fbpx
Wikipedia

Matthew effect

The Matthew effect of accumulated advantage, sometimes called the Matthew principle, is the tendency of individuals to accrue social or economic success in proportion to their initial level of popularity, friends, and wealth. It is sometimes summarized by the adage or platitude "the rich get richer and the poor get poorer".[1][2] The term was coined by sociologists Robert K. Merton and Harriet Zuckerman[3] in 1968[4] and takes its name from a loose interpretation of the Parable of the Talents in the biblical Gospel of Matthew.

The Matthew effect may largely be explained by preferential attachment, whereby wealth or credit is distributed among individuals according to how much they already have. This has the net effect of making it increasingly difficult for low ranked individuals to increase their totals because they have fewer resources to risk over time, and increasingly easy for high rank individuals to preserve a large total because they have a large amount to risk.[5]

Early studies of Matthew effects were primarily concerned with the inequality in the way scientists were recognized for their work. However, Norman W. Storer, of Columbia University, led a new wave of research. He believed he discovered that the inequality that existed in the social sciences also existed in other institutions.[6]

Etymology edit

The concept is named according to two of the parables of Jesus in the synoptic Gospels (Table 2, of the Eusebian Canons).

The concept concludes both synoptic versions of the parable of the talents:

For to every one who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.

— Matthew 25:29, RSV.

I tell you, that to every one who has will more be given; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.

— Luke 19:26, RSV.

The concept concludes two of the three synoptic versions of the parable of the lamp under a bushel (absent in the version of Matthew):

For to him who has will more be given; and from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.

— Mark 4:25, RSV.

Take heed then how you hear; for to him who has will more be given, and from him who has not, even what he thinks that he has will be taken away.

— Luke 8:18, RSV.

The concept is presented again in Matthew outside of a parable during Christ's explanation to his disciples of the purpose of parables:

And he answered them, "To you it has been given to know the secrets of the kingdom of heaven, but to them it has not been given. For to him who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away."

— Matthew 13:11–12, RSV.

Sociology of science edit

In the sociology of science, "Matthew effect" was a term coined by Robert K. Merton and Harriet Anne Zuckerman to describe how, among other things, eminent scientists will often get more credit than a comparatively unknown researcher, even if their work is similar; it also means that credit will usually be given to researchers who are already famous.[4][7] For example, a prize will almost always be awarded to the most senior researcher involved in a project, even if all the work was done by a graduate student. This was later formulated by Stephen Stigler as Stigler's law of eponymy – "No scientific discovery is named after its original discoverer" – with Stigler explicitly naming Merton as the true discoverer, making his "law" an example of itself.

Merton and Zuckerman furthermore argued that in the scientific community the Matthew effect reaches beyond simple reputation to influence the wider communication system, playing a part in social selection processes and resulting in a concentration of resources and talent. They gave as an example the disproportionate visibility given to articles from acknowledged authors, at the expense of equally valid or superior articles written by unknown authors. They also noted that the concentration of attention on eminent individuals can lead to an increase in their self-assurance, pushing them to perform research in important but risky problem areas.[4]

