fbpx
Wikipedia

Blinded experiment

In a blind or blinded experiment, information which may influence the participants of the experiment is withheld until after the experiment is complete. Good blinding can reduce or eliminate experimental biases that arise from a participants' expectations, observer's effect on the participants, observer bias, confirmation bias, and other sources. A blind can be imposed on any participant of an experiment, including subjects, researchers, technicians, data analysts, and evaluators. In some cases, while blinding would be useful, it is impossible or unethical. For example, it is not possible to blind a patient to their treatment in a physical therapy intervention. A good clinical protocol ensures that blinding is as effective as possible within ethical and practical constraints.

During the course of an experiment, a participant becomes unblinded if they deduce or otherwise obtain information that has been masked to them. For example, a patient who experiences a side effect may correctly guess their treatment, becoming unblinded. Unblinding is common in blinded experiments, particularly in pharmacological trials. In particular, trials on pain medication and antidepressants are poorly blinded. Unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of a study is a source of experimental error, as the bias that was eliminated by blinding is re-introduced. The CONSORT reporting guidelines recommend that all studies assess and report unblinding. In practice, very few studies do so.[1]

Blinding is an important tool of the scientific method, and is used in many fields of research. In some fields, such as medicine, it is considered essential.[2] In clinical research, a trial that is not a blinded trial is called an open trial.

History edit

The first known blind experiment was conducted by the French Royal Commission on Animal Magnetism in 1784 to investigate the claims of mesmerism as proposed by Charles d'Eslon, a former associate of Franz Mesmer. In the investigations, the researchers (physically) blindfolded mesmerists and asked them to identify objects that the experimenters had previously filled with "vital fluid". The subjects were unable to do so.[citation needed]

In 1817, the first blind experiment recorded to have occurred outside of a scientific setting compared the musical quality of a Stradivarius violin to one with a guitar-like design. A violinist played each instrument while a committee of scientists and musicians listened from another room so as to avoid prejudice.[3][4]

An early example of a double-blind protocol was the Nuremberg salt test of 1835 performed by Friedrich Wilhelm von Hoven, Nuremberg's highest-ranking public health official,[5] as well as a close friend of Friedrich Schiller.[6] This trial contested the effectiveness of homeopathic dilution.[5]

In 1865, Claude Bernard published his Introduction to the Study of Experimental Medicine, which advocated for the blinding of researchers.[7] Bernard's recommendation that an experiment's observer should not know the hypothesis being tested contrasted starkly with the prevalent Enlightenment-era attitude that scientific observation can only be objectively valid when undertaken by a well-educated, informed scientist.[8] The first study recorded to have a blinded researcher was conducted in 1907 by W. H. R. Rivers and H. N. Webber to investigate the effects of caffeine.[9] The need to blind researchers became widely recognized in the mid-20th century.[10]

Background edit

Bias edit

A number of biases are present when a study is insufficiently blinded. Patient-reported outcomes can be different if the patient is not blinded to their treatment.[11] Likewise, failure to blind researchers results in observer bias.[12] Unblinded data analysts may favor an analysis that supports their existing beliefs (confirmation bias). These biases are typically the result of subconscious influences, and are present even when study participants believe they are not influenced by them.[13]

Terminology edit

In medical research, the terms single-blind, double-blind and triple-blind are commonly used to describe blinding. These terms describe experiments in which (respectively) one, two, or three parties are blinded to some information. Most often, single-blind studies blind patients to their treatment allocation, double-blind studies blind both patients and researchers to treatment allocations, and triple-blinded studies blind patients, researcher, and some other third party (such as a monitoring committee) to treatment allocations. However, the meaning of these terms can vary from study to study.[14]

CONSORT guidelines state that these terms should no longer be used because they are ambiguous. For instance, "double-blind" could mean that the data analysts and patients were blinded; or the patients and outcome assessors were blinded; or the patients and people offering the intervention were blinded, etc. The terms also fail to convey the information that was masked and the amount of unblinding that occurred. It is not sufficient to specify the number of parties that have been blinded. To describe an experiment's blinding, it is necessary to report who has been blinded to what information, and how well each blind succeeded.[15]

Unblinding edit

"Unblinding" occurs in a blinded experiment when information becomes available to one from whom it has been masked. In clinical studies, unblinding may occur unintentionally when a patient deduces their treatment group. Unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of an experiment is a source of bias. Some degree of premature unblinding is common in blinded experiments.[16] When a blind is imperfect, its success is judged on a spectrum with no blind (or complete failure of blinding) on one end, perfect blinding on the other, and poor or good blinding between. Thus, the common view of studies as blinded or unblinded is an example of a false dichotomy.[17]

Success of blinding is assessed by questioning study participants about information that has been masked to them (e.g. did the participant receive the drug or placebo?). In a perfectly blinded experiment, the responses should be consistent with no knowledge of the masked information. However, if unblinding has occurred, the responses will indicate the degree of unblinding. Since unblinding cannot be measured directly, but must be inferred from participants' responses, its measured value will depend on the nature of the questions asked. As a result, it is not possible to measure unblinding in a way that is completely objective. Nonetheless, it is still possible to make informed judgments about the quality of a blind. Poorly blinded studies rank above unblinded studies and below well-blinded studies in the hierarchy of evidence.[18]

Post-study unblinding edit

Post-study unblinding is the release of masked data upon completion of a study. In clinical studies, post-study unblinding serves to inform subjects of their treatment allocation. Removing a blind upon completion of a study is never mandatory, but is typically performed as a courtesy to study participants. Unblinding that occurs after the conclusion of a study is not a source of bias, because data collection and analysis are both complete at this time.[19]

