fbpx
Wikipedia

Internal model (motor control)

In the subject area of control theory, an internal model is a process that simulates the response of the system in order to estimate the outcome of a system disturbance. The internal model principle was first articulated in 1976 by B. A. Francis and W. M. Wonham[1] as an explicit formulation of the Conant and Ashby good regulator theorem. [2] It stands in contrast to classical control, in that the classical feedback loop fails to explicitly model the controlled system (although the classical controller may contain an implicit model).[3][4]

Forward model of an arm movement. The motor command, u(t), of the arm movement is input to the plant and the predicted position of the body, x̃(t), is output.

The internal model theory of motor control argues that the motor system is controlled by the constant interactions of the “plant” and the “controller.” The plant is the body part being controlled, while the internal model itself is considered part of the controller. Information from the controller, such as information from the central nervous system (CNS), feedback information, and the efference copy, is sent to the plant which moves accordingly.

Internal models can be controlled through either feed-forward or feedback control. Feed-forward control computes its input into a system using only the current state and its model of the system. It does not use feedback, so it cannot correct for errors in its control. In feedback control, some of the output of the system can be fed back into the system's input, and the system is then able to make adjustments or compensate for errors from its desired output. Two primary types of internal models have been proposed: forward models and inverse models. In simulations, models can be combined to solve more complex movement tasks.

Forward models edit

 
Figure 1. The desired position of the body is the reference input to the hypothetical controller, which generates the necessary motor command. This motor command is sent to the plant to move the body and an efference copy of the motor command is sent to a forward model. The output from the forward model (predicted body position) is compared with the output from the plant (body position). Noise from the system or the environment may cause differences between the actual and predicted body positions. The error (difference) between the actual and predicted positions can provide feedback to improve the movement for the next iteration of the internal model.

In their simplest form, forward models take the input of a motor command to the “plant” and output a predicted position of the body.

The motor command input to the forward model can be an efference copy, as seen in Figure 1. The output from that forward model, the predicted position of the body, is then compared with the actual position of the body. The actual and predicted position of the body may differ due to noise introduced into the system by either internal (e.g. body sensors are not perfect, sensory noise) or external (e.g. unpredictable forces from outside the body) sources. If the actual and predicted body positions differ, the difference can be fed back as an input into the entire system again so that an adjusted set of motor commands can be formed to create a more accurate movement.

Inverse models edit

 
Figure 2. Inverse model of a reaching task. The arm's desired trajectory, Xref(t), is input into the model, which generates the necessary motor commands, ũ(t), to control the arm.

Inverse models use the desired and actual position of the body as inputs to estimate the necessary motor commands which would transform the current position into the desired one. For example, in an arm reaching task, the desired position (or a trajectory of consecutive positions) of the arm is input into the postulated inverse model, and the inverse model generates the motor commands needed to control the arm and bring it into this desired configuration (Figure 2). Inverse internal models are also in close connection with the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis (UCM), see also here.

Combined forward and inverse models edit

Theoretical work has shown that in models of motor control, when inverse models are used in combination with a forward model, the efference copy of the motor command output from the inverse model can be used as an input to a forward model for further predictions. For example, if, in addition to reaching with the arm, the hand must be controlled to grab an object, an efference copy of the arm motor command can be input into a forward model to estimate the arm's predicted trajectory. With this information, the controller can then generate the appropriate motor command telling the hand to grab the object. It has been proposed that if they exist, this combination of inverse and forward models would allow the CNS to take a desired action (reach with the arm), accurately control the reach and then accurately control the hand to grip an object.[5]

Adaptive Control theory edit

With the assumption that new models can be acquired and pre-existing models can be updated, the efference copy is important for the adaptive control of a movement task. Throughout the duration of a motor task, an efference copy is fed into a forward model known as a dynamics predictor whose output allows prediction of the motor output. When applying adaptive control theory techniques to motor control, efference copy is used in indirect control schemes as the input to the reference model.