Examples edit

  • Experiments manipulating download counts or bestseller lists for books and music have shown consumer activity follows the apparent popularity.[8][9][10]
  • A model for career progress quantitatively incorporates the Matthew Effect in order to predict the distribution of individual career length in competitive professions. The model predictions are validated by analyzing the empirical distributions of career length for careers in science and professional sports (e.g. Major League Baseball).[11] As a result, the disparity between the large number of short careers and the relatively small number of extremely long careers can be explained by the "rich-get-richer" mechanism, which in this framework, provides more experienced and more reputable individuals with a competitive advantage in obtaining new career opportunities.
  • In his 2011 book The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence Has Declined, cognitive psychologist Steven Pinker refers to the Matthew Effect in societies, whereby everything seems to go right in some, and wrong in others. He speculates in Chapter 9 that this could be the result of a positive feedback loop in which reckless behavior by some individuals creates a chaotic environment that encourages reckless behavior by others. He cites research by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson showing that the more unstable the environment, the more steeply people discount the future, and thus the less forward-looking their behavior.
  • A large Matthew effect was discovered in a study of science funding in the Netherlands, where winners just above the funding threshold were found to accumulate more than twice as much funding during the subsequent eight years as non-winners with near-identical review scores that fell just below the threshold.[12]
  • In science, dramatic differences in productivity may be explained by three phenomena: sacred spark, cumulative advantage, and search costs minimization by journal editors. The sacred spark paradigm suggests that scientists differ in their initial abilities, talent, skills, persistence, work habits, etc. that provide particular individuals with an early advantage. These factors have a multiplicative effect which helps these scholars succeed later. The cumulative advantage model argues that an initial success helps a researcher gain access to resources (e.g., teaching release, best graduate students, funding, facilities, etc.), which in turn results in further success. Search costs minimization by journal editors takes place when editors try to save time and effort by consciously or subconsciously selecting articles from well-known scholars. Whereas the exact mechanism underlying these phenomena is yet unknown, it is documented that a minority of all academics produce the most research output and attract the most citations.[13]

Education edit

In education, the term "Matthew effect" has been adopted by psychologist Keith Stanovich and popularised by education theorist Anthony Kelly to describe a phenomenon observed in research on how new readers acquire the skills to read. Effectively, early success in acquiring reading skills usually leads to later successes in reading as the learner grows, while failing to learn to read before the third or fourth year of schooling may be indicative of lifelong problems in learning new skills.[14]

This is because children who fall behind in reading would read less, increasing the gap between them and their peers. Later, when students need to "read to learn" (where before they were learning to read), their reading difficulty creates difficulty in most other subjects. In this way they fall further and further behind in school, dropping out at a much higher rate than their peers.

In the words of Stanovich:

Slow reading acquisition has cognitive, behavioral, and motivational consequences that slow the development of other cognitive skills and inhibit performance on many academic tasks. In short, as reading develops, other cognitive processes linked to it track the level of reading skill. Knowledge bases that are in reciprocal relationships with reading are also inhibited from further development. The longer this developmental sequence is allowed to continue, the more generalized the deficits will become, seeping into more and more areas of cognition and behavior. Or to put it more simply – and sadly – in the words of a tearful nine-year-old, already falling frustratingly behind his peers in reading progress, "Reading affects everything you do."[15]

This effect has been used successfully in legal cases, such as Brody v. Dare County Board of Education.[16] Such cases argue that early education intervention is essential for disabled children, and that failing to do so negatively impacts those children.[17]

Network science edit

In network science, the Matthew effect is used to describe the preferential attachment of earlier nodes in a network, which explains that these nodes tend to attract more links early on.[18] "Because of preferential attachment, a node that acquires more connections than another one will increase its connectivity at a higher rate, and thus an initial difference in the connectivity between two nodes will increase further as the network grows, while the degree of individual nodes will grow proportional with the square root of time."[5] The Matthew Effect therefore explains the growth of some nodes in vast networks such as the Internet.[19]

Markets with social influence edit

Social influence often induces a rich-get-richer phenomenon where popular products tend to become even more popular.[20] An example of the Matthew Effect's role on social influence is an experiment by Salganik, Dodds, and Watts in which they created an experimental virtual market named MUSICLAB. In MUSICLAB, people could listen to music and choose to download the songs they enjoyed the most. The song choices were unknown songs produced by unknown bands. There were two groups tested; one group was given zero additional information on the songs and one group was told the popularity of each song and the number of times it had previously been downloaded.[21]

