fbpx
Wikipedia

Disconfirmed expectancy

Disconfirmed expectancy is a psychological term for what is commonly known as a failed prophecy. According to the American social psychologist Leon Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance, disconfirmed expectancies create a state of psychological discomfort because the outcome contradicts expectancy. Upon recognizing the falsification of an expected event an individual will experience the competing cognitions, "I believe [X]," and, "I observed [Y]." The individual must either discard the now disconfirmed belief or justify why it has not actually been disconfirmed. As such, disconfirmed expectancy and the factors surrounding the individual's consequent actions have been studied in various settings.

Initial study edit

Disconfirmed expectancy was famously illustrated in the 1956 book When Prophecy Fails by Leon Festinger, Henry W. Riecken, and Stanley Schachter. The book gave an inside account of a doomsday cult led by Dorothy Martin (given the alias "Marion Keech" to preserve her privacy), of Chicago. Martin claimed to have received messages from aliens forecasting a flood that would end the world on December 21, 1954. Festinger and his researchers took the chance to pretend to be a part of the cult in order to observe its behaviors and reaction when the flood failed to occur. Furthermore, Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance, which would be published the following year, predicted that the failure of the prophecy would not break the cult. Instead the group members would look for ways to justify their actions and maintain confidence in the cult.[1]

Some members abandoned the group when the prophecy failed to be fulfilled, but most stayed. Those who stayed did not have weakened resolve. There was in fact an increase in their proselytization and their fervor for the new religion. The prediction of the Earth's destruction became a disconfirmed expectancy which resulted in the dissonant cognitions "the world is going to end" and "the world did not end". Those who left the cult accepted that they were wrong and discarded their false cognition. Those who stayed instead looked for ways to explain the event in a way that would maintain their beliefs. Eventually they came to the agreement that the planet was spared because of their prayers and actions. Festinger et al. theorized that five conditions must be present for this to occur; that is, there are five conditions that must be met such that a disconfirmation can lead to increased strength of belief: strong belief, commitment to the belief, the possibility that the belief is false, recognition of the disconfirmation, and strong social support (these are detailed below).[2]

Conditions for attitude change edit

In When Prophecy Fails the authors describe five conditions under which disconfirmation of a belief may lead to increased fervor in a believer:[citation needed]

  1. A belief must be held with conviction
    The believer must behave in a manner consistent with this belief
  2. The believer must be committed to the belief
    This is apparent when the person holding the belief commits to some action(s) that is difficult to undo, such as selling all of one's possessions or participating in an illegal lifestyle, as with the traditionally polygamist Fundamentalist Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-Day Saints
  3. The belief must be falsifiable
    That is, the belief must be specific enough that a real-world event could disconfirm it
  4. Contradictory evidence must be presented to and recognized by the believer
    The belief must be unequivocally disconfirmed to the believer
  5. The individual believer must have social support

The fifth condition is especially important as it states the specific circumstance under which the belief can continue after disconfirmation. Points one and two are necessary conditions that are likely to influence the believer to be resistant to changing the belief. Points three and four are the factors that indicate the belief is flawed and should be absolutely discarded. Thus, the believer is subject to a large amount of cognitive dissonance: "I believe something that is not true." In the absence of adequate social support, it is likely that the belief will be discarded to alleviate this pressure.[3]

Subsequent study of failed prophecy edit

Following Festinger et al., many others have studied cults based around an unlikely prophecy. As of 1999, twelve other groups had been studied under similar circumstances.[4] Research on these other groups formed around prophetic revelations suggests that their survival following disconfirmation is a more complex matter than When Prophecy Fails describes it. There are at least five different patterns of adaptive proselytization which have been observed:

  1. Survive and begin proselytization
  2. Survive and continue proselytization
  3. Survive and decline in proselytization
  4. Survive but do not proselytize
  5. Neither survive nor proselytize

While one of the patterns here involves the dissolution of the group ("neither survive nor proselytize") this is in the extreme minority as 11 of the 12 groups referenced above continued after disconfirmation of their respective prophecies.[4]

