fbpx
Wikipedia

Attachment measures

Attachment measures, or attachment assessments, are the various procedures used to assess the attachment system in children and adults. These procedures can assess patterns of attachment and individual self-protective strategies. Some assessments work across the several models of attachment and some are model-specific. Many assessments allow children and adults' attachment strategies to be classified into three primary attachment pattern groups:[1] B-pattern (balanced, secure), A-pattern (avoidant, dismissive, cognitive), C-pattern (anxious-ambivalent, preoccupied, affective). In most models, each pattern group is further broken down into several sub-patterns.[2] Some assessments are capable of finding additional information about an individual, such as unresolved trauma, depression, history of family triangulation, and lifespan changes in the attachment pattern. Some assessments specifically or additionally look for caregiving behaviors, as caregiving and attachment are widely considered two separate systems for organizing thoughts, feelings, and behavior.[3][2] Some methods assess disorders of attachment or romantic attachment.

Attachment models are typically generated from the schools of developmental science or social psychology, although both emanate from the Bowlby-Ainsworth framework.[2] Ainsworth's Strange Situation Procedure was the first formal attachment assessment, and is still in wide use. Each school, while having the same basic foundation, may be studying different phenomenon.[2]: 467  Assessments are typically conducted by observing behavior in a structured setting, by analyzing the transcript of a structured interview using technical discourse analysis methods, or by self-reports from a questionnaire. Social psychology models primarily utilize self-reports.

Some attachment models, such as the Berkeley (or ABC+D) model, consider disorganized attachment to be a pattern or category. The D classification was thought to represent a breakdown in the attachment-caregiving partnership such that the child does not have an organized behavioral or representational strategy to achieve protection and care from the attachment figure. However, the disorganized concept has been determined to be invalid for people older than 20 months.[4] Other models, such as the Dynamic-Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM), describe virtually all attachment behavior and patterns within (or in a combination of) the three primary A, B, C patterns. The DMM considers all attachment behavior to be an organized effort to adapt within a given caregiving environment to optimize available caregiver protection and maximize survival.

Measures of attachment in infants edit

Attachment measures for infants rely on observing the infant with a caregiver, typically videotaped, either in a natural or contrived environment. Measures for older children, including toddlers, are listed in subsequent sections.

Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) edit

The Strange Situation procedure was formulated to observe attachment relationships between a caregiver and children between the age of nine and 18 months. It was developed by Mary Ainsworth, a developmental psychologist[5] Originally it was devised to enable children to be classified into the attachment styles known as secure, anxious-avoidant and anxious-ambivalent. As research accumulated and atypical patterns of attachment became more apparent it was further developed by Main and Solomon in 1986 and 1990 to include the new category of disorganized/disoriented attachment.[6][7]

In this procedure the child is observed playing for 20 minutes while caregivers and strangers enter and leave the room, recreating the flow of the familiar and unfamiliar presence in most children's lives. The situation varies in stressfulness and the child's responses are observed. The child experiences the following situations:

  1. Mother (or other familiar caregiver) and baby enter room.
  2. Mother sits quietly on a chair, responding if the infant seeks attention.
  3. A stranger enters, talks to the mother then gradually approaches infant with a toy. The mother leaves the room.
  4. The stranger leaves the infant playing unless he/she is inactive and then tries to interest the infant in toys. If the infant becomes distressed this episode is ended.
  5. Mother enters and waits to see how the infant greets her. The stranger leaves quietly and the mother waits until the baby settles, and then she leaves again.
  6. The infant is alone. This episode is curtailed if the infant appears to be distressed.
  7. The stranger comes back and repeats episode 3.
  8. The mother returns and the stranger goes. Reunion behaviour is noted and then the situation is ended.

Two aspects of the child's behaviour are observed:

  • The amount of exploration (e.g. playing with new toys) the child engages in throughout, and
  • The child's reactions to the departure and return of its caregiver.