As a result, the group that saw which songs were the most popular and were downloaded the most were then biased to choose those songs as well. The songs that were most popular and downloaded the most stayed at the top of the list and consistently received the most plays. To summarize the experiment's findings, the performance rankings had the largest effect boosting expected downloads the most. Download rankings had a decent effect; however, not as impactful as the performance rankings.[22] Also, Abeliuk et al. (2016) proved that when utilizing “performance rankings”, a monopoly will be created for the most popular songs.[23]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Gladwell, Malcolm (2008-11-18). Outliers: The Story of Success (1 ed.). Little, Brown and Company. ISBN 978-0-316-01792-3.
  2. ^ Shaywitz, David A. (2008-11-15). "The Elements of Success". The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved 2009-01-12.
  3. ^ "The Matthew Effect in Science, II : Cumulative Advantage and the Symbolism of Intellectual Property by Robert K. Merton" (PDF). Retrieved 2019-05-04.
  4. ^ a b c Merton, Robert K. (1968). "The Matthew Effect in Science" (PDF). Science. 159 (3810): 56–63. Bibcode:1968Sci...159...56M. doi:10.1126/science.159.3810.56. PMID 17737466. S2CID 3526819.
  5. ^ a b Perc, Matjaž (2014). "The Matthew effect in empirical data". Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 12 (104): 20140378. arXiv:1408.5124. Bibcode:2014arXiv1408.5124P. doi:10.1098/rsif.2014.0378. PMC 4233686. PMID 24990288.
  6. ^ Rigney, Daniel (2010). "Matthew Effects in the Economy.” The Matthew Effect: How Advantage Begets Further Advantage. Columbia University Press. pp. pp. 35–52.
  7. ^ Merton, Robert K (1988). "The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative advantage and the symbolism of intellectual property" (PDF). Isis. 79 (4): 606–623. doi:10.1086/354848. S2CID 17167736.
  8. ^ Salganik, Matthew J.; Dodds, Peter S.; Watts, Duncan J. (2006). "Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market" (PDF). Science. 311 (5762): 854–856. Bibcode:2006Sci...311..854S. doi:10.1126/science.1121066. PMID 16469928. S2CID 7310490.
  9. ^ Sorenson, Alan T (2007). "Bestseller Lists and Product Variety" (PDF). Journal of Industrial Economics. 55 (4): 715–738. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6451.2007.00327.x. S2CID 49028945.
  10. ^ van de Rijt, A.; Kang, S.; Restivo, M.; Patil, A. (2014). "Field Experiments of Success-Breeds-Success Dynamics" (PDF). PNAS. 111 (19): 6934–6939. Bibcode:2014PNAS..111.6934V. doi:10.1073/pnas.1316836111. PMC 4024896. PMID 24778230.
  11. ^ Petersen, Alexander M.; Jung, Woo-Sung; Yang, Jae-Suk; Stanley, H. Eugene (2011). "Quantitative and Empirical demonstration of the Matthew Effect in a study of Career Longevity". PNAS. 108 (1): 18–23. arXiv:0806.1224. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108...18P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1016733108. PMC 3017158. PMID 21173276.
  12. ^ Bol, T.; de Vaan, M.; van de Rijt, A. (2018). "The Matthew Effect in Science Funding" (PDF). PNAS. 115 (19): 4887–4890. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.4887B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1719557115. PMC 5948972. PMID 29686094.
  13. ^ Serenko, A.; Cox, R.; Bontis, N.; Booker, L. (2011). "The Superstar Phenomenon in the Knowledge Management and Intellectual Capital Academic Discipline" (PDF). Journal of Informetrics. 5: 333–345.
  14. ^ Kempe, C., Eriksson-Gustavsson, A. L., & Samuelsson, S (2011). "Are There any Matthew Effects in Literacy and Cognitive Development?". Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. 55 (2): 181–196. doi:10.1080/00313831.2011.554699. S2CID 145163197.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Adams, Marilyn J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 59–60.
  16. ^ "Wrightslaw - North Carolina, Review Officer Special Education Decision". www.wrightslaw.com. Retrieved 2022-12-22.
  17. ^ "Assessment & Testing - The Matthew Effect - Wrightslaw.com". www.wrightslaw.com. Retrieved 2022-12-22.
  18. ^ Barabási, A-L; Albert, R (1999). "Emergence of scaling in random networks". Science. 286 (5439): 509–512. arXiv:cond-mat/9910332. Bibcode:1999Sci...286..509B. doi:10.1126/science.286.5439.509. PMID 10521342. S2CID 524106.
  19. ^ Guadamuz, Andres (2011). Networks, Complexity And Internet Regulation – Scale-Free Law. Edward Elgar. ISBN 9781848443105.
  20. ^ Altszyler, E; Berbeglia, F.; Berbeglia, G.; Van Hentenryck, P. (2017). "Transient dynamics in trial-offer markets with social influence: Trade-offs between appeal and quality". PLOS ONE. 12 (7): e0180040. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1280040A. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0180040. PMC 5528888. PMID 28746334.
  21. ^ Berbeglia, F.; Van Hentenryck, P. (2017-02-10). Taming the Matthew Effect in Online Markets with Social Influence (PDF). Thirty-First AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence. Vol. 31. San Francisco. doi:10.1609/aaai.v31i1.10511. from the original on 2022-12-30. Retrieved 2022-12-30.
  22. ^ Salganik, Matthew J.; Dodds, Peter S.; Watts, Duncan J. (2006-02-10). "Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market". Science. 311 (5762): 854–856. Bibcode:2006Sci...311..854S. doi:10.1126/science.1121066. PMID 16469928. S2CID 7310490.
  23. ^ Abeliuk, Andrés; Berbeglia, Gerardo; Cebrian, Manuel; Van Hentenryck, Pascal (2015-04-01). Huerta-Quintanilla, Rodrigo (ed.). "The Benefits of Social Influence in Optimized Cultural Markets". PLOS ONE. 10 (4): e0121934. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1021934A. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0121934. PMC 4382093. PMID 25831093.