In addition to proselytization there are other adaptive strategies including reaffirmation and rationalization which are influenced not only by in-group social support, but also decisive leadership, sophistication of ideology, vagueness of the prophecy, ritual framing, and organization.[4] Rationalization can often go to lengths to justify the false belief. One of the more popular forms of rationalization is "spiritualization" where the event is said to have happened in the spiritual level and not the physical.[5]

Attributional processing edit

Though much research has been done showing the role that disconfirmed expectancy plays in the attitudes and behavior of cultists, it likely plays a larger more general role in attributional processing and has been shown to instigate causal analysis.[6]

In theory, when events conform to expectation there is little chance that people will analyze the causes of those events. For an expected event, there is no need to update the pre-existing causal theory that was driving the expectancy. On this logic an unexpected event would likely instigate causal analysis as the existing theory has been proven false or incomplete.[6]

In 1973 Newtson showed two groups of people an acted sequence and instructed them to break the sequence into parts. Those who watched an actor perform in an unexpected manner were more likely to break the sequence into smaller meaningful units than those watching an actor perform in an expected manner.[7] Newtson concluded that participants in this study were attending more closely and forming more detailed causal analyses when the actor's behavior deviated from expectation.

A later study by Pyszcynski and Greenberg took a more direct approach by creating an expectancy in participants, either fulfilling or deviating from that expectancy, and then presenting the participants with various information to choose from. They found that people were more likely to look for information that would be "useful for inferring a cause" following disconfirmation and less likely to do so following a confirmation of their expectancy.[6]

Though people are more likely to engage in causal processing when there is a discrepancy between belief and outcome, there is a strong bias towards expectancy confirmation.[8][9] Similarly, disconfirming behavior can be discredited in many ways, including but not limited to selective attention to confirmatory evidence and biased labeling.[10]

Uses as a methodology edit

As noted above, disconfirmed expectancy is often paired with cognitive dissonance because the disconfirmation results in two competing cognitions within the individual. As such, disconfirmed expectancy is often used as a reliable method for inducing cognitive dissonance in experimental designs. Generally this is done by introducing an outcome which is dissonant with the participant's established self-concept. The self-concept is often induced as well by creating a strong expectancy toward a certain outcome. For example, in Carlsmith and Aronson (1963) participants were led to believe that a set of signals would reliably precede certain outcomes.[11] To create the disconfirmation, after a few trials the experimenters paired a new outcome with a previous stimulus.

When this is not possible, participants are sorted into groups or placed onto a graded scale according to their prior beliefs. Experimenters can do this as Festinger and his researchers did, i.e. join a cult and observe the impending disconfirmation,[2] though this can also be achieved by pre-testing participants and grouping them based on their responses. These methods—covert/participant observation, pre-selection—are not ideal because they pose the problem of selection bias due to non-random assignment.

Selected studies edit

The following studies have been selected to show a variety of effects found using experimental paradigms which utilize disconfirmed expectancy. This is by no means an exhaustive or comprehensive list. See Further reading below for more comprehensive reviews on the material.

Hedonic consequences edit

Disconfirmed expectancies can influence basic hedonic judgment. According to Festinger, cognitive dissonance produces "psychological discomfort".[1] As Carlsmith and Aronson (1963) extrapolated, it follows that this discomfort puts the individual in a negative hedonic state. Furthermore, they theorized that a negative hedonic state should bias individuals to judge environmental objects in a more negative manner.[11]

In the specific study, participants were asked to taste various solutions and rate them on bitterness and sweetness. Participants were additionally instructed to predict whether a bitter solution or a sweet solution would be tasted next, and they were given multiple trials where predictions were rigged to be correct. When participants were incorrect in their guess and had also been correct on the two preceding trials it was labeled a disconfirmed expectancy, as they had developed an expectancy over the two correct trials which was then denied. Following the disconfirmed expectancies participants reliably rated solutions as less pleasant: sweet solutions were rated less sweet and bitter solutions were rated more bitter.[11]

Consumer dissatisfaction edit

Dissonance theory predicts consumers experience cognitive dissonance when product expectation deviates from product performance. To reduce this disparity and alleviate psychological discomfort the consumer is likely to bring expectations in line with product performance.[1] This statement has been contested[12][13] and some evidence against is given in a classic paper showing that attitudes tend to further polarize when the conflicting information falls outside of the individual's latitude of acceptance.[14] That is, if the performance is too poor the product will simply be rejected and the consumer will like it even less.