Premature unblinding edit

Premature unblinding is any unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of a study. In contrast with post-study unblinding, premature unblinding is a source of bias. A code-break procedure dictates when a subject should be unblinded prematurely. A code-break procedure should only allow for unblinding in cases of emergency. Unblinding that occurs in compliance with code-break procedure is strictly documented and reported.[20]

Premature unblinding may also occur when a participant infers from experimental conditions information that has been masked to them. A common cause for unblinding is the presence of side effects (or effects) in the treatment group. In pharmacological trials, premature unblinding can be reduced with the use of an active placebo, which conceals treatment allocation by ensuring the presence of side effects in both groups.[21] However, side effects are not the only cause of unblinding; any perceptible difference between the treatment and control groups can contribute to premature unblinding.[citation needed]

A problem arises in the assessment of blinding because asking subjects to guess masked information may prompt them to try to infer that information. Researchers speculate that this may contribute to premature unblinding.[22] Furthermore, it has been reported that some subjects of clinical trials attempt to determine if they have received an active treatment by gathering information on social media and message boards. While researchers counsel patients not to use social media to discuss clinical trials, their accounts are not monitored. This behavior is believed to be a source of unblinding.[23] CONSORT standards and good clinical practice guidelines recommend the reporting of all premature unblinding.[24][25] In practice, unintentional unblinding is rarely reported.[1]

Significance edit

Bias due to poor blinding tends to favor the experimental group, resulting in inflated effect size and risk of false positives.[24] Success or failure of blinding is rarely reported or measured; it is implicitly assumed that experiments reported as "blind" are truly blind.[1] Critics have pointed out that without assessment and reporting, there is no way to know if a blind succeeded. This shortcoming is especially concerning given that even a small error in blinding can produce a statistically significant result in the absence of any real difference between test groups when a study is sufficiently powered (i.e. statistical significance is not robust to bias). As such, many statistically significant results in randomized controlled trials may be caused by error in blinding.[26] Some researchers have called for the mandatory assessment of blinding efficacy in clinical trials.[18]

Applications edit

In medicine edit

Blinding is considered essential in medicine,[27] but is often difficult to achieve. For example, it is difficult to compare surgical and non-surgical interventions in blind trials. In some cases, sham surgery may be necessary for the blinding process. A good clinical protocol ensures that blinding is as effective as possible within ethical and practical constrains.

Studies of blinded pharmacological trials across widely varying domains find evidence of high levels of unblinding. Unblinding has been shown to affect both patients and clinicians. This evidence challenges the common assumption that blinding is highly effective in pharmacological trials. Unblinding has also been documented in clinical trials outside of pharmacology.[28]

Pain edit

A 2018 meta-analysis found that assessment of blinding was reported in only 23 out of 408 randomized controlled trials for chronic pain (5.6%). The study concluded upon analysis of pooled data that the overall quality of the blinding was poor, and the blinding was "not successful." Additionally, both pharmaceutical sponsorship and the presence of side effects were associated with lower rates of reporting assessment of blinding.[29]

Depression edit

Studies have found evidence of extensive unblinding in antidepressant trials: at least three-quarters of patients were able to correctly guess their treatment assignment.[30] Unblinding also occurs in clinicians.[31] Better blinding of patients and clinicians reduces effect size. Researchers concluded that unblinding inflates effect size in antidepressant trials.[32][33][34] Some researchers believe that antidepressants are not effective for the treatment of depression and only outperform placebos due to systematic error. These researchers argue that antidepressants are just active placebos.[35][36]

Acupuncture edit

While the possibility of blinded trials on acupuncture is controversial, a 2003 review of 47 randomized controlled trials found no fewer than four methods of blinding patients to acupuncture treatment: 1) superficial needling of true acupuncture points, 2) use of acupuncture points which are not indicated for the condition being treated, 3) insertion of needles outside of true acupuncture points, and 4) the use of placebo needles which are designed not to penetrate the skin. The authors concluded that there was "no clear association between type of sham intervention used and the results of the trials."[37]

A 2018 study on acupuncture which used needles that did not penetrate the skin as a sham treatment found that 68% of patients and 83% of acupuncturists correctly identified their group allocation. The authors concluded that the blinding had failed, but that more advanced placebos may someday offer the possibility of well-blinded studies in acupuncture.[38]

In physics edit

It is standard practice in physics to perform blinded data analysis. After data analysis is complete, one is allowed to unblind the data. A prior agreement to publish the data regardless of the results of the analysis may be made to prevent publication bias.[13]

In social sciences edit

Social science research is particularly prone to observer bias, so it is important in these fields to properly blind the researchers. In some cases, while blind experiments would be useful, they are impractical or unethical. Blinded data analysis can reduce bias, but is rarely used in social science research.[39]

In forensics edit

In a police photo lineup, an officer shows a group of photos to a witness and asks the witness to identify the individual who committed the crime. Since the officer is typically aware of who the suspect is, they may (subconsciously or consciously) influence the witness to choose the individual that they believe committed the crime. There is a growing movement in law enforcement to move to a blind procedure in which the officer who shows the photos to the witness does not know who the suspect is.[40][41]