Further reading edit

  • Bahr, Peter Riley (2007). "Double jeopardy: Testing the effects of multiple basic skill deficiencies on successful remediation". Research in Higher Education. 48 (6): 695–725. doi:10.1007/s11162-006-9047-y. S2CID 144937969.
  • Cunningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.
  • Rigney, Daniel (2010). The Matthew Effect: How Advantage Begets Further Advantage. Columbia University Press.
  • Stanovich, Keith E (1986). "Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy" (PDF). Reading Research Quarterly. 21 (4): 360–407. doi:10.1598/rrq.21.4.1.
  • Stanovich, Keith E. (2000). Progress in Understanding Reading: Scientific Foundations and New Frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.nningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.

matthew, effect, accumulated, advantage, sometimes, called, matthew, principle, tendency, individuals, accrue, social, economic, success, proportion, their, initial, level, popularity, friends, wealth, sometimes, summarized, adage, platitude, rich, richer, poo. The Matthew effect of accumulated advantage sometimes called the Matthew principle is the tendency of individuals to accrue social or economic success in proportion to their initial level of popularity friends and wealth It is sometimes summarized by the adage or platitude the rich get richer and the poor get poorer 1 2 The term was coined by sociologists Robert K Merton and Harriet Zuckerman 3 in 1968 4 and takes its name from a loose interpretation of the Parable of the Talents in the biblical Gospel of Matthew The Matthew effect may largely be explained by preferential attachment whereby wealth or credit is distributed among individuals according to how much they already have This has the net effect of making it increasingly difficult for low ranked individuals to increase their totals because they have fewer resources to risk over time and increasingly easy for high rank individuals to preserve a large total because they have a large amount to risk 5 Early studies of Matthew effects were primarily concerned with the inequality in the way scientists were recognized for their work However Norman W Storer of Columbia University led a new wave of research He believed he discovered that the inequality that existed in the social sciences also existed in other institutions 6 Contents 1 Etymology 2 Sociology of science 2 1 Examples 3 Education 4 Network science 5 Markets with social influence 6 See also 7 References 8 Further readingEtymology editThe concept is named according to two of the parables of Jesus in the synoptic Gospels Table 2 of the Eusebian Canons The concept concludes both synoptic versions of the parable of the talents For to every one who has will more be given and he will have abundance but from him who has not even what he has will be taken away Matthew 25 29 RSV I tell you that to every one who has will more be given but from him who has not even what he has will be taken away Luke 19 26 RSV The concept concludes two of the three synoptic versions of the parable of the lamp under a bushel absent in the version of Matthew For to him who has will more be given and from him who has not even what he has will be taken away Mark 4 25 RSV Take heed then how you hear for to him who has will more be given and from him who has not even what he thinks that he has will be taken away Luke 8 18 RSV The concept is presented again in Matthew outside of a parable during Christ s explanation to his disciples of the purpose of parables And he answered them To you it has been given to know the secrets of the kingdom of heaven but to them it has not been given For to him who has will more be given and he will have abundance but from him who has not even what he has will be taken away Matthew 13 11 12 RSV Sociology of science editIn the sociology of science Matthew effect was a term coined by Robert K Merton and Harriet Anne Zuckerman to describe how among other things eminent scientists will often get more credit than a comparatively unknown researcher even if their work is similar it also means that credit will usually be given to researchers who are already famous 4 7 For example a prize will almost always be awarded to the most senior researcher involved in a project even if all the work was done by a graduate student This was later formulated by Stephen Stigler as Stigler s law