Anderson (1973) suggests an assimilation-contrast model that combines the two models. He notes that "there is a point beyond which consumers will not accept increasing disparity between product claims and actual [negative] performance".[15] At this point, which Muzafer Sherif would label the edge of one's latitude of acceptance, reasonable consideration is no longer given towards product performance and the individual is further polarized towards rejection. Anderson further notes that these results pertain to simply explained products and may not generalize to more complex items.[15]

Vacationing edit

Pizam and Milman (1993) show that expectancy disconfirmations are good predictor of satisfaction among first-time visitors to a destination. In this study disconfirmations were regressed on a rating of fulfillment of expectations. The larger the discrepancy between expectation and outcome the more that factor influenced the fulfillment rating. Not all disconfirmations are good predictors of the fulfillment rating. The types of disconfirmations that prove to be good predictors are dependent on the goals of the traveler. For instance a disconfirmation related to the "quality of deluxe hotels" is more influential for travelers categorized as "sun-seekers" than for those categorized as "culture seekers". This suggests that individual factors are weighted for their relative importance to one's own satisfaction.[16]

Evaluations of communicators edit

Expectancy plays a big role in the judgment of communicators and their behavior. When people have expectancies regarding a communicator they evaluate the communicator' attributes and behavior much differently. Prior expectations are likely to continue to be held even after interaction with the communicator, and these expectations will influence later judgments.[17]

There seems to be a general expectancy towards pleasant communication and when this is violated evaluations of the target speaker are more negative, even on evaluations of personal attributes. This can work in the opposite direction as well, and a positive deviation from a negative expectancy has a stronger effect on evaluations than confirmations do. That is, merely interacting with another person can leave a less favorable impression of that person than if a prior negative expectancy was held and then disconfirmed through interaction.[17]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Festinger, Leon (1985) [1st Pub. 1957]. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. ISBN 978-0-8047-0131-0. Retrieved 7 November 2013Reissued by Stanford University Press 1962. Paperback{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: postscript (link) ISBN 0-8047-0911-4
  2. ^ a b Festinger, Leon; Riecken, Henry W. & Schachter, Stanley (1956). When Prophecy Fails. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. doi:10.1037/10030-000. ISBN 978-0-06-131132-1.
  3. ^ Festinger, Leon; Riecken, Henry W. & Schachter, Stanley (1956). "Unfulfilled prophecies and disappointed messiahs". When Prophecy Fails. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. pp. 3–32. doi:10.1037/10030-001. ISBN 978-0-06-131132-1.
  4. ^ a b c Dawson, Lorne L. (October 1999). "When Prophecy Fails and Faith Persists: A Theoretical Overview". Nova Religio: The Journal of Alternative and Emergent Religions. 3 (1): 60–82. doi:10.1525/nr.1999.3.1.60. JSTOR 10.1525/nr.1999.3.1.60.
  5. ^ Melton, J. Gordon (Fall 1985). "Spiritualization and Reaffirmation: What Really Happens When Prophecy Fails". American Studies. 26 (2): 17–29. JSTOR 40641958.
  6. ^ a b c Pyszcynski, Thomas A.; Greenberg, Jeff (1981). "Role of disconfirmed expectancies in the instigation of attributional processing". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 40 (1): 31–38. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.40.1.31.
  7. ^ Newtson, Darren (1973). "Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 28: 28–38. doi:10.1037/h0035584.
  8. ^ Swann, William B. Jr.; Giuliano, Toni (1987). "Confirmatory Search Strategies in Social Interaction: How, When, Why, and With What Consequences". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 5 (4): 511–524. doi:10.1521/jscp.1987.5.4.511.
  9. ^ Snyder, Mark (1984). "When Belief Creates Reality". In Berkowitz, Leonard (ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 18. Academic Press. pp. 247–305. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60146-X. ISBN 978-0-12-015218-6. ISSN 0065-2601.
  10. ^ Turnbull, Dale T.; Turnbull, William (Feb 1986). "Expectancies and Interpersonal Processes". Annual Review of Psychology. 37: 233–256. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.37.020186.001313.
  11. ^ a b c Carlsmith, J. Merrill; Aronson, Elliot (Feb 1963). "Some hedonic consequences of the confirmation and disconfirmation of expectancies". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (2): 151–156. doi:10.1037/h0042692. PMID 14018688.
  12. ^ Insko, Chester A. (1967). Theories of Attitude Change. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  13. ^ Rosenberg, Milton J. (Jan 1965). "When dissonance fails: On eliminating evaluation apprehension from attitude measurement". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1 (1): 28–42. doi:10.1037/h0021647. PMID 14328700.
  14. ^ Hovland, Carl I.; Harvey, O.J.; Sherif, Muzafer (Sep 1957). "Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communication and attitude change". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 55 (2): 244–252. doi:10.1037/h0048480. PMID 13474895.
  15. ^ a b Anderson, Rolph E. (Feb 1973). "Consumer Dissatisfaction: The Effect of Disconfirmed Expectancy on Perceived Product Performance". Journal of Marketing Research. 10 (1): 38–44. doi:10.2307/3149407. JSTOR 3149407.
  16. ^ Pizam, Abraham; Milman, Ady (May 1993). "Predicting satisfaction among first time visitors to a destination by using the expectancy disconfirmation theory". International Journal of Hospitality Management. 12 (2): 197–209. doi:10.1016/0278-4319(93)90010-7. ISSN 0278-4319.
  17. ^ a b Burgoon, Judee K.; Le Poire, Beth A. (1993). "Effects of Communication Expectancies, Actual Communication, and Expectancy Disconfirmation on Evaluations of Communicators and Their Communication Behavior". Human Communication Research. 20: 67–96. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1993.tb00316.x.

