fbpx
Wikipedia

Covariation model

Harold Kelley's covariation model (1967, 1971, 1972, 1973)[1] is an attribution theory in which people make causal inferences to explain why other people and ourselves behave in a certain way. It is concerned with both social perception and self-perception (Kelley, 1973).

The covariation principle states that, "an effect is attributed to the one of its possible causes with which, over time, it covaries" (Kelley, 1973:108). That is, a certain behaviour is attributed to potential causes that appear at the same time. This principle is useful when the individual has the opportunity to observe the behaviour over several occasions.[2] Causes of an outcome can be attributed to the person (internal), the stimulus (external), the circumstance, or some combination of these factors (Hewstone et al., 1973). Attributions are made based on three criteria: Consensus, Distinctiveness, and Consistency (Kelley, 1973).

Consensus edit

Consensus is the co-variation of behavior across different people. If many people find Lisa attractive, the consensus is high. If only Arnold finds Lisa attractive, consensus is low. High consensus is attributed to the stimulus (in the above example, to Lisa), while low consensus is attributed to the person (in this case, Arnold).

Distinctiveness edit

Distinctiveness refers to how unique the behavior is to the particular situation. There is a low distinctiveness if an individual behaves similarly in all situations, and there exists a high distinctiveness when the person only shows the behaviour in particular situations. If the distinctiveness is high, one will attribute this behaviour more to the circumstance instead of person (Gilovich et al., 2005).

Referring to the example of Dr. Stanton's complimenting Barry's work, if Dr. Stanton almost never compliments other people's work, he shows high distinctiveness. But if he compliments everybody's work, this is low distinctiveness, and one will attribute the behaviour to the person, in this case, Dr. Stanton (Orvis et al., 1975).

Consistency edit

Consistency is the covariation of behavior across time. If Jane is generous all the time, she shows high consistency. If Jane is rarely generous or is generous only at specific times, perhaps around the holidays, she shows low consistency. High consistency is attributed to the person (Jane is a generous person), while low consistency is attributed to the circumstance (the holidays make people generous).

Making attributions using consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency edit

According to Hewstone and Jaspars (1987), we are able to determine whether a person would likely make a personal (internal), stimulus (external) or circumstantial attribution by assessing the levels of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency in a given situation:

Low Consensus, Low Distinctiveness, High Consistency = Personal Attribution
High Consensus, High Distinctiveness, High Consistency = Stimulus Attribution
High Consensus, Low Distinctiveness, Low Consistency = Circumstance Attribution

In reference to McArthur's study (1972), consider the following example: "John laughs at the comedian" This outcome could be caused by something in the person (John), the stimulus (the comedian) the circumstances (the comedy club on that night), or some combination of these factors (Hewstone et al., 1987).

If John is the only person laughing at the comedian (low consensus), he laughs at the comedian at other comedy clubs (high consistency), and he laughs at other comedians (low distinctiveness), then the effect is seen as caused by something in the person (John).

If everyone is laughing at the comedian (high consensus), John laughs at the comedian at other comedy clubs (high consistency), and he does not laugh at other comedians (high distinctiveness), then the effect is seen as caused by something in the stimulus (the comedian).

If everyone is laughing at the comedian (high consensus), John doesn't laugh at the comedian at other comedy clubs (low consistency), and he laughs at other comedians at the club (low distinctiveness) then the effect is seen as caused by something in the circumstance (the comedy club on that night).

Causal schema edit

A causal schema refers to the way a person thinks about plausible causes in relation to a given effect. It provides him or her with the means of making causal attributions when the information provided is limited. The three causal schemata recognized by Kelley are Multiple Sufficient Causes, Multiple Necessary Causes, and Causal Schema for Compensatory Causes (Kelley, 1973).

Multiple Sufficient Causes: He or she may believe that either cause A or cause B suffices to produce a given effect (Kelley et al., 1980). For example, if an athlete fails a drug test (effect), we reason that he or she may be attempting to cheat (cause A) or may have been tricked into taking a banned substance (cause B). Either cause sufficiently attributes to the effect (McLeod, 2010).

Multiple Necessary Causes: Both A and B are necessary to produce a given effect (Kelley et al., 1980). For example, if an athlete wins a marathon (effect), we reason that he or she must be very fit (cause A), and highly motivated (cause B) (McLeod, 2010).

Causal Schema for Compensatory Causes: The effect occurs if either A or B is maximally present, or if both A and B are moderately present. For example, success (effect) depends on high ability (cause A) or low task difficulty (cause B). Success will occur if either cause is highly present or if both are moderately present (Kelley 1973).