Beginning in 1970, a series of expansions were added to Ainsworth's original patterns. They include the following: B4 (1970),[8] A/C (1985)[9][10] D/disorganized (1986), B5 (1988, 1992)[11][12] A+, C+, and Depressed (1992, 2010).[12][13] At later ages, additional categories have been described. Each of these patterns reflects a different kind of attachment relationship of the infant with the mother/caregiver. An infant may have a different pattern of attachment to each parent as well as to alternate caregivers. Pattern of attachment is thus not a part of the infant, but is characteristic of the protective and comforting quality of a specific relationship. These attachment patterns are associated with behavioral patterns and can help further predict a child's future personality.[14]

Infant CARE-Index (ICI) edit

The Infant CARE-Index (ICI) is procedure that assesses risk in parent/infant relationships. It was developed by Patricia Crittenden early in the development of the Dynamic-Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM) and can be used from birth, that is before infant's attachment strategies are established, and up to 15 months of age. The ICI involves a 3 minute  video recording of a typical play interaction between a caregiver and infant, without requiring a separation or other stressor.

The focus is on the dyadic synchrony, that is the extent to which adult sensitivity to infant signals result in infant cooperation in play.[15] Lower synchrony is indicative of higher risk. In addition caregiver's interactions with the infant are assessed using three scales, sensitive controlling and unresponsive, and infant interactions are assessed from the perspective of their caregiver, using four scales, cooperative, compulsive, difficult and passive.[16] The ICI does not conclude attachment strategies but it is highly correlated to the maternal scales in the infant Strange Situation assessment patterns of attachment.[17] It can be used in screening, to identify levels of risk, and as a tool for clinical intervention and evaluation and has been used in numerous research projects. The ICI is a statistically valid and reliable assessment, and trained reliable coders can typically code an interaction in 15–20 minutes.[18] (Tryphonopoulos, 2014)

The ICI is often used within the context of child welfare in Britain and the wider UK. It is also used as a developmental screening tool as a way to identify parents who may benefit from more support or specific interventions that support their ability to sensitively attune to their infant.[19]

Measures of attachment in toddlerhood and early-middle childhood edit

As the SSP is not suitable beyond 18 months of age, other measures have been developed for older ages groups, which include observational measures (in a controlled or naturalistic environment), representational methods and interview methods. Some are developed for research purposes whereas others have been developed for clinical use. Effective training of evaluators is essential, as some items to be assessed require interpretation reliability (e.g., child is "suddenly aggressive toward mother for no reason").[20]

Controlled observational methods edit

Preschool strange situation edit

Although originally designed for 1-year-old children, Ainsworth’s strange situation has been adapted to measure the attachment and exploratory behavior of children between the ages of 2-4½ years-old.[21] A fundamental feature of the strange situation is that the situation the child is placed in must elicit stress. If the strange situation fails to stress the child, it cannot serve as an adequate environment for the measurement of attachment.[22] The preschool strange situation features several alterations to facilitate the creation of stress in older children. These modifications include a slightly longer separation, changes in the role and/or gender of the stranger, and changes in the instructions to the caregiver. Some versions of the preschool strange situations omit the stranger altogether, thus leaving the child alone in the room throughout both separations.[23] The coding system used to interpret the attachment style expressed by the child has also been modified. Rather than focusing entirely on the expression of specific behaviors and emotions, the revised coding system assesses ways in which a variety of behaviors, such as talking, are organized to maintain and negotiate proximity and contact. Cassidy, Marvin and the MacArthur Working group published a version of the Strange Situation procedure designed for children within the age group of 3- to 4-years-old. In addition to categorizing a child’s attachment as secure, insecure/avoidant, insecure/ambivalent, and insecure/disorganized, the measure includes a seven-point avoidance scale and nine-point security scale.[22]

Main & Cassidy attachment classification system edit

This system, devised in 1988, analyses the reunion of child and parent after a 1-hour separation. It is aimed at 6-year-olds and classifies their attachment status.[24]

Preschool Assessment of Attachment (PAA) edit

The PAA was devised by Patricia Crittenden for the purpose of assessing patterns of attachment in 18-month to 5-year-old children. It uses the coding method developed with the Dynamic-Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM). Like the SSP it involves an 8-segment 21-minute procedure which is video recorded and then coded. The classifications include all the SSP patterns plus patterns that begin to develop during the second year of life, A3-A4 and C3-C4. The DMM maintains Ainsworth's ABC labeling scheme, but renames the three basic strategies for negotiating interpersonal relationships. Balanced for B-patterns, cognitive for A-patterns, and affective for C-patterns. It is also intended to be able to distinguish the unendangered from the endangered compulsive and obsessive subpatterns that may have implications for emotional and behavioral development.[25] It has been determined scientifically validated and reliable.[26][27]