In music edit

Auditions for symphony orchestras take place behind a curtain so that the judges cannot see the performer. Blinding the judges to the gender of the performers has been shown to increase the hiring of women.[42] Blind tests can also be used to compare the quality of musical instruments.[43][44]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Bello, Segun; Moustgaard, Helene; Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn (October 2014). "The risk of unblinding was infrequently and incompletely reported in 300 randomized clinical trial publications". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 67 (10): 1059–1069. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.05.007. ISSN 1878-5921. PMID 24973822.
  2. ^ "Oxford Centre for Evidence-based Medicine - Levels of Evidence (March 2009) - CEBM". cebm.net. 11 June 2009. from the original on 26 October 2017. Retrieved 2 May 2018.
  3. ^ Fétis FJ (1868). Biographie Universelle des Musiciens et Bibliographie Générale de la Musique, Tome 1 (Second ed.). Paris: Firmin Didot Frères, Fils, et Cie. p. 249. Retrieved 2011-07-21.
  4. ^ Dubourg G (1852). The Violin: Some Account of That Leading Instrument and its Most Eminent Professors... (Fourth ed.). London: Robert Cocks and Co. pp. 356–357. Retrieved 2011-07-21.
  5. ^ a b Stolberg, M. (December 2006). "Inventing the randomized double-blind trial: the Nuremberg salt test of 1835". Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 99 (12): 642–643. doi:10.1177/014107680609901216. PMC 1676327. PMID 17139070.
  6. ^ Biographie Des Doctor Friedrich Wilhelm Von Hoven (1840), ISBN 1104040891
  7. ^ Bernard, Claude; Dagognet, François (2008). Introduction à l'étude de la médecine expérimentale. Champs. Paris: Flammarion. ISBN 978-2-08-121793-5.
  8. ^ Daston L (2005). "Scientific Error and the Ethos of Belief". Social Research. 72 (1): 18. doi:10.1353/sor.2005.0016. S2CID 141036212.
  9. ^ Rivers WH, Webber HN (August 1907). "The action of caffeine on the capacity for muscular work". The Journal of Physiology. 36 (1): 33–47. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1907.sp001215. PMC 1533733. PMID 16992882.
  10. ^ Alder K (2006). Kramer LS, Maza SC (eds.). A Companion to Western Historical Thought. Blackwell Companions to History. Wiley-Blackwell. p. 307. ISBN 978-1-4051-4961-7. Retrieved 2012-02-11. Shortly after the start of the Cold War [...] double-blind reviews became the norm for conducting scientific medical research, as well as the means by which peers evaluated scholarship, both in science and in history. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  11. ^ Hróbjartsson, A; Emanuelsson, F; Skou Thomsen, AS; Hilden, J; Brorson, S (August 2014). "Bias due to lack of patient blinding in clinical trials. A systematic review of trials randomizing patients to blind and nonblind sub-studies". International Journal of Epidemiology. 43 (4): 1272–83. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu115. PMC 4258786. PMID 24881045.
  12. ^ Bello, S; Krogsbøll, LT; Gruber, J; Zhao, ZJ; Fischer, D; Hróbjartsson, A (September 2014). "Lack of blinding of outcome assessors in animal model experiments implies risk of observer bias". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 67 (9): 973–83. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.04.008. PMID 24972762.
  13. ^ a b MacCoun, Robert; Perlmutter, Saul (7 October 2015). "Blind analysis: Hide results to seek the truth". Nature. 526 (7572): 187–189. Bibcode:2015Natur.526..187M. doi:10.1038/526187a. PMID 26450040.
  14. ^ Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Altman DG (February 2002). "The landscape and lexicon of blinding in randomized trials". Annals of Internal Medicine. 136 (3): 254–9. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-136-3-200202050-00022. PMID 11827510. S2CID 34932997.
  15. ^ Moher, David; Hopewell, Sally; Schulz, Kenneth F.; Montori, Victor; Gøtzsche, Peter C.; Devereaux, P. J.; Elbourne, Diana; Egger, Matthias; Altman, Douglas G. (23 March 2010). "CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials". BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.). 340: c869. doi:10.1136/bmj.c869. ISSN 1756-1833. PMC 2844943. PMID 20332511.
  16. ^ Bello, Segun; Moustgaard, Helene; Hróbjartsson, Asbjørn (2017). "Unreported formal assessment of unblinding occurred in 4 of 10 randomized clinical trials, unreported loss of blinding in 1 of 10 trials". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 81: 42–50. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.08.002. ISSN 1878-5921. PMID 27555081.
  17. ^ Schulz, Kenneth F.; Grimes, David A. (23 February 2002). "Blinding in randomised trials: hiding who got what". Lancet. 359 (9307): 696–700. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07816-9. ISSN 0140-6736. PMID 11879884. S2CID 11578262.
  18. ^ a b Kolahi, J; Bang, H; Park, J (December 2009). "Towards a proposal for assessment of blinding success in clinical trials: up-to-date review". Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology. 37 (6): 477–84. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0528.2009.00494.x. ISSN 1600-0528. PMC 3044082. PMID 19758415.
  19. ^ Dinnett EM, Mungall MM, Kent JA, Ronald ES, McIntyre KE, Anderson E, Gaw A (2005). "Unblinding of trial participants to their treatment allocation: lessons from the Prospective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER)". Clin Trials. 2 (3): 254–259. doi:10.1191/1740774505cn089oa. PMID 16279148. S2CID 36252366.
  20. ^ Quittell, Lynne M. (3 October 2018). "The Scientific and Social Implications of Unblinding a Study Subject". The American Journal of Bioethics. 18 (10): 71–73. doi:10.1080/15265161.2018.1513589. ISSN 1526-5161. PMID 30339067. S2CID 53014880.