Limitations edit

Kelley's covariation model also has its limitations. The critique of the model mainly concerns the lack of distinction between intentional and unintentional behavior, and between reason and cause explanations (Malle, 1999).

Intentional behavior occurs when there is a desire for an outcome, together with a belief that a certain behavior will lead to the desired outcome. These beliefs and desires are mental states acting as reasons behind an intention to act. When behavior is unintentional, the behavior is not explained by reasons, but rather by cause explanations not related to mental states of desire and belief. Malle (1999) found that whether behavior is intentional or unintentional predicts the type of explanation, and that the type of explanation presented predicts the judgement of intentionality.

Malle (1999) also pointed at the differential effect of being an actor versus observer, the effect of the self-serving bias and the distinction between subjective and rational reasoning as important factors acting on attributions of behavior. This is not accounted for by the covariation model. Malle offers a new theoretical framework to give a broader and more comprehensive understanding of attributions of behavior.

Notes edit

  1. ^ also known as 'ANOVA model' (Kelley & Michela, 1980:462)
  2. ^ see also Kelley's discounting principle (1971, 1972): single observation

References edit

  • Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Social psychology. New York: Norton & Company.
  • Hewstone, M., & Jaspars J. (1987). Covariation and causal attribution: A logical model of the intuitive analysis of variance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 4: 663-672
  • Himmelfarb, S. (1972). Integration and attribution theories in personality impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23:309-13.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. New York: General Learning Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York: General Learning Press.
  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
  • Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 457–501.
  • Kelley, H. H., & Stahelski, A. J. (1970). The inference of intention from moves in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 401–419.
  • Malle, B. F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 23–48.
  • McLeod, S. A. (2010). Attribution Theory. Retrieved from http://www.simplypsychology.org/attribution-theory.html
  • McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.
  • Orvis, B. R., Cunningham, J. D. & Kelley, H. H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The roles of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 605–616.
  • Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 400–408.