Further reading edit

  • Olson, James M.; Roese, Neal J.; Zanna, Mark P. (1996). "Expectancies". In Edward Tory Higgins & Arie W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 211–238). New York: Guilford Press.[ISBN missing]
  • Miller, D. T., & Turnbull, W. (1986). "Expectancies and interpersonal processes". Annual Review of Psychology, 37(1), 233–256.

disconfirmed, expectancy, psychological, term, what, commonly, known, failed, prophecy, according, american, social, psychologist, leon, festinger, theory, cognitive, dissonance, disconfirmed, expectancies, create, state, psychological, discomfort, because, ou. Disconfirmed expectancy is a psychological term for what is commonly known as a failed prophecy According to the American social psychologist Leon Festinger s theory of cognitive dissonance disconfirmed expectancies create a state of psychological discomfort because the outcome contradicts expectancy Upon recognizing the falsification of an expected event an individual will experience the competing cognitions I believe X and I observed Y The individual must either discard the now disconfirmed belief or justify why it has not actually been disconfirmed As such disconfirmed expectancy and the factors surrounding the individual s consequent actions have been studied in various settings Contents 1 Initial study 1 1 Conditions for attitude change 2 Subsequent study of failed prophecy 3 Attributional processing 4 Uses as a methodology 4 1 Selected studies 4 1 1 Hedonic consequences 4 1 2 Consumer dissatisfaction 4 1 3 Vacationing 4 1 4 Evaluations of communicators 5 See also 6 References 7 Further readingInitial study editMain article When Prophecy Fails Disconfirmed expectancy was famously illustrated in the 1956 book When Prophecy Fails by Leon Festinger Henry W Riecken and Stanley Schachter The book gave an inside account of a doomsday cult led by Dorothy Martin given the alias Marion Keech to preserve her privacy of Chicago Martin claimed to have received messages from aliens forecasting a flood that would end the world on December 21 1954 Festinger and his researchers took the chance to pretend to be a part of the cult in order to observe its behaviors and reaction when the flood failed to occur Furthermore Festinger s theory of cognitive dissonance which would be published the following year predicted that the failure of the prophecy would not break the cult Instead the group members would look for ways to justify their actions and maintain confidence in the cult 1 Some members abandoned the group when the prophecy failed to be fulfilled but most stayed Those who stayed did not have weakened resolve There was in fact an increase in their proselytization and their fervor for the new religion The prediction of the Earth s destruction became a disconfirmed expectancy which resulted in the dissonant cognitions the world is going to end and the world did not end Those who left the cult accepted that they were wrong and discarded their false cognition Those who stayed instead looked for ways to explain the event in a way that would maintain their beliefs Eventually they came to the agreement that the planet was spared because of their prayers and actions Festinger et al theorized that five conditions must be present for this to occur that is there are five conditions that must be met such that a disconfirmation can lead to increased strength of belief strong belief commitment to the belief the possibility that the belief is false recognition of the disconfirmation and strong social support these are detailed below 2 Conditions for attitude change edit In When Prophecy Fails the authors describe five conditions under which disconfirmation of a belief may lead to increased fervor in a believer citation needed A belief must be held with conviction The believer must behave in a manner consistent with this belief The believer must be committed to the belief This is apparent when the person holding the belief commits to some action s that is difficult to undo such as selling all of one s possessions or participating in an illegal lifestyle as with the traditionally polygamist Fundamentalist Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints The belief must be falsifiable That is the belief must be specific enough that a real world event could disconfirm it Contradictory evidence must be presented to and recognized by the believer The belief must be unequivocally disconfirmed to the believer The individual believer must have social support The fifth condition is especially important as it states the