  21. ^ Double, D. B. (19 October 1996). "Placebo mania. Placebo controlled trials are needed to provide data on effectiveness of active treatment". BMJ: British Medical Journal. 313 (7063): 1008–9. doi:10.1136/bmj.313.7063.1008b. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 2352320. PMID 8892442.
  22. ^ Rees, Judy R.; Wade, Timothy J.; Levy, Deborah A.; Colford, John M.; Hilton, Joan F. (February 2005). "Changes in beliefs identify unblinding in randomized controlled trials: a method to meet CONSORT guidelines". Contemporary Clinical Trials. 26 (1): 25–37. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2004.11.020. PMID 15837450.
  23. ^ Ledford, Heidi. "A question of Control" (PDF). nature.com. Nature Magazine. Retrieved 24 April 2019.
  24. ^ a b Moher, David; Altman, Douglas G.; Schulz, Kenneth F. (24 March 2010). "CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials". BMJ. 340: c332. doi:10.1136/bmj.c332. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 2844940. PMID 20332509.
  25. ^ "E6(R2) Good Clinical Practice: Integrated Addendum to ICH E6(R1) Guidance for Industry" (PDF). fda.gov. 2019-04-05. Retrieved 21 April 2019.
  26. ^ Siegfried, Tom (2010). "Odds are, it's wrong: Science fails to face the shortcomings of statistics". Science News. 177 (7): 26–29. doi:10.1002/scin.5591770721. ISSN 1943-0930.
  27. ^ "Oxford Centre for Evidence-based Medicine - Levels of Evidence (March 2009) - CEBM". cebm.net. 11 June 2009. from the original on 26 October 2017. Retrieved 2 May 2018.
  28. ^ JUL 2009, The Pharmaceutical Journal31 (31 July 2009). "An example of problems that arise from clinical trials and how to avoid them". Pharmaceutical Journal. 283: 129–130. Retrieved 24 April 2019.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  29. ^ Colagiuri, Ben; Sharpe, Louise; Scott, Amelia (September 2018). "The Blind Leading the Not-So-Blind: A Meta-Analysis of Blinding in Pharmacological Trials for Chronic Pain". The Journal of Pain. 20 (5): 489–500. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2018.09.002. ISSN 1526-5900. PMID 30248448. S2CID 52813251. Retrieved 22 April 2019.
  30. ^ Perlis, Roy H.; Ostacher, Michael; Fava, Maurizio; Nierenberg, Andrew A.; Sachs, Gary S.; Rosenbaum, Jerrold F. (2010). "Assuring that double-blind is blind". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 167 (3): 250–252. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060820. ISSN 1535-7228. PMID 20194487. S2CID 207628021.
  31. ^ White, K.; Kando, J.; Park, T.; Waternaux, C.; Brown, W. A. (December 1992). "Side effects and the "blindability" of clinical drug trials". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 149 (12): 1730–1731. doi:10.1176/ajp.149.12.1730. ISSN 0002-953X. PMID 1443253.
  32. ^ Moncrieff, Joanna; Wessely, Simon; Hardy, Rebecca (2 January 2018). "Meta-analysis of trials comparing antidepressants with active placebos". British Journal of Psychiatry. 172 (3): 227–231. doi:10.1192/bjp.172.3.227. ISSN 0007-1250. PMID 9614471. S2CID 4975797.
  33. ^ Greenberg, RP; Bornstein, RF; Greenberg, MD; Fisher, S (October 1992). "A meta-analysis of antidepressant outcome under "blinder" conditions". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 60 (5): 664–9, discussion 670–7. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.60.5.664. ISSN 0022-006X. PMID 1401382.
  34. ^ Moncrieff, J; Wessely, S; Hardy, R (2004). "Active placebos versus antidepressants for depression". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2012 (1): CD003012. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003012.pub2. ISSN 1469-493X. PMC 8407353. PMID 14974002.
  35. ^ Ioannidis, JP (27 May 2008). "Effectiveness of antidepressants: an evidence myth constructed from a thousand randomized trials?". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 3: 14. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-3-14. ISSN 1747-5341. PMC 2412901. PMID 18505564.
  36. ^ Kirsch, Irving (2014). "Antidepressants and the Placebo Effect". Zeitschrift für Psychologie. 222 (3): 128–134. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000176. ISSN 2190-8370. PMC 4172306. PMID 25279271.
  37. ^ Dincer, F; Linde, K. (December 2003). "Sham interventions in randomized clinical trials of acupuncture—a review". Complementary Therapies in Medicine. 11 (4): 235–242. doi:10.1016/S0965-2299(03)00124-9. PMID 15022656.
  38. ^ Vase, L; Baram, S; Takakura, N; Takayama, M; Yajima, H; Kawase, A; Schuster, L; Kaptchuk, TJ; Schou, S; Jensen, TS; Zachariae, R; Svensson, P (2015). "Can acupuncture treatment be double-blinded? An evaluation of double-blind acupuncture treatment of postoperative pain". PLOS ONE. 10 (3): e0119612. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1019612V. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0119612. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4352029. PMID 25747157.
  39. ^ Sanders, Robert (2015-10-08). "'Blind analysis' could reduce bias in social science research". Berkeley News. Retrieved 2019-08-29.
  40. ^ Dittmann M (July–August 2004). "Accuracy and the accused: Psychologists work with law enforcement on research-based improvements to crime-suspect identification". Monitor on Psychology. 35 (7). American Psychological Association: 74.
  41. ^ Koerner BI (July–August 2002). "Under the Microscope". Legal Affairs. Retrieved 2 May 2018.
  42. ^ Miller CC (25 February 2016). "Is Blind Hiring the Best Hiring?". The New York Times. Retrieved 26 April 2019.
  43. ^ "Violinists can't tell the difference between Stradivarius violins and new ones" by Ed Yong, Discover, 2 January 2012.
  44. ^ Fritz, C. (2012). "Player preferences among new and old violins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (3): 760–63. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109..760F. doi:10.1073/pnas.1114999109. PMC 3271912. PMID 22215592.