covariation, model, this, article, includes, list, general, references, lacks, sufficient, corresponding, inline, citations, please, help, improve, this, article, introducing, more, precise, citations, july, 2021, learn, when, remove, this, message, harold, ke. This article includes a list of general references but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations July 2021 Learn how and when to remove this message Harold Kelley s covariation model 1967 1971 1972 1973 1 is an attribution theory in which people make causal inferences to explain why other people and ourselves behave in a certain way It is concerned with both social perception and self perception Kelley 1973 The covariation principle states that an effect is attributed to the one of its possible causes with which over time it covaries Kelley 1973 108 That is a certain behaviour is attributed to potential causes that appear at the same time This principle is useful when the individual has the opportunity to observe the behaviour over several occasions 2 Causes of an outcome can be attributed to the person internal the stimulus external the circumstance or some combination of these factors Hewstone et al 1973 Attributions are made based on three criteria Consensus Distinctiveness and Consistency Kelley 1973 Contents 1 Consensus 2 Distinctiveness 3 Consistency 4 Making attributions using consensus distinctiveness and consistency 5 Causal schema 6 Limitations 7 Notes 8 ReferencesConsensus editConsensus is the co variation of behavior across different people If many people find Lisa attractive the consensus is high If only Arnold finds Lisa attractive consensus is low High consensus is attributed to the stimulus in the above example to Lisa while low consensus is attributed to the person in this case Arnold Distinctiveness editDistinctiveness refers to how unique the behavior is to the particular situation There is a low distinctiveness if an individual behaves similarly in all situations and there exists a high distinctiveness when the person only shows the behaviour in particular situations If the distinctiveness is high one will attribute this behaviour more to the circumstance instead of person Gilovich et al 2005 Referring to the example of Dr Stanton s complimenting Barry s work if Dr Stanton almost never compliments other people s work he shows high distinctiveness But if he compliments everybody s work this is low distinctiveness and one will attribute the behaviour to the person in this case Dr Stanton Orvis et al 1975 Consistency editConsistency is the covariation of behavior across time If Jane is generous all the time she shows high consistency If Jane is rarely generous or is generous only at specific times perhaps around the holidays she shows low consistency High consistency is attributed to the person Jane is a generous person while low consistency is attributed to the circumstance the holidays make people generous Making attributions using consensus distinctiveness and consistency editAccording to Hewstone and Jaspars 1987 we are able to determine whether a person would likely make a personal internal stimulus external or circumstantial attribution by assessing the levels of consensus distinctiveness and consistency in a given situation Low Consensus Low Distinctiveness High Consistency Personal Attribution High Consensus High Distinctiveness High Consistency Stimulus Attribution High Consensus Low Distinctiveness Low Consistency Circumstance Attribution In reference to McArthur s study 1972 consider the following example John laughs at the comedian This outcome could be caused by something in the person John the stimulus the comedian the circumstances the comedy club on that night or some combination of these factors Hewstone et al 1987 If John is the only person laughing at the comedian low consensus he laughs at the comedian at other comedy clubs high consistency and he laughs at other comedians low distinctiveness then the effect is seen as caused by something in the person John If everyone is laughing at the comedian high consensus John laughs at the comedian at other comedy clubs high consistency and he does not laugh at other comedians high distinctiveness then the effect is seen as caused by something in the stimulus the comedian If everyone is laughing at the comedian high consensus John doesn t laugh at the comedian at other comedy clubs low consistency and he laughs at other comedians at the club low distinctiveness then the effect is seen as caused by something in the circumstance the comedy club on that night Causal schema editA causal schema refers to the way a person thinks about plausible causes in relation to a given effect It provides him or her with the means of making causal attributions when the information provided is limited The three causal schemata recognized by Kelley are Multiple Sufficient Causes Multiple Necessary Causes and Causal Schema for Compensatory Causes Kelley 1973 Multiple Sufficient Causes He or she may believe that either cause A or cause B suffices to produce a given effect Kelley et al 1980 For example if an athlete fails a drug test effect we reason that he or she may be attempting to cheat cause A or may have been tricked into taking a banned substance cause B Either cause sufficiently attributes to the effect McLeod 2010 Multiple Necessary Causes Both A and B are necessary to produce a given effect Kelley et al 1980 For example if an athlete wins a marathon effect we reason that he or she must be very fit cause A and highly motivated cause B McLeod 2010 Causal Schema for Compensatory Causes The effect occurs if either A or B is maximally present or if both A and B are moderately present For example success effect depends on high ability cause A or low task difficulty cause B Success will occur if either cause is highly present or if both are moderately present Kelley 1973 Limitations editKelley s covariation model also has its limitations The critique of the model mainly concerns the lack of distinction between intentional and unintentional behavior and between reason and cause explanations Malle 1999 Intentional behavior occurs when there is a desire for an outcome together with a belief that a certain behavior will lead to the desired outcome These beliefs and desires are mental states acting as reasons behind an intention to act When behavior is unintentional the behavior is not explained by reasons but rather by cause explanations not related to mental states of desire and belief Malle 1999 found that whether behavior is intentional or unintentional predicts the type of explanation and that the type of explanation presented predicts the judgement of intentionality Malle 1999 also pointed at the differential effect of being an actor versus observer the effect of the self serving bias and the distinction between subjective and rational reasoning as important factors acting on attributions of behavior This is not accounted for by the covariation model Malle offers a new theoretical framework to give a broader and more comprehensive understanding of attributions of behavior Notes edit also known as ANOVA model Kelley amp Michela 1980 462 see also Kelley s discounting principle 1971 1972 single observationReferences editGilovich T Keltner D amp Nisbett R E 2005 Social psychology New York Norton amp Company Hewstone M amp Jaspars J 1987 Covariation and causal attribution A logical model of the intuitive analysis of variance Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 4 663 672 Himmelfarb S 1972 Integration and attribution theories in personality impression formation Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 23 309 13 Kelley H H 1967 Attribution theory in social psychology In D Levine Ed Nebraska symposium on motivation Lincoln University of Nebraska Press Kelley H H 1971 Attribution in social interaction New York General Learning Press Kelley H H 1972 Causal schemata and the attribution process New York General Learning Press Kelley H H 1973 The processes of causal attribution American Psychologist 28 107 128 Kelley H H amp Michela J L 1980 Attribution theory and research Annual Review of Psychology 31 457 501 Kelley H H amp Stahelski A J 1970 The inference of intention from moves in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 6 401 419 Malle B F 1999 How people explain behavior A new theoretical framework Personality and Social Psychology Review 3 23 48 McLeod S A 2010 Attribution Theory Retrieved from http www simplypsychology org attribution theory html McArthur L A 1972 The how and what of why Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 22 171 193 Orvis B R Cunningham J D amp Kelley H H 1975 A closer examination of causal inference The roles of consensus distinctiveness and consistency information Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32 605 616 Valins S 1966 Cognitive effects of false heart rate feedback Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4 400 408 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Covariation model amp oldid 1161482426, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.