Marschak Interaction Method (MIM) edit

The MIM is a structured observation of the interaction between parent and child. The MIM was created by Marianne Marschak in the 1960s at the Yale Child Study Center. Salo & Makela (2006) of Finland have standardized and published a rating scale for the MIM for research purposes. Anne Stewart has developed the MIM Behavior Rating Scale (MIMBRS).[28]

Naturalistic observational methods edit

Attachment Q-sort edit

This method, devised by Waters and Deane in 1985, utilizes Q-Sort methodology. It is based on a set period of observation of children aged 1 – 5 in a number of environments. It consists of nearly 100 items intended to cover the spectrum of attachment related behaviors including secure base and exploratory behaviors, affective response and social cognition.[29] The observer sorts the cards corresponding to the degree to which the child exhibits the item, which is then scored.[30] The overall score for each child will result in a variable ranging from +1.0 (i.e., very secure) to -1.0 (i.e., very insecure).[31] Despite its ability to classify secure attachment, the score derived from the Q-set measure does not classify the type of insecure attachment.[29]

Representational (story stem, narrative and picture) methods edit

This approach uses dolls and narrative to enact a story. The dolls represent family members. The interviewer enacts the beginning of the story and then hands the dolls over for the child to complete it with varying degrees of prompting and encouragement. These techniques are designed to access the child's internal working models of their attachment relationships. Methods include the MacArthur Story Stem Battery (MSSB) and the Attachment Story Completion Test, developed in 1990 for children between the age of 3 to 8 years; the Story Stem Assessment Profile (SSAP) developed in 1990 for children aged 4 – 8; the Attachment Doll Play Assessment developed in 1995 for children age 4.5-11; the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task (MCAST) developed in 2000 for children aged 4.5 - 8.5.

Attachment Story Completion Task (ASCT) edit

The Attachment Story Completion Task (ASCT) is a semi-projective attachment measure designed by Inge Bretherton and colleagues to assess the internal working model of children between the age of 3 to 9 years old (though it requires modification when used with older children). The measure evaluates a child's attachment style by analyzing how a child resolves a stress inducing story. In a 30-minute recorded interview, five story stems are presented through the use of props, such as small family figures. The stories are designed to access how that child interacts with their primary caregiver in five situations: separation, confrontation, fear, reunion, and pain.[32] The Interviewer prompts the child to complete each story by saying "show me, or tell me what happens next." The information derived from the interview is later coded according to the organization and content of the story completion. Avoidant attachment, for example, can be disclosed by a child refusing to acknowledge the attachment issue presented in the story stem (through claiming that the event did not take place). A child may also avoid addressing attachment by focusing solely on minor details, such as how the protagonist is dressed. Secure attachment, alternately, is indicated when a child provides coherent and constructive resolutions to the stories.[33]

Attachment Doll Play Assessment (ADPA) edit

The ADPA was developed by Judith Solomon and Carol George (Solomon et al., 1995) [34] as a variation of the ACST. The measure, unlike some of the doll play measures was validated from concurrent mother-child Strange Situation patterns at ages 5-7 years. The assessment identified five attachment groups - secure, avoidant, ambivalent, and the punitive and caregiving patterns of middle childhood disorganization. The ADPA has two unique features not found in other doll play assessments. One is the instruction to the child to select their own family, as compared with other assessments that create a standard family set of dolls. This variation was found important for children in the D group because they frequently do not select a mother-figure or they select an adult doll to represent the self. Another is a coding system that is based on defensive processes rather than content or rating scales. The focus on defense is unique to Solomon and George's work and served as the foundation for assessing defenses in the Adult Attachment Projective Picture adult attachment assessment. The ADPA has been used cross-culturally in Japan and with French speakers in Canada.