blinded, experiment, blind, blinded, experiment, information, which, influence, participants, experiment, withheld, until, after, experiment, complete, good, blinding, reduce, eliminate, experimental, biases, that, arise, from, participants, expectations, obse. In a blind or blinded experiment information which may influence the participants of the experiment is withheld until after the experiment is complete Good blinding can reduce or eliminate experimental biases that arise from a participants expectations observer s effect on the participants observer bias confirmation bias and other sources A blind can be imposed on any participant of an experiment including subjects researchers technicians data analysts and evaluators In some cases while blinding would be useful it is impossible or unethical For example it is not possible to blind a patient to their treatment in a physical therapy intervention A good clinical protocol ensures that blinding is as effective as possible within ethical and practical constraints During the course of an experiment a participant becomes unblinded if they deduce or otherwise obtain information that has been masked to them For example a patient who experiences a side effect may correctly guess their treatment becoming unblinded Unblinding is common in blinded experiments particularly in pharmacological trials In particular trials on pain medication and antidepressants are poorly blinded Unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of a study is a source of experimental error as the bias that was eliminated by blinding is re introduced The CONSORT reporting guidelines recommend that all studies assess and report unblinding In practice very few studies do so 1 Blinding is an important tool of the scientific method and is used in many fields of research In some fields such as medicine it is considered essential 2 In clinical research a trial that is not a blinded trial is called an open trial Contents 1 History 2 Background 2 1 Bias 2 2 Terminology 3 Unblinding 3 1 Post study unblinding 3 2 Premature unblinding 3 3 Significance 4 Applications 4 1 In medicine 4 1 1 Pain 4 1 2 Depression 4 1 3 Acupuncture 4 2 In physics 4 3 In social sciences 4 4 In forensics 4 5 In music 5 See also 6 ReferencesHistory editThe first known blind experiment was conducted by the French Royal Commission on Animal Magnetism in 1784 to investigate the claims of mesmerism as proposed by Charles d Eslon a former associate of Franz Mesmer In the investigations the researchers physically blindfolded mesmerists and asked them to identify objects that the experimenters had previously filled with vital fluid The subjects were unable to do so citation needed In 1817 the first blind experiment recorded to have occurred outside of a scientific setting compared the musical quality of a Stradivarius violin to one with a guitar like design A violinist played each instrument while a committee of scientists and musicians listened from another room so as to avoid prejudice 3 4 An early example of a double blind protocol was the Nuremberg salt test of 1835 performed by Friedrich Wilhelm von Hoven Nuremberg s highest ranking public health official 5 as well as a close friend of Friedrich Schiller 6 This trial contested the effectiveness of homeopathic dilution 5 In 1865 Claude Bernard published his Introduction to the Study of Experimental Medicine which advocated for the blinding of researchers 7 Bernard s recommendation that an experiment s observer should not know the hypothesis being tested contrasted starkly with the prevalent Enlightenment era attitude that scientific observation can only be objectively valid when undertaken by a well educated informed scientist 8 The first study recorded to have a blinded researcher was conducted in 1907 by W H R Rivers and H N Webber to investigate the effects of caffeine 9 The need to blind researchers became widely recognized in the mid 20th century 10 Background editBias edit A number of biases are present when a study is insufficiently blinded Patient reported outcomes can be different if the patient is not blinded to their treatment 11 Likewise failure to blind researchers results in observer bias 12 Unblinded data analysts may favor an analysis that supports their existing beliefs confirmation bias These biases are typically the result of subconscious influences and are present even when study participants believe they are not influenced by them 13 Terminology edit Double blind redirects here Not to be confused with Double bind In medical research the terms single blind double blind and triple blind are commonly used to describe blinding These terms describe experiments in which respectively one two or three parties are blinded to some information Most often single blind studies blind patients to their treatment allocation double blind studies blind both patients and researchers to treatment allocations and triple blinded studies blind patients researcher and some other third party such as a monitoring committee to treatment allocations However the meaning of these terms can vary from study to study 14 CONSORT guidelines state that these terms should no longer be used because they are ambiguous For instance double blind could mean that the data analysts and patients were blinded or the patients and outcome assessors were blinded or the patients and people offering the intervention were blinded etc The terms also fail to convey the information that was masked and the amount of unblinding that occurred It is not sufficient to specify the number of parties that have been blinded To describe an experiment s blinding it is necessary to report who has been blinded to what information and how well each blind succeeded 15 Unblinding edit Unblinding occurs in a blinded experiment when information becomes available to one from whom it has been masked In clinical studies unblinding may occur unintentionally when a patient deduces their treatment group Unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of an experiment is a source of bias Some degree of premature unblinding is common in blinded experiments 16 When a blind is imperfect its success