specific circumstance under which the belief can continue after disconfirmation Points one and two are necessary conditions that are likely to influence the believer to be resistant to changing the belief Points three and four are the factors that indicate the belief is flawed and should be absolutely discarded Thus the believer is subject to a large amount of cognitive dissonance I believe something that is not true In the absence of adequate social support it is likely that the belief will be discarded to alleviate this pressure 3 Subsequent study of failed prophecy editFollowing Festinger et al many others have studied cults based around an unlikely prophecy As of 1999 twelve other groups had been studied under similar circumstances 4 Research on these other groups formed around prophetic revelations suggests that their survival following disconfirmation is a more complex matter than When Prophecy Fails describes it There are at least five different patterns of adaptive proselytization which have been observed Survive and begin proselytization Survive and continue proselytization Survive and decline in proselytization Survive but do not proselytize Neither survive nor proselytize While one of the patterns here involves the dissolution of the group neither survive nor proselytize this is in the extreme minority as 11 of the 12 groups referenced above continued after disconfirmation of their respective prophecies 4 In addition to proselytization there are other adaptive strategies including reaffirmation and rationalization which are influenced not only by in group social support but also decisive leadership sophistication of ideology vagueness of the prophecy ritual framing and organization 4 Rationalization can often go to lengths to justify the false belief One of the more popular forms of rationalization is spiritualization where the event is said to have happened in the spiritual level and not the physical 5 Attributional processing editThough much research has been done showing the role that disconfirmed expectancy plays in the attitudes and behavior of cultists it likely plays a larger more general role in attributional processing and has been shown to instigate causal analysis 6 In theory when events conform to expectation there is little chance that people will analyze the causes of those events For an expected event there is no need to update the pre existing causal theory that was driving the expectancy On this logic an unexpected event would likely instigate causal analysis as the existing theory has been proven false or incomplete 6 In 1973 Newtson showed two groups of people an acted sequence and instructed them to break the sequence into parts Those who watched an actor perform in an unexpected manner were more likely to break the sequence into smaller meaningful units than those watching an actor perform in an expected manner 7 Newtson concluded that participants in this study were attending more closely and forming more detailed causal analyses when the actor s behavior deviated from expectation A later study by Pyszcynski and Greenberg took a more direct approach by creating an expectancy in participants either fulfilling or deviating from that expectancy and then presenting the participants with various information to choose from They found that people were more likely to look for information that would be useful for inferring a cause following disconfirmation and less likely to do so following a confirmation of their expectancy 6 Though people are more likely to engage in causal processing when there is a discrepancy between belief and outcome there is a strong bias towards expectancy confirmation 8 9 Similarly disconfirming behavior can be discredited in many ways including but not limited to selective attention to confirmatory evidence and biased labeling 10 Uses as a methodology editAs noted above disconfirmed expectancy is often paired with cognitive dissonance because the disconfirmation results in two competing cognitions within the individual As such disconfirmed expectancy is often used as a reliable method for inducing cognitive dissonance in experimental designs Generally this is done by introducing an outcome which is dissonant with the participant s established self concept The self concept is often induced as well by creating a strong expectancy toward a certain outcome For example in Carlsmith and Aronson 1963 participants were led to believe that a set of signals would reliably precede certain outcomes 11 To create the disconfirmation after a few trials the experimenters