of eponymy No scientific discovery is named after its original discoverer with Stigler explicitly naming Merton as the true discoverer making his law an example of itself Merton and Zuckerman furthermore argued that in the scientific community the Matthew effect reaches beyond simple reputation to influence the wider communication system playing a part in social selection processes and resulting in a concentration of resources and talent They gave as an example the disproportionate visibility given to articles from acknowledged authors at the expense of equally valid or superior articles written by unknown authors They also noted that the concentration of attention on eminent individuals can lead to an increase in their self assurance pushing them to perform research in important but risky problem areas 4 Examples edit Experiments manipulating download counts or bestseller lists for books and music have shown consumer activity follows the apparent popularity 8 9 10 A model for career progress quantitatively incorporates the Matthew Effect in order to predict the distribution of individual career length in competitive professions The model predictions are validated by analyzing the empirical distributions of career length for careers in science and professional sports e g Major League Baseball 11 As a result the disparity between the large number of short careers and the relatively small number of extremely long careers can be explained by the rich get richer mechanism which in this framework provides more experienced and more reputable individuals with a competitive advantage in obtaining new career opportunities In his 2011 book The Better Angels of Our Nature Why Violence Has Declined cognitive psychologist Steven Pinker refers to the Matthew Effect in societies whereby everything seems to go right in some and wrong in others He speculates in Chapter 9 that this could be the result of a positive feedback loop in which reckless behavior by some individuals creates a chaotic environment that encourages reckless behavior by others He cites research by Martin Daly and Margo Wilson showing that the more unstable the environment the more steeply people discount the future and thus the less forward looking their behavior A large Matthew effect was discovered in a study of science funding in the Netherlands where winners just above the funding threshold were found to accumulate more than twice as much funding during the subsequent eight years as non winners with near identical review scores that fell just below the threshold 12 In science dramatic differences in productivity may be explained by three phenomena sacred spark cumulative advantage and search costs minimization by journal editors The sacred spark paradigm suggests that scientists differ in their initial abilities talent skills persistence work habits etc that provide particular individuals with an early advantage These factors have a multiplicative effect which helps these scholars succeed later The cumulative advantage model argues that an initial success helps a researcher gain access to resources e g teaching release best graduate students funding facilities etc which in turn results in further success Search costs minimization by journal editors takes place when editors try to save time and effort by consciously or subconsciously selecting articles from well known scholars Whereas the exact mechanism underlying these phenomena is yet unknown it is documented that a minority of all academics produce the most research output and attract the most citations 13 Education editIn education the term Matthew effect has been adopted by psychologist Keith Stanovich and popularised by education theorist Anthony Kelly to describe a phenomenon observed in research on how new readers acquire the skills to read Effectively early success in acquiring reading skills usually leads to later successes in reading as the learner grows while failing to learn to read before the third or fourth year of schooling may be indicative of lifelong problems in learning new skills 14 This is because children who fall behind in reading would read less increasing the gap between them and their peers Later when students need to read to learn where before they were learning to read their reading difficulty creates difficulty in most other subjects In this way they fall further and further behind in school