is judged on a spectrum with no blind or complete failure of blinding on one end perfect blinding on the other and poor or good blinding between Thus the common view of studies as blinded or unblinded is an example of a false dichotomy 17 Success of blinding is assessed by questioning study participants about information that has been masked to them e g did the participant receive the drug or placebo In a perfectly blinded experiment the responses should be consistent with no knowledge of the masked information However if unblinding has occurred the responses will indicate the degree of unblinding Since unblinding cannot be measured directly but must be inferred from participants responses its measured value will depend on the nature of the questions asked As a result it is not possible to measure unblinding in a way that is completely objective Nonetheless it is still possible to make informed judgments about the quality of a blind Poorly blinded studies rank above unblinded studies and below well blinded studies in the hierarchy of evidence 18 Post study unblinding edit Post study unblinding is the release of masked data upon completion of a study In clinical studies post study unblinding serves to inform subjects of their treatment allocation Removing a blind upon completion of a study is never mandatory but is typically performed as a courtesy to study participants Unblinding that occurs after the conclusion of a study is not a source of bias because data collection and analysis are both complete at this time 19 Premature unblinding edit Premature unblinding is any unblinding that occurs before the conclusion of a study In contrast with post study unblinding premature unblinding is a source of bias A code break procedure dictates when a subject should be unblinded prematurely A code break procedure should only allow for unblinding in cases of emergency Unblinding that occurs in compliance with code break procedure is strictly documented and reported 20 Premature unblinding may also occur when a participant infers from experimental conditions information that has been masked to them A common cause for unblinding is the presence of side effects or effects in the treatment group In pharmacological trials premature unblinding can be reduced with the use of an active placebo which conceals treatment allocation by ensuring the presence of side effects in both groups 21 However side effects are not the only cause of unblinding any perceptible difference between the treatment and control groups can contribute to premature unblinding citation needed A problem arises in the assessment of blinding because asking subjects to guess masked information may prompt them to try to infer that information Researchers speculate that this may contribute to premature unblinding 22 Furthermore it has been reported that some subjects of clinical trials attempt to determine if they have received an active treatment by gathering information on social media and message boards While researchers counsel patients not to use social media to discuss clinical trials their accounts are not monitored This behavior is believed to be a source of unblinding 23 CONSORT standards and good clinical practice guidelines recommend the reporting of all premature unblinding 24 25 In practice unintentional unblinding is rarely reported 1 Significance edit Bias due to poor blinding tends to favor the experimental group resulting in inflated effect size and risk of false positives 24 Success or failure of blinding is rarely reported or measured it is implicitly assumed that experiments reported as blind are truly blind 1 Critics have pointed out that without assessment and reporting there is no way to know if a blind succeeded This shortcoming is especially concerning given that even a small error in blinding can produce a statistically significant result in the absence of any real difference between test groups when a study is sufficiently powered i e statistical significance is not robust to bias As such many statistically significant results in randomized controlled trials may be caused by error in blinding 26 Some researchers have called for the mandatory assessment of blinding efficacy in clinical trials 18 Applications editIn medicine edit Blinding is considered essential in medicine 27 but is often difficult to achieve For example it is difficult to compare surgical and non surgical interventions in blind trials In some cases sham surgery may be necessary for the blinding process A good clinical protocol ensures that blinding is as effective as possible within ethical and practical constrains Studies of blinded pharmacological trials across widely varying domains find evidence of high levels of unblinding Unblinding has been shown to affect both patients and clinicians This evidence challenges the common assumption that blinding is highly effective in pharmacological trials Unblinding has also been documented in clinical trials outside of pharmacology 28 Pain edit A 2018 meta analysis found that assessment of blinding was reported in only 23 out of 408 randomized controlled trials for chronic pain 5 6 The study concluded upon analysis of pooled data that the overall quality of the blinding was poor and the blinding was not successful Additionally both pharmaceutical sponsorship and the presence of side effects were associated with lower rates of reporting assessment of blinding 29 Depression edit Studies have found evidence of extensive unblinding in antidepressant trials at least three quarters of patients were able to correctly guess their treatment assignment 30 Unblinding also occurs in clinicians 31 Better blinding of patients and clinicians reduces effect size Researchers concluded that unblinding inflates effect size in antidepressant trials 32 33 34 Some researchers believe that antidepressants are not effective for the treatment of depression and only outperform placebos due to systematic error These researchers argue that antidepressants are just active placebos 35 36 Acupuncture edit While the possibility of blinded trials on acupuncture is controversial a 2003 review of 47 randomized controlled trials found no