Manchester Child Attachment Story Task (MCAST) edit

The MCAST[35] is a semi-structured doll play 'story stem' methodology, developed by Jonathan Green, Charlie Stanley, Ruth Goldwyn and Vicky Smith,[36][37] to evaluate and understand the internal (mental) representations of their attachment relationship with a specific primary caregiver in children of 4 to 8.5 years. The concepts and procedures used have a basis in the Strange Situation Procedure and Adult Attachment Interview, and involves 4 story stem vignettes involving two dolls representing the caregiver-child dyad of interest and a dolls house, presented with affective arousal to mobilise attachment representations in a way that children of this age range find accessible and engaging. Responses are usually videotaped in order to reliably rate aspects of the child's represented narrative content and behaviour, and the child's own behaviour, to ascertain an attachment classification, with a particular focus on disorganised attachment, as well as providing other supporting ratings. Clinical development of the MCAST started in 1992, validation was published in 2000, and it has been since used in a range of cultural contexts[38] and clinical and at-risk groups.[39] Training is required for its use.

Picture response techniques edit

Like the stem stories, these techniques are designed to access the child's internal working models of attachment relationships. The child is shown attachment related pictures and asked to respond. Methods include the Separation Anxiety Test (SAT) developed in 1972 for children aged between 11 and 17. Revised versions have been produced for 4 - 7-year-olds. The SAT was doctored.

Interview methods edit

Child Attachment Interview (CAI) edit

This is a semi-structured interview designed by Target et al. (2003) for children aged 7 to 11. It is based on the Adult Attachment Interview, adapted for children by focusing on representations of relationships with parents and attachment-related events. Scores are based on both verbal and non-verbal communications.[40]

Disturbances of Attachment Interview (DAI) edit

Disturbances of Attachment Interview developed, by Smyke and Zeanah, (1999), is a semi-structured interview designed to be administered by clinicians to caregivers. This method is designed to pick up not only reactive attachment disorder but also Zeannah et al.'s (1993) suggested new alternative categories of disorders of attachment.[41] It covers 12 items, namely having a discriminated, preferred adult, seeking comfort when distressed, responding to comfort when offered, social and emotional reciprocity, emotional regulation, checking back after venturing away from the care giver, reticence with unfamiliar adults, willingness to go off with relative strangers, self endangering behavior, excessive clinging, vigilance/hypercompliance and role reversal.

Measures of attachment in adolescents edit

Attachment Interview for Childhood and Adolescence (AICA) edit

This is a version of the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) rendered age appropriate for adolescents. The classifications of dismissing, secure, preoccupied and unresolved are the same as under the AAI described below.

Measures in adults edit

The three main ways of measuring attachment in adults include the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI), the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP), and self-report questionnaires. The AAI and AAP are based on a developmental perspective, while the self-report questionnaires are based on a social psychology perspective. Each assessment was created with somewhat different aims in mind. Shaver and Fraley, coming from the social psychology perspective, note:

"If you are a novice in this research area, what is most important for you to know is that self-report measures of romantic attachment and the AAI were initially developed completely independently and for quite different purposes. One asks about a person's feelings and behaviors in the context of romantic or other close relationships; the other is used to make inferences about the defenses associated with an adult's current state of mind regarding childhood relationships with parents. In principle, these might have been substantially associated, but in fact they seem to be only moderately related--at least as currently assessed. One kind of measure receives its construct validity mostly from studies of romantic relationships, the other from prediction of a person's child's behavior in Ainsworth's Strange Situation. Correlations of the two kinds of measures with other variables are likely to differ, although a few studies have found the AAI to be related to marital relationship quality and a few have found self-report romantic attachment measures to be related to parenting." (Shaver & Fraley, 2004) [42]

The AAI, the AAP, and the self-report questionnaires offer distinct, but equally useful, perspectives on adult attachment.[2]: 467 

Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) edit

Developed by Carol George, Nancy Kaplan, and Mary Main in 1984,[43] this is a quasi-clinical semi-structured interview that takes about one hour to administer. It utilizes either the ABC+D (or Berkeley) or DMM model for discourse analysis,[44] and originally involved about twenty questions. It has since had extensive research validation to support it.

The AAI-Berkeley model is discussed in Chapter 25 of Attachment Theory, Research and Clinical Applications (2nd ed.), edited by J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver, Guilford Press, NY, 2008. The chapter title is "The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and Current Perspectives," and is written by E. Hesse. The interview taps into adult representation of attachment (i.e. internal working models) by assessing general and specific recollections from their childhood. The interview is coded based on quality of discourse (especially coherence) and content. Categories are designed to predict parental stances on Berkeley infant data.