paired a new outcome with a previous stimulus When this is not possible participants are sorted into groups or placed onto a graded scale according to their prior beliefs Experimenters can do this as Festinger and his researchers did i e join a cult and observe the impending disconfirmation 2 though this can also be achieved by pre testing participants and grouping them based on their responses These methods covert participant observation pre selection are not ideal because they pose the problem of selection bias due to non random assignment Selected studies edit The following studies have been selected to show a variety of effects found using experimental paradigms which utilize disconfirmed expectancy This is by no means an exhaustive or comprehensive list See Further reading below for more comprehensive reviews on the material Hedonic consequences edit Disconfirmed expectancies can influence basic hedonic judgment According to Festinger cognitive dissonance produces psychological discomfort 1 As Carlsmith and Aronson 1963 extrapolated it follows that this discomfort puts the individual in a negative hedonic state Furthermore they theorized that a negative hedonic state should bias individuals to judge environmental objects in a more negative manner 11 In the specific study participants were asked to taste various solutions and rate them on bitterness and sweetness Participants were additionally instructed to predict whether a bitter solution or a sweet solution would be tasted next and they were given multiple trials where predictions were rigged to be correct When participants were incorrect in their guess and had also been correct on the two preceding trials it was labeled a disconfirmed expectancy as they had developed an expectancy over the two correct trials which was then denied Following the disconfirmed expectancies participants reliably rated solutions as less pleasant sweet solutions were rated less sweet and bitter solutions were rated more bitter 11 Consumer dissatisfaction edit Dissonance theory predicts consumers experience cognitive dissonance when product expectation deviates from product performance To reduce this disparity and alleviate psychological discomfort the consumer is likely to bring expectations in line with product performance 1 This statement has been contested 12 13 and some evidence against is given in a classic paper showing that attitudes tend to further polarize when the conflicting information falls outside of the individual s latitude of acceptance 14 That is if the performance is too poor the product will simply be rejected and the consumer will like it even less Anderson 1973 suggests an assimilation contrast model that combines the two models He notes that there is a point beyond which consumers will not accept increasing disparity between product claims and actual negative performance 15 At this point which Muzafer Sherif would label the edge of one s latitude of acceptance reasonable consideration is no longer given towards product performance and the individual is further polarized towards rejection Anderson further notes that these results pertain to simply explained products and may not generalize to more complex items 15 Vacationing edit Pizam and Milman 1993 show that expectancy disconfirmations are good predictor of satisfaction among first time visitors to a destination In this study disconfirmations were regressed on a rating of fulfillment of expectations The larger the discrepancy between expectation and outcome the more that factor influenced the fulfillment rating Not all disconfirmations are good predictors of the fulfillment rating The types of disconfirmations that prove to be good predictors are dependent on the goals of the traveler For instance a disconfirmation related to the quality of deluxe hotels is more influential for travelers categorized as sun seekers than for those categorized as culture seekers This suggests that individual factors are weighted for their relative importance to one s own satisfaction 16 Evaluations of communicators edit Expectancy plays a big role in the judgment of communicators and their behavior When people have expectancies regarding a communicator they evaluate the communicator attributes and behavior much differently Prior expectations are likely to continue to be held even after interaction with the communicator and these expectations will influence later judgments 17 There seems to be a general expectancy towards pleasant communication and when this is violated evaluations of the target speaker are more negative