fewer than four methods of blinding patients to acupuncture treatment 1 superficial needling of true acupuncture points 2 use of acupuncture points which are not indicated for the condition being treated 3 insertion of needles outside of true acupuncture points and 4 the use of placebo needles which are designed not to penetrate the skin The authors concluded that there was no clear association between type of sham intervention used and the results of the trials 37 A 2018 study on acupuncture which used needles that did not penetrate the skin as a sham treatment found that 68 of patients and 83 of acupuncturists correctly identified their group allocation The authors concluded that the blinding had failed but that more advanced placebos may someday offer the possibility of well blinded studies in acupuncture 38 In physics edit It is standard practice in physics to perform blinded data analysis After data analysis is complete one is allowed to unblind the data A prior agreement to publish the data regardless of the results of the analysis may be made to prevent publication bias 13 In social sciences edit Social science research is particularly prone to observer bias so it is important in these fields to properly blind the researchers In some cases while blind experiments would be useful they are impractical or unethical Blinded data analysis can reduce bias but is rarely used in social science research 39 In forensics edit In a police photo lineup an officer shows a group of photos to a witness and asks the witness to identify the individual who committed the crime Since the officer is typically aware of who the suspect is they may subconsciously or consciously influence the witness to choose the individual that they believe committed the crime There is a growing movement in law enforcement to move to a blind procedure in which the officer who shows the photos to the witness does not know who the suspect is 40 41 In music edit Main article Blind audition Auditions for symphony orchestras take place behind a curtain so that the judges cannot see the performer Blinding the judges to the gender of the performers has been shown to increase the hiring of women 42 Blind tests can also be used to compare the quality of musical instruments 43 44 See also edit nbsp Science portalAllocation concealment Black boxing Blind taste test Jadad scale Randomized controlled trial RCT Observational study Metascience Royal Commission on Animal MagnetismReferences edit a b c Bello Segun Moustgaard Helene Hrobjartsson Asbjorn October 2014 The risk of unblinding was infrequently and incompletely reported in 300 randomized clinical trial publications Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 67 10 1059 1069 doi 10 1016 j jclinepi 2014 05 007 ISSN 1878 5921 PMID 24973822 Oxford Centre for Evidence based Medicine Levels of Evidence March 2009 CEBM cebm net 11 June 2009 Archived from the original on 26 October 2017 Retrieved 2 May 2018 Fetis FJ 1868 Biographie Universelle des Musiciens et Bibliographie Generale de la Musique Tome 1 Second ed Paris Firmin Didot Freres Fils et Cie p 249 Retrieved 2011 07 21 Dubourg G 1852 The Violin Some Account of That Leading Instrument and its Most Eminent Professors Fourth ed London Robert Cocks and Co pp 356 357 Retrieved 2011 07 21 a b Stolberg M December 2006 Inventing the randomized double blind trial the Nuremberg salt test of 1835 Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 99 12 642 643 doi 10 1177 014107680609901216 PMC 1676327 PMID 17139070 Biographie Des Doctor Friedrich Wilhelm Von Hoven 1840 ISBN 1104040891 Bernard Claude Dagognet Francois 2008 Introduction a l etude de la medecine experimentale Champs Paris Flammarion ISBN 978 2 08 121793 5 Daston L 2005 Scientific Error and the Ethos of Belief Social Research 72 1 18 doi 10 1353 sor 2005 0016 S2CID 141036212 Rivers WH Webber HN August 1907 The action of caffeine on the capacity for muscular work The Journal of Physiology 36 1 33 47 doi 10 1113 jphysiol 1907 sp001215 PMC 1533733 PMID 16992882 Alder K 2006 Kramer LS Maza SC eds A Companion to Western Historical Thought Blackwell Companions to History Wiley Blackwell p 307 ISBN 978 1 4051 4961 7 Retrieved 2012 02 11 Shortly after the start of the Cold War double blind reviews became the norm for conducting scientific medical research as well as the means by which peers evaluated scholarship both in science and in history a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a work ignored help Hrobjartsson A Emanuelsson F Skou Thomsen AS Hilden J Brorson S August 2014 Bias due to lack of patient blinding in clinical trials A systematic review of trials randomizing patients to blind and nonblind sub studies International Journal of Epidemiology 43 4 1272 83 doi 10 1093 ije dyu115 PMC 4258786 PMID 24881045 Bello S Krogsboll LT Gruber J Zhao ZJ Fischer D Hrobjartsson A September 2014 Lack of blinding of outcome assessors in animal model experiments implies risk of observer bias Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 67 9 973 83 doi 10 1016 j jclinepi 2014 04 008 PMID 24972762 a b MacCoun Robert Perlmutter Saul 7 October 2015 Blind analysis Hide results to seek the truth Nature 526 7572 187 189 Bibcode 2015Natur 526 187M doi 10 1038 526187a PMID 26450040 Schulz KF Chalmers I Altman DG February 2002 The landscape and lexicon of blinding in randomized trials Annals of Internal Medicine 136 3 254 9 doi 10 7326 0003 4819 136 3 200202050 00022 PMID 11827510 S2CID 34932997 Moher David Hopewell Sally Schulz Kenneth F Montori Victor Gotzsche Peter C Devereaux P J Elbourne Diana Egger Matthias Altman Douglas G 23 March 2010 CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials BMJ Clinical Research Ed 340 c869 doi 10 1136 bmj c869 ISSN 1756 1833 PMC 2844943 PMID 20332511 Bello Segun Moustgaard Helene Hrobjartsson Asbjorn 2017 Unreported formal assessment of unblinding occurred in 4 of 10 randomized clinical trials unreported loss of blinding in 1 of 10 trials Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 81 42 50 doi 10 1016 j jclinepi 2016 08 002 ISSN 1878 5921 PMID 27555081 Schulz Kenneth F Grimes David A 23 February 2002 Blinding