and stated by the following equation McCready 1965 1985 Ono 1970 Komoda and Ono 1974 Reed 1989 Kaneko amp Uchikawa 1997 S D tan 8 displaystyle S D tan theta nbsp Ross amp Plug 2002 Page 31 dubbed this new rule the perceptual size distance invariance hypothesis Retinal size cortical size and 8 editAs already noted the magnitude of an object s visual angle 8 determines the size R of its retinal image And the size of the retinal image normally determines the extent of the neural activity pattern the retina s neural activity eventually generates in the primary visual cortex area V1 or Brodmann area 17 This cortical area harbors a distorted but spatially isomorphic map of the retina see Retinotopy This neurological relationship recently was confirmed by Murray Boyaci amp Kersten 2006 using functional magnetic resonance imaging The retinal image is not perceived or sensed That is experimental psychologists long ago rejected any idea that people sense a proximal stimulus such as the retinal image As Gogel 1969 1997 has repeatedly emphasized there is no sensation which could be called the perceived retinal image size R Also rejected is a popular idea that an object s perceived size results from a scaling of retinal size an illogical process that somehow magnifies the very small retinal size to yield the viewed object s much larger perceived linear size S Instead the physical retinal extent R normally determines the magnitude of the perceived visual angle 8 But as already noted other factors can intervene to slightly change 8 for a target forming a constant sized retinal image and thereby create a visual angle illusion Indeed the major discovery by Murray et al 2006 concerns this flexible relationship between R and 8 as described below Visual angle illusions and area V1 edit The Murray et al 2006 observers viewed a flat picture with two disks that subtended the same visual angle 8 and formed retinal images of the same size R but the perceived angular size 8 for one disk was larger than 8 for the other say 17 larger due to differences in their background patterns And in cortical Area V1 the sizes of the activity patterns related to the disks were unequal despite the fact that the retinal images were the same size The difference between these cortical sizes in Area V1 for the illusion disks was essentially the same as the difference produced by two non illusory disks whose retinal image sizes differed by say 17 The researchers pointed out that their findings dramatically disagree with the hypothetical models of neural events being proposed in nearly all current theories of visual spatial perception Murray et al 2006 also noted that the flat illusion pattern they used can represent other classic size illusions such as the Ponzo illusion and as well the moon illusion which is a visual angle illusion for most observers McCready 1965 1986 Restle 1970 Plug amp Ross 1989 p 21 Ross amp Plug 2002 A detailed meta analysis of the Murray et al 2006 results is available in McCready 2007 Appendix B The size distance paradox editThe classical size distance invariance hypothesis edit Conventional textbook theories of size and distance perception do not refer to the perceived visual angle e g Gregory 1963 1970 1998 2008 and some researchers even deny that it exists Kaufman amp Kaufman 2002 This idea that one does not see the different directions in which objects lie from oneself is a basis of the so called size distance invariance hypothesis SDIH That old SDIH logic geometry is typically illustrated using a diagram that resembles Figure 2 but has the physical visual angle 8 substituted for the perceived visual angle 8 The equation for the SDIH thus is S D tan 8 displaystyle S D tan theta nbsp Here S is typically called the perceived size or apparent size more precisely it is the perceived linear size measured in meters When rearranged as S D tan 8 the equation expresses Emmert s law However at least since 1962 researchers have pointed out that many classic size and distance illusions can be neither described nor explained using the SDIH so a new hypothesis is needed Boring 1962 Gruber 1956 McCready 1965 Baird 1970 Ono 1970 For instance consider the simple Ebbinghaus illusion Example the Ebbinghaus illusion edit Main article Ebbinghaus illusion nbsp The two orange circles are exactly the same size however the one on the left seems smaller The two central circles are the same linear size S and the same viewing distance D so they subtend the same visual angle 8 and form equal sized retinal images But the lower one looks larger than the upper one According to the SDIH looks larger can mean only that S is greater and with the physical angle 8 the same for both the SDIH requires that D be greater for the lower one than for the upper one However for most observers both circles appear unequal while also appearing at the same distance on the same page This commonly found disagreement between published data and the SDIH is known as the size distance paradox Gruber 1956 Ono et al 1974 The paradox completely vanishes however when the illusion is described instead as basically a visual angle illusion That is the perceived visual angle 8 is larger for the lower circle than for the upper circle It is as if its retinal image were larger So according to the new perceptual invariance hypothesis S D