Scientists edit

A wide range of scientists contribute to progress on the internal model hypothesis. Michael I. Jordan, Emanuel Todorov and Daniel Wolpert contributed significantly to the mathematical formalization. Sandro Mussa-Ivaldi, Mitsuo Kawato, Claude Ghez, Reza Shadmehr, Randy Flanagan and Konrad Kording contributed with numerous behavioral experiments. The DIVA model of speech production developed by Frank H. Guenther and colleagues uses combined forward and inverse models to produce auditory trajectories with simulated speech articulators. Two interesting inverse internal models for the control of speech production[6] were developed by Iaroslav Blagouchine & Eric Moreau.[7] Both models combine the optimum principles and the equilibrium-point hypothesis (motor commands λ are taken as coordinates of the internal space). The input motor command λ is found by minimizing the length of the path traveled in the internal space, either under the acoustical constraint (the first model), or under the both acoustical and mechanical constraints (the second model). The acoustical constraint is related to the quality of the produced speech (measured in terms of formants), while the mechanical one is related to the stiffness of the tongue's body. The first model, in which the stiffness remains uncontrolled, is in agreement with the standard UCM hypothesis. In contrast, the second optimum internal model, in which the stiffness is prescribed, displays the good variability of speech (at least, in the reasonable range of stiffness) and is in agreement with the more recent versions of the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis (UCM). There is also a rich clinical literature on internal models including work from John Krakauer,[8] Pietro Mazzoni, Maurice A. Smith, Kurt Thoroughman, Joern Diedrichsen, and Amy Bastian.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ B. A. Francis and W. M. Wonham, "The internal model principle of control theory", Automatica 12 (1976) 457–465.
  2. ^ Roger C. Conant and W. Ross Ashby, "Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system", International Journal of Systems Science vol 1 (1970), 89–97.
  3. ^ Jan Swevers, "Internal model control (IMC) 2017-08-30 at the Wayback Machine", 2006
  4. ^ Perry Y. Li, "Internal Model Principle and Repetitive Control"
  5. ^ Kawato, M (1999). "Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 9 (6): 718–727. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8. PMID 10607637. S2CID 878792.
  6. ^ Also with simulated speech articulators, such as biomechanical tongue models (BTM).
  7. ^ Iaroslav Blagouchine and Eric Moreau. Control of a Speech Robot via an Optimum Neural-Network-Based Internal Model with Constraints. IEEE Transactions on Robotics, vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 142—159, February 2010.
  8. ^ "Sensory Prediction Errors Drive Cerebellum-Dependent Adaptation of Reaching", Tseng, Diedrichsen, Krakauer, et al., Journal of Neurophysiology, 98:54-62, May 16, 2007