even on evaluations of personal attributes This can work in the opposite direction as well and a positive deviation from a negative expectancy has a stronger effect on evaluations than confirmations do That is merely interacting with another person can leave a less favorable impression of that person than if a prior negative expectancy was held and then disconfirmed through interaction 17 See also editTrue believer syndrome Great Disappointment the response of the Millerite Movement to failed prophecies in 1844References edit a b c Festinger Leon 1985 1st Pub 1957 A theory of cognitive dissonance Stanford CA Stanford University Press ISBN 978 0 8047 0131 0 Retrieved 7 November 2013 Reissued by Stanford University Press 1962 Paperback a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint postscript link ISBN 0 8047 0911 4 a b Festinger Leon Riecken Henry W amp Schachter Stanley 1956 When Prophecy Fails Minneapolis MN University of Minnesota Press doi 10 1037 10030 000 ISBN 978 0 06 131132 1 Festinger Leon Riecken Henry W amp Schachter Stanley 1956 Unfulfilled prophecies and disappointed messiahs When Prophecy Fails Minneapolis MN University of Minnesota Press pp 3 32 doi 10 1037 10030 001 ISBN 978 0 06 131132 1 a b c Dawson Lorne L October 1999 When Prophecy Fails and Faith Persists A Theoretical Overview Nova Religio The Journal of Alternative and Emergent Religions 3 1 60 82 doi 10 1525 nr 1999 3 1 60 JSTOR 10 1525 nr 1999 3 1 60 Melton J Gordon Fall 1985 Spiritualization and Reaffirmation What Really Happens When Prophecy Fails American Studies 26 2 17 29 JSTOR 40641958 a b c Pyszcynski Thomas A Greenberg Jeff 1981 Role of disconfirmed expectancies in the instigation of attributional processing Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40 1 31 38 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 40 1 31 Newtson Darren 1973 Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 28 28 38 doi 10 1037 h0035584 Swann William B Jr Giuliano Toni 1987 Confirmatory Search Strategies in Social Interaction How When Why and With What Consequences Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 5 4 511 524 doi 10 1521 jscp 1987 5 4 511 Snyder Mark 1984 When Belief Creates Reality In Berkowitz Leonard ed Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol 18 Academic Press pp 247 305 doi 10 1016 S0065 2601 08 60146 X ISBN 978 0 12 015218 6 ISSN 0065 2601 Turnbull Dale T Turnbull William Feb 1986 Expectancies and Interpersonal Processes Annual Review of Psychology 37 233 256 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 37 020186 001313 a b c Carlsmith J Merrill Aronson Elliot Feb 1963 Some hedonic consequences of the confirmation and disconfirmation of expectancies The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66 2 151 156 doi 10 1037 h0042692 PMID 14018688 Insko Chester A 1967 Theories of Attitude Change New York Appleton Century Crofts Rosenberg Milton J Jan 1965 When dissonance fails On eliminating evaluation apprehension from attitude measurement Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1 1 28 42 doi 10 1037 h0021647 PMID 14328700 Hovland Carl I Harvey O J Sherif Muzafer Sep 1957 Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communication and attitude change The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 55 2 244 252 doi 10 1037 h0048480 PMID 13474895 a b Anderson Rolph E Feb 1973 Consumer Dissatisfaction The Effect of Disconfirmed Expectancy on Perceived Product Performance Journal of Marketing Research 10 1 38 44 doi 10 2307 3149407 JSTOR 3149407 Pizam Abraham Milman Ady May 1993 Predicting satisfaction among first time visitors to a destination by using the expectancy disconfirmation theory International Journal of Hospitality Management 12 2 197 209 doi 10 1016 0278 4319 93 90010 7 ISSN 0278 4319 a b Burgoon Judee K Le Poire Beth A 1993 Effects of Communication Expectancies Actual Communication and Expectancy Disconfirmation on Evaluations of Communicators and Their Communication Behavior Human Communication Research 20 67 96 doi 10 1111 j 1468 2958 1993 tb00316 x Further reading editOlson James M Roese Neal J Zanna Mark P 1996 Expectancies In Edward Tory Higgins amp Arie W Kruglanski Eds Social psychology Handbook of basic principles pp 211 238 New York Guilford Press ISBN missing Miller D T amp Turnbull W 1986 Expectancies and interpersonal processes Annual Review of Psychology 37 1 233 256 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Disconfirmed expectancy amp oldid 1183217558, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.