tan 8 with 8 larger for the lower circle and with D correctly the same for both circles then S becomes larger for the lower one by the same ratio that 8 is larger That is the reason the lower one looks a larger linear size on the page is because it looks a larger angular size than the upper one Explaining visual angle illusions remains difficult edit The new hypothesis that includes 8 along with S describes the Ebbinghaus illusion and many other classic size illusions more completely and more logically than does the popular SDIH What still needs to be explained however is why the basic visual angle illusion occurs in each example Describing the few existing explanations for visual angle illusions is beyond the scope of this present entry The most recent theories have been presented mostly in articles concerning the moon illusion Baird et al 1990 Enright 1989a 1989b Hershenson 1982 1989b Higashiyama 1992 McCready 1986 1999 2007 Plug amp Ross 1989 Reed 1989 Roscoe 1989 and especially in two moon illusion books Hershenson 1989 Ross amp Plug 2002 which make it quite clear that vision scientists have not yet agreed upon any particular theory of visual angle illusions There also is the lesser known but evidently the largest visual angle illusion of oculomotor micropsia convergence micropsia for which a few different explanations are being considered McCready 1965 2007 Ono 1970 Komoda amp Ono 1974 Ono et al 1974 Enright 1987b 1989a 1989b This is a partial list of size and distance illusions that begin as visual angle illusions angular size illusions for most observers Moon illusion Oculomotor micropsia convergence micropsia Ebbinghaus illusion Titchner circles Hering illusion Ponzo illusion Muller Lyer illusion Orbison illusion Jastrow illusion Wundt illusion Curvature of the apparent fronto parallel plane AFPP Notes edit In some theories the cyclopean eye is in effect approximately midway between where one feels one s eye are located in one s body image of one s head Ono 1970 Ono Mapp amp Howard 2002 Some other theories define the place from which one feels one is viewing the world as the visual egocenter Roelofs 19xx McCready 1964 1965 Sakuma amp Pfaff 1979 which among observers ranges in effect from about midway between the eyes to at least as far back as the center of the head about 4 inches behind the eyes approximately midway between the two ears on the axis for horizontal head rotations The subjective experiences of visual directions were fully researched by Ewald Hering 1942 1879 and by Hermann von Helmholtz 1962 1910 who distinguished between the perceived oculocentric directions and the perceived egocentric directions They and other theorists have pointed out that a viewed point s egocentric direction d B and d A here is determined by a process that necessarily combines the position of the point s image on the retina with information about the position of the eye with respect to the head and body References editBaird J C 1970 Psychophysical analysis of visual space Oxford London Pergamon Press Baird J C Wagner M Fuld K 1990 A simple but powerful theory of the moon illusion Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 16 3 675 677 doi 10 1037 0096 1523 16 3 675 PMID 2144580 Barbeito R Ono H 1979 Four methods of locating the egocenter a comparison of their predictive validities and reliabilities Behav Res Methods Instrum 11 31 36 doi 10 3758 bf03205428 Enright J T 1987a Art and the oculomotor system Perspective illustrations evoke vergence changes Perception 16 6 731 746 doi 10 1068 p160731 PMID 3454431 S2CID 34545108 Enright J T 1987b Perspective vergence Oculomotor responses to line drawings Vision Research 27 9 1513 1526 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 211 4341 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 87 90160 X PMID 3445485 S2CID 9183517 Enright J T 1989a Manipulating stereopsis and vergence in an outdoor setting Moon sky and horizon Vision Research 29 12 1815 1824 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 89 90162 4 PMID 2631401 S2CID 26009881 Enright J T 1989b 4 The eye the brain and the size of the moon Toward a unified oculomotor hypothesis for the moon illusion in Hershenson M ed The Moon Illusion Hillsdale NJ L Earlbaum Gogel W C 1969 The sensing of retinal size Vision Research 9 9 1079 94 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 69 90049 2 PMID 5350376 Gogel W C Eby D W 1997 Measures of perceived linear size sagittal motion and visual angle from optical expansions and contractions Perception amp Psychophysics 59 5 783 806 doi 10 3758 BF03206024 PMID 9259645 Gregory R L 1963 Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling Nature 199 4894 678 680 doi 10 1038 199678a0 PMID 14074555 S2CID 4153670 Gregory R L 1970 The intelligent eye New York McGraw Hill Gregory R L 1998 Eye and brain 5th ed Oxford Oxford University Press Gregory R L 2008 Emmert s law and the moon illusion Spatial Vision 21 3 5 407 420 n doi 10 1163 156856808784532509 PMID 18534112 Gruber H E 1956 The size distance paradox A reply to Gilinsky American Journal of Psychology 69 3 469 476 doi 10 2307 1419056 JSTOR 1419056 PMID 13354816 Helmholtz H von 1962 1910 translated by Southall J P C ed Treatise on physiological optics vol 3 New York Dover Hering E 1977 1879 The Theory of Binocular Vision New York Plenum