internal, model, motor, control, subject, area, control, theory, internal, model, process, that, simulates, response, system, order, estimate, outcome, system, disturbance, internal, model, principle, first, articulated, 1976, francis, wonham, explicit, formul. In the subject area of control theory an internal model is a process that simulates the response of the system in order to estimate the outcome of a system disturbance The internal model principle was first articulated in 1976 by B A Francis and W M Wonham 1 as an explicit formulation of the Conant and Ashby good regulator theorem 2 It stands in contrast to classical control in that the classical feedback loop fails to explicitly model the controlled system although the classical controller may contain an implicit model 3 4 Forward model of an arm movement The motor command u t of the arm movement is input to the plant and the predicted position of the body x t is output The internal model theory of motor control argues that the motor system is controlled by the constant interactions of the plant and the controller The plant is the body part being controlled while the internal model itself is considered part of the controller Information from the controller such as information from the central nervous system CNS feedback information and the efference copy is sent to the plant which moves accordingly Internal models can be controlled through either feed forward or feedback control Feed forward control computes its input into a system using only the current state and its model of the system It does not use feedback so it cannot correct for errors in its control In feedback control some of the output of the system can be fed back into the system s input and the system is then able to make adjustments or compensate for errors from its desired output Two primary types of internal models have been proposed forward models and inverse models In simulations models can be combined to solve more complex movement tasks Contents 1 Forward models 2 Inverse models 3 Combined forward and inverse models 4 Adaptive Control theory 5 Scientists 6 See also 7 ReferencesForward models edit nbsp Figure 1 The desired position of the body is the reference input to the hypothetical controller which generates the necessary motor command This motor command is sent to the plant to move the body and an efference copy of the motor command is sent to a forward model The output from the forward model predicted body position is compared with the output from the plant body position Noise from the system or the environment may cause differences between the actual and predicted body positions The error difference between the actual and predicted positions can provide feedback to improve the movement for the next iteration of the internal model In their simplest form forward models take the input of a motor command to the plant and output a predicted position of the body The motor command input to the forward model can be an efference copy as seen in Figure 1 The output from that forward model the predicted position of the body is then compared with the actual position of the body The actual and predicted position of the body may differ due to noise introduced into the system by either internal e g body sensors are not perfect sensory noise or external e g unpredictable forces from outside the body sources If the actual and predicted body positions differ the difference can be fed back as an input into the entire system again so that an adjusted set of motor commands can be formed to create a more accurate movement Inverse models edit nbsp Figure 2 Inverse model of a reaching task The arm s desired trajectory Xref t is input into the model which generates the necessary motor commands ũ t to control the arm Inverse models use the desired and actual position of the body as inputs to estimate the necessary motor commands which would transform the current position into the desired one For example in an arm reaching task the desired position or a trajectory of consecutive positions of the arm is input into the postulated inverse model and the inverse model generates the motor commands needed to control the arm and bring it into this desired configuration Figure 2 Inverse internal models are also in close connection with the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis UCM see also here Combined forward and inverse models editTheoretical work has shown that in models of motor control when inverse models are used in combination with a forward model the efference copy of the motor command output from the inverse model can be used as an input to a forward model for further predictions For example if in addition to reaching with the arm the hand must be controlled to grab an object an efference copy of the arm motor command can be input into a forward model to estimate the arm s predicted trajectory With this information the controller can then generate the appropriate motor command telling the hand to grab the object It has been proposed that if they exist this combination of inverse and forward models would allow the CNS to take a desired action reach with the arm accurately control the reach and then accurately control the hand to grip an object 5 Adaptive Control theory editWith the assumption that new models can be acquired and pre existing models can be updated the efference copy is important for the adaptive control of a movement task Throughout the duration of a motor task an efference copy is fed into a forward model known as a dynamics predictor whose output allows prediction of the motor output When applying adaptive control theory techniques to motor control efference copy is used in indirect control schemes as the input to the reference model Scientists editA wide range of scientists contribute to progress on the internal model hypothesis Michael I Jordan Emanuel Todorov and Daniel Wolpert contributed significantly to the mathematical formalization Sandro Mussa Ivaldi Mitsuo Kawato Claude Ghez Reza Shadmehr Randy Flanagan and Konrad Kording contributed with numerous behavioral experiments The DIVA model of speech production developed by Frank H Guenther and colleagues uses combined forward and inverse models to produce auditory trajectories with simulated speech articulators Two interesting inverse internal models for the control of speech production 6 were developed by Iaroslav Blagouchine amp Eric Moreau 7 Both models combine the optimum principles and the equilibrium point hypothesis motor commands l are taken as coordinates of the internal space The input motor command l is found by minimizing the length of the path traveled in the internal space either under the acoustical constraint the first model or under the both acoustical and mechanical constraints the second model The acoustical constraint is related to the quality of the produced speech measured in terms of formants while the mechanical one is related to the stiffness of the tongue s body The first model in which the stiffness remains uncontrolled is in agreement with the standard UCM hypothesis In contrast the second optimum internal model in which the stiffness is prescribed displays the good variability of speech at least in the reasonable range of stiffness and is in agreement with the more recent versions of the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis UCM There is also a rich clinical literature on internal models including work from John Krakauer 8 Pietro Mazzoni Maurice A Smith Kurt Thoroughman Joern Diedrichsen and Amy Bastian See also editRepetitive controlReferences edit B A Francis and W M Wonham The internal model principle of control theory Automatica 12 1976 457 465 Roger C Conant and W Ross Ashby Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system International Journal of Systems Science vol 1 1970 89 97 Jan Swevers Internal model control IMC Archived 2017 08 30 at the Wayback Machine 2006 Perry Y Li Internal Model Principle and Repetitive Control Kawato M 1999 Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning Current Opinion in Neurobiology 9 6 718 727 doi 10 1016 S0959 4388 99 00028 8 PMID 10607637 S2CID 878792 Also with simulated speech articulators such as biomechanical tongue models BTM Iaroslav Blagouchine and Eric Moreau Control of a Speech Robot via an Optimum Neural Network Based Internal Model with Constraints IEEE Transactions on Robotics vol 26 no 1 pp 142 159 February 2010 Sensory Prediction Errors Drive Cerebellum Dependent Adaptation of Reaching Tseng Diedrichsen Krakauer et al Journal of Neurophysiology 98 54 62 May 16 2007 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Internal model motor control amp oldid 1175435651, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.