in randomised trials hiding who got what Lancet 359 9307 696 700 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 02 07816 9 ISSN 0140 6736 PMID 11879884 S2CID 11578262 a b Kolahi J Bang H Park J December 2009 Towards a proposal for assessment of blinding success in clinical trials up to date review Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology 37 6 477 84 doi 10 1111 j 1600 0528 2009 00494 x ISSN 1600 0528 PMC 3044082 PMID 19758415 Dinnett EM Mungall MM Kent JA Ronald ES McIntyre KE Anderson E Gaw A 2005 Unblinding of trial participants to their treatment allocation lessons from the Prospective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk PROSPER Clin Trials 2 3 254 259 doi 10 1191 1740774505cn089oa PMID 16279148 S2CID 36252366 Quittell Lynne M 3 October 2018 The Scientific and Social Implications of Unblinding a Study Subject The American Journal of Bioethics 18 10 71 73 doi 10 1080 15265161 2018 1513589 ISSN 1526 5161 PMID 30339067 S2CID 53014880 Double D B 19 October 1996 Placebo mania Placebo controlled trials are needed to provide data on effectiveness of active treatment BMJ British Medical Journal 313 7063 1008 9 doi 10 1136 bmj 313 7063 1008b ISSN 0959 8138 PMC 2352320 PMID 8892442 Rees Judy R Wade Timothy J Levy Deborah A Colford John M Hilton Joan F February 2005 Changes in beliefs identify unblinding in randomized controlled trials a method to meet CONSORT guidelines Contemporary Clinical Trials 26 1 25 37 doi 10 1016 j cct 2004 11 020 PMID 15837450 Ledford Heidi A question of Control PDF nature com Nature Magazine Retrieved 24 April 2019 a b Moher David Altman Douglas G Schulz Kenneth F 24 March 2010 CONSORT 2010 Statement updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials BMJ 340 c332 doi 10 1136 bmj c332 ISSN 0959 8138 PMC 2844940 PMID 20332509 E6 R2 Good Clinical Practice Integrated Addendum to ICH E6 R1 Guidance for Industry PDF fda gov 2019 04 05 Retrieved 21 April 2019 Siegfried Tom 2010 Odds are it s wrong Science fails to face the shortcomings of statistics Science News 177 7 26 29 doi 10 1002 scin 5591770721 ISSN 1943 0930 Oxford Centre for Evidence based Medicine Levels of Evidence March 2009 CEBM cebm net 11 June 2009 Archived from the original on 26 October 2017 Retrieved 2 May 2018 JUL 2009 The Pharmaceutical Journal31 31 July 2009 An example of problems that arise from clinical trials and how to avoid them Pharmaceutical Journal 283 129 130 Retrieved 24 April 2019 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint numeric names authors list link Colagiuri Ben Sharpe Louise Scott Amelia September 2018 The Blind Leading the Not So Blind A Meta Analysis of Blinding in Pharmacological Trials for Chronic Pain The Journal of Pain 20 5 489 500 doi 10 1016 j jpain 2018 09 002 ISSN 1526 5900 PMID 30248448 S2CID 52813251 Retrieved 22 April 2019 Perlis Roy H Ostacher Michael Fava Maurizio Nierenberg Andrew A Sachs Gary S Rosenbaum Jerrold F 2010 Assuring that double blind is blind The American Journal of Psychiatry 167 3 250 252 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2009 09060820 ISSN 1535 7228 PMID 20194487 S2CID 207628021 White K Kando J Park T Waternaux C Brown W A December 1992 Side effects and the blindability of clinical drug trials The American Journal of Psychiatry 149 12 1730 1731 doi 10 1176 ajp 149 12 1730 ISSN 0002 953X PMID 1443253 Moncrieff Joanna Wessely Simon Hardy Rebecca 2 January 2018 Meta analysis of trials comparing antidepressants with active placebos British Journal of Psychiatry 172 3 227 231 doi 10 1192 bjp 172 3 227 ISSN 0007 1250 PMID 9614471 S2CID 4975797 Greenberg RP Bornstein RF Greenberg MD Fisher S October 1992 A meta analysis of antidepressant outcome under blinder conditions Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 60 5 664 9 discussion 670 7 doi 10 1037 0022 006X 60 5 664 ISSN 0022 006X PMID 1401382 Moncrieff J Wessely S Hardy R 2004 Active placebos versus antidepressants for depression The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2012 1 CD003012 doi 10 1002 14651858 CD003012 pub2 ISSN 1469 493X PMC 8407353 PMID 14974002 Ioannidis JP 27 May 2008 Effectiveness of antidepressants an evidence myth constructed from a thousand randomized trials Philosophy Ethics and Humanities in Medicine 3 14 doi 10 1186 1747 5341 3 14 ISSN 1747 5341 PMC 2412901 PMID 18505564 Kirsch Irving 2014 Antidepressants and the Placebo Effect Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 222 3 128 134 doi 10 1027 2151 2604 a000176 ISSN 2190 8370 PMC 4172306 PMID 25279271 Dincer F Linde K December 2003 Sham interventions in randomized clinical trials of acupuncture a review Complementary Therapies in Medicine 11 4 235 242 doi 10 1016 S0965 2299 03 00124 9 PMID 15022656 Vase L Baram S Takakura N Takayama M Yajima H Kawase A Schuster L Kaptchuk TJ Schou S Jensen TS Zachariae R Svensson P 2015 Can acupuncture treatment be double blinded An evaluation of double blind acupuncture treatment of postoperative pain PLOS ONE 10 3 e0119612 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1019612V doi 10 1371 journal pone 0119612 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4352029 PMID 25747157 Sanders Robert 2015 10 08 Blind analysis could reduce bias in social science research Berkeley News Retrieved 2019 08 29 Dittmann M July August 2004 Accuracy and the accused Psychologists work with law enforcement on research based improvements to crime suspect identification Monitor on Psychology 35 7 American Psychological Association 74 Koerner BI July August 2002 Under the Microscope Legal Affairs Retrieved 2 May 2018 Miller CC 25 February 2016 Is Blind Hiring the Best Hiring The New York Times Retrieved 26 April 2019 Violinists can t tell the difference between Stradivarius violins and new ones by Ed Yong Discover 2 January 2012 Fritz C 2012 Player preferences among new and old violins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 3 760 63 Bibcode 2012PNAS 109 760F doi 10 1073 pnas 1114999109 PMC 3271912 PMID 22215592 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Blinded experiment amp oldid 1193403410 Unblinding, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.