Press translation Hershenson M 1982 Moon illusion and spiral aftereffect Illusions due to the loom zoom system Journal of Experimental Psychology General 111 4 423 440 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 111 4 423 PMID 6219186 Hershenson M 1989 5 Moon illusion as anomaly in Hershenson M ed The Moon Illusion Hillsdale NJ L Earlbaum Higashiyama A 1992 Anisotropic perception of visual angle Implications for the horizontal vertical illusion overconstancy of size and the moon illusion Perception amp Psychophysics 51 3 218 230 doi 10 3758 BF03212248 PMID 1561047 Higashiyama A Shimono K 1994 How accurate is size and distance perception for very far terrestrial objects Perception amp Psychophysics 55 4 429 442 doi 10 3758 BF03205300 PMID 8036122 Joynson R B 1949 The problem of size and distance Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 3 119 135 doi 10 1080 17470214908416754 S2CID 144334816 Kaneko H Uchikawa K 1997 Perceived angular size and linear size the role of binocular disparity and visual surround Perception 26 1 17 27 doi 10 1068 p260017 PMID 9196687 S2CID 41489059 Kaufman L Kaufman J H 2000 Explaining the moon illusion Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97 1 500 505 doi 10 1073 pnas 97 1 500 PMC 26692 PMID 10618447 Komoda M K Ono H 1974 Oculomotor adjustments and size distance perception Perception amp Psychophysics 15 2 353 360 doi 10 3758 BF03213958 McCready D 1963 Visual acuity under conditions that induce size illusions Doctoral dissertation University of Michigan See Dissertation Abstracts International 1964 24 5573 McCready D 1964 Location of the Visual Egocenter Paper presented at meeting of the Midwestern Section of the Association for Research in Ophthalmology Rochester MN May 1964 McCready D 1965 Size distance perception and accommodation convergence micropsia A critique Vision Research 5 3 189 206 doi 10 1016 0042 6989 65 90065 9 PMID 5862949 McCready D 1983 Moon Illusions and Other Visual Illusions Redefined Psychology Department Report University of Wisconsin Whitewater p 86 McCready D 1985 On size distance and visual angle perception Perception amp Psychophysics 37 4 323 334 doi 10 3758 BF03211355 PMID 4034350 McCready D 1986 Moon illusions redescribed Perception amp Psychophysics 39 1 64 72 doi 10 3758 BF03207585 PMID 3703663 McCready D 1994 Toward the Distance Cue Theory of Visual Angle Illusions Psychology Department Report University of Wisconsin Whitewater p 40 McCready D 1999 2007 The moon illusion explained PDF Murray S O Boyaci H Kersten D March 1 2006 The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex PDF Nature Neuroscience 9 3 429 434 doi 10 1038 nn1641 PMID 16462737 S2CID 8391410 archived from the original PDF on March 18 2015 Ono H 1970 Some thoughts on different perceptual tasks related to size and distance in Baird J C ed Human space perception Proceedings of the Dartmouth conference Psychonomic Monograph Supplement vol 3 13 Whole No 45 Ono H Mapp A P Howard I P 2002 The cyclopean eye in vision The new and old data continue to hit you right between the eyes Vision Research 42 10 1307 1324 doi 10 1016 S0042 6989 01 00281 4 PMID 12044760 S2CID 8467032 Ono H Muter P Mitson L 1974 Size distance paradox with accommodative micropsia Perception amp Psychophysics 15 2 301 307 doi 10 3758 BF03213948 Oyama T 1977 Feature analysers optical illusions and figural aftereffects Perception 6 4 401 406 doi 10 1068 p060401 PMID 917729 S2CID 21941416 Plug C Ross H E 1989 2 Historical Review in Hershenson M ed The Moon Illusion Hillsdale NJ L Earlbaum Plug C Ross H E 1994 The natural moon illusion A multifactor angular account Perception 23 3 321 333 doi 10 1068 p230321 PMID 7971109 S2CID 41311235 Reed C F 1984 Terrestrial passage theory of the moon illusion Journal of Experimental Psychology General 113 4 489 500 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 113 4 489 PMID 6240520 Reed C F 1989 11 Terrestrial and celestial passage in Hershenson M ed The Moon Illusion Hillsdale NJ L Earlbaum Restle F 1970 Moon illusion explained on the basis of relative size Science 167 3921 1092 1096 doi 10 1126 science 167 3921 1092 PMID 17829398 Rock I McDermott W 1964 The perception of visual angle Acta Psychologica 22 119 134 doi 10 1016 0001 6918 64 90011 3 Roelofs C O 1959 Considerations on the visual egocenter Acta Psychologica 16 226 234 doi 10 1016 0001 6918 59 90096 4 Roscoe S N 1985 Bigness is in the eye of the beholder Human Factors 27 6 615 636 doi 10 1177 001872088502700601 PMID 3914446 S2CID 34272970 Roscoe S N 1989 3 The zoom lens hypothesis in Hershenson M ed The Moon Illusion Hillsdale NJ L Earlbaum Ross H E Plug C 2002 The mystery of the moon illusion Exploring size perception Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 850862 5 Sakuma Y Pfaff W 1979 Considerations on the visual egocentre Acta Psychologica 16 226 234 doi 10 1016 0001 6918 59 90096 4 Wade N J Ono H Mapp A P 2006 The lost direction in binocular vision The neglected signs posted by Walls Towne and Leconte Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 42 1 61 86 doi 10 1002 jhbs 20135 PMID 16345004 Yarbus A L 1967 Eye Movements and Vision New York Plenum Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Perceived visual angle amp oldid 1044077008, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.