dropping out at a much higher rate than their peers In the words of Stanovich Slow reading acquisition has cognitive behavioral and motivational consequences that slow the development of other cognitive skills and inhibit performance on many academic tasks In short as reading develops other cognitive processes linked to it track the level of reading skill Knowledge bases that are in reciprocal relationships with reading are also inhibited from further development The longer this developmental sequence is allowed to continue the more generalized the deficits will become seeping into more and more areas of cognition and behavior Or to put it more simply and sadly in the words of a tearful nine year old already falling frustratingly behind his peers in reading progress Reading affects everything you do 15 This effect has been used successfully in legal cases such as Brody v Dare County Board of Education 16 Such cases argue that early education intervention is essential for disabled children and that failing to do so negatively impacts those children 17 Network science editIn network science the Matthew effect is used to describe the preferential attachment of earlier nodes in a network which explains that these nodes tend to attract more links early on 18 Because of preferential attachment a node that acquires more connections than another one will increase its connectivity at a higher rate and thus an initial difference in the connectivity between two nodes will increase further as the network grows while the degree of individual nodes will grow proportional with the square root of time 5 The Matthew Effect therefore explains the growth of some nodes in vast networks such as the Internet 19 Markets with social influence editSocial influence often induces a rich get richer phenomenon where popular products tend to become even more popular 20 An example of the Matthew Effect s role on social influence is an experiment by Salganik Dodds and Watts in which they created an experimental virtual market named MUSICLAB In MUSICLAB people could listen to music and choose to download the songs they enjoyed the most The song choices were unknown songs produced by unknown bands There were two groups tested one group was given zero additional information on the songs and one group was told the popularity of each song and the number of times it had previously been downloaded 21 As a result the group that saw which songs were the most popular and were downloaded the most were then biased to choose those songs as well The songs that were most popular and downloaded the most stayed at the top of the list and consistently received the most plays To summarize the experiment s findings the performance rankings had the largest effect boosting expected downloads the most Download rankings had a decent effect however not as impactful as the performance rankings 22 Also Abeliuk et al 2016 proved that when utilizing performance rankings a monopoly will be created for the most popular songs 23 See also editAttention inequality Capital accumulation Convergence Google Scholar effect The internal contradictions of capital accumulation Lindy effect Matilda effect Metcalfe s law Pareto distribution Positive feedback Preferential attachment Quotation Misquotations Social network analysis Virtuous circle and vicious circle Wealth concentrationReferences edit Gladwell Malcolm 2008 11 18 Outliers The Story of Success 1 ed Little Brown and Company ISBN 978 0 316 01792 3 Shaywitz David A 2008 11 15 The Elements of Success The Wall Street Journal Retrieved 2009 01 12 The Matthew Effect in Science II Cumulative Advantage and the Symbolism of Intellectual Property by Robert K Merton PDF Retrieved 2019 05 04 a b c Merton Robert K 1968 The Matthew Effect in Science PDF Science 159 3810 56 63 Bibcode 1968Sci 159 56M doi 10 1126 science 159 3810 56 PMID 17737466 S2CID 3526819 a b Perc Matjaz 2014 The Matthew effect in empirical data Journal of the Royal Society Interface 12 104 20140378 arXiv 1408 5124 Bibcode 2014arXiv1408 5124P doi 10 1098 rsif 2014 0378 PMC 4233686 PMID 24990288 Rigney Daniel 2010 Matthew Effects in the Economy The Matthew Effect How Advantage Begets Further Advantage Columbia University Press pp pp 35 52 Merton Robert K 1988 The Matthew Effect in Science II Cumulative advantage and the symbolism of intellectual property PDF Isis 79 4 606 623 doi 10 