AAI Attachment status, using the Berkeley (or ABC+D) model, originally utilized Ainsworth's patterns, A1 and A2, B1-5, and C1 and C2, and also disorganization. (Disorganization was initially considered a pattern, then seen as a status, and then determined to be not usable as a concept for adults or children over 20 months.)[45][44] The Berkeley model developed to include over 20 patterns.[46] Four broad categories have included:

  • Autonomous: They value attachment relationships, describe them in a balanced way and as influential. Their discourse is coherent, internally consistent, and non-defensive in nature.
  • Dismissing: They show memory lapses. Minimize negative aspects and deny personal impact on relationships. Their positive descriptions are often contradicted or unsupported. The discourse is defensive.
  • Preoccupied: Experience continuing preoccupation with their own parents. Incoherent discourse. Have angry or ambivalent representations of the past.
  • Unresolved/Disorganized: Show trauma resulting from unresolved loss or abuse.

Some of the strongest external validation of the measures involves its demonstrated ability to predict interviewees' children's classifications in the Strange Situation. The measure also has been shown to have some overlap with attachment constructs measured by the less time-intensive measures of the peer/romantic attachment tradition (Hazan & Shaver, Bartholomew), as reported by Shaver, P. R., Belsky, J., & Brennan, K. A. (2000).[47] However, there are important differences in what is measured by the AAI using the Berkeley model—rather than being a measure of romantic attachment, it taps primarily into a person's state of mind regarding their attachment in their family of origin.

The DMM-AAI uses a slightly modified set of interview questions, and uses principles and analysis techniques from attachment theory as described by Patricia Crittenden and the Dynamic-Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM). The DMM method is more comprehensive, assessing more self-protective attachment strategies, more types of trauma, and more types of memory systems.[46][48] Both the DMM-AAI and Berkeley-AAI methods assess information processing, memory system use, reflective integration, and are able to identify issues such as unresolved trauma and depression. The DMM-AAI method can identify additional issues such as intrusions of negative affect, disorientation, and reorganizing or reorganized attachment strategies.

Assessing adult attachment: A dynamic-maturational approach to discourse analysis (2011)[48] is the coding manual for the DMM-AAI. This manual is published and available at any bookseller, and provides a comprehensive and detailed description of the AAI process, DMM-attachment theory, and the attachment system's impact on aspects of humanity such as patterns of information processing, memory system function, and the effect of trauma. It also offers a brief comparison of the two attachment models using the AAI. The Berkeley/ABC+D model has never published a coding manual or comprehensive model of attachment.[44]

The DMM model of attachment also expands the range of non-B patterns beyond Ainsworth's original patterns of A1-A2, B1-5, and C1-C2 patterns. It identifies the additional patterns of A3-8 and C3-8. It also describes how A and C patterns can be combined by individuals, such as A4-C5/6.[49][48] It replaces the disorganized category with finer descriptions of the A and C attachment patterns. The DMM also uses a different naming scheme for the general and specific patterns.

Adult Attachment Projective Picture System (AAP) edit

Developed by Carol George and Malcolm West in 1999, this is a free response task that involves telling stories in response to eight picture stimuli (1 warm-up & 7 attachment scenes). The advantage of using a picture free-response system is that individuals are not asked to describe their own experiences, a method that has been shown to be subject to social desirability and defensive processes especially for assessing attachment trauma. (This method is similar to the School-Age Assessment of Attachment (SAA), below.) George and colleagues have described the AAP assessment in articles and books,[50][51] and in a 2023 book, how to implement the AAP in clinical practice for assessment, client conceptualization, treatment planning, analysis, and as a therapeutic guide.[52]

The AAP can identify three organized patterns of attachment, secure, insecure-avoidant (or deactivating defenses), insecure-preoccupied/ambivalent (or cognitive disconnection), and also insecure-unresolved (disorganized attachment).[53][54] The AAP identifies the same adult attachment groups as the early version of the AAI (Berkeley model), as described above. In addition to providing adult group classifications, the AAP is also used to code individual attachment defensive information processing patterns, experiences of attachment trauma, attachment synchrony, and personal agency.[53]

The strongest concurrent validation of the measure is the correspondence between AAP and AAI classification agreement. The AAP is demonstrated to be increasingly useful in clinical and neurobiological settings.[55]

Patient Attachment Coding System (PACS) edit

Developed by Alessandro Talia and Madeleine Miller-Bottome in 2012, it