1086 354848 S2CID 17167736 Salganik Matthew J Dodds Peter S Watts Duncan J 2006 Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market PDF Science 311 5762 854 856 Bibcode 2006Sci 311 854S doi 10 1126 science 1121066 PMID 16469928 S2CID 7310490 Sorenson Alan T 2007 Bestseller Lists and Product Variety PDF Journal of Industrial Economics 55 4 715 738 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6451 2007 00327 x S2CID 49028945 van de Rijt A Kang S Restivo M Patil A 2014 Field Experiments of Success Breeds Success Dynamics PDF PNAS 111 19 6934 6939 Bibcode 2014PNAS 111 6934V doi 10 1073 pnas 1316836111 PMC 4024896 PMID 24778230 Petersen Alexander M Jung Woo Sung Yang Jae Suk Stanley H Eugene 2011 Quantitative and Empirical demonstration of the Matthew Effect in a study of Career Longevity PNAS 108 1 18 23 arXiv 0806 1224 Bibcode 2011PNAS 108 18P doi 10 1073 pnas 1016733108 PMC 3017158 PMID 21173276 Bol T de Vaan M van de Rijt A 2018 The Matthew Effect in Science Funding PDF PNAS 115 19 4887 4890 Bibcode 2018PNAS 115 4887B doi 10 1073 pnas 1719557115 PMC 5948972 PMID 29686094 Serenko A Cox R Bontis N Booker L 2011 The Superstar Phenomenon in the Knowledge Management and Intellectual Capital Academic Discipline PDF Journal of Informetrics 5 333 345 Kempe C Eriksson Gustavsson A L amp Samuelsson S 2011 Are There any Matthew Effects in Literacy and Cognitive Development Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 55 2 181 196 doi 10 1080 00313831 2011 554699 S2CID 145163197 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Adams Marilyn J 1990 Beginning to Read Thinking and Learning about Print Cambridge MA MIT Press pp 59 60 Wrightslaw North Carolina Review Officer Special Education Decision www wrightslaw com Retrieved 2022 12 22 Assessment amp Testing The Matthew Effect Wrightslaw com www wrightslaw com Retrieved 2022 12 22 Barabasi A L Albert R 1999 Emergence of scaling in random networks Science 286 5439 509 512 arXiv cond mat 9910332 Bibcode 1999Sci 286 509B doi 10 1126 science 286 5439 509 PMID 10521342 S2CID 524106 Guadamuz Andres 2011 Networks Complexity And Internet Regulation Scale Free Law Edward Elgar ISBN 9781848443105 Altszyler E Berbeglia F Berbeglia G Van Hentenryck P 2017 Transient dynamics in trial offer markets with social influence Trade offs between appeal and quality PLOS ONE 12 7 e0180040 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1280040A doi 10 1371 journal pone 0180040 PMC 5528888 PMID 28746334 Berbeglia F Van Hentenryck P 2017 02 10 Taming the Matthew Effect in Online Markets with Social Influence PDF Thirty First AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence Vol 31 San Francisco doi 10 1609 aaai v31i1 10511 Archived from the original on 2022 12 30 Retrieved 2022 12 30 Salganik Matthew J Dodds Peter S Watts Duncan J 2006 02 10 Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market Science 311 5762 854 856 Bibcode 2006Sci 311 854S doi 10 1126 science 1121066 PMID 16469928 S2CID 7310490 Abeliuk Andres Berbeglia Gerardo Cebrian Manuel Van Hentenryck Pascal 2015 04 01 Huerta Quintanilla Rodrigo ed The Benefits of Social Influence in Optimized Cultural Markets PLOS ONE 10 4 e0121934 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1021934A doi 10 1371 journal pone 0121934 PMC 4382093 PMID 25831093 Further reading editBahr Peter Riley 2007 Double jeopardy Testing the effects of multiple basic skill deficiencies on successful remediation Research in Higher Education 48 6 695 725 doi 10 1007 s11162 006 9047 y S2CID 144937969 Cunningham A E amp Chen Y J 2014 Rich get richer effect Matthew Effects In P Brooks amp V Kempe Eds Encyclopedia of Language Development New York Sage Rigney Daniel 2010 The Matthew Effect How Advantage Begets Further Advantage Columbia University Press Stanovich Keith E 1986 Matthew Effects in Reading Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy PDF Reading Research Quarterly 21 4 360 407 doi 10 1598 rrq 21 4 1 Stanovich Keith E 2000 Progress in Understanding Reading Scientific Foundations and New Frontiers New York Guilford Press nningham A E amp Chen Y J 2014 Rich get richer effect Matthew Effects In P Brooks amp V Kempe Eds Encyclopedia of Language Development New York Sage Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Matthew effect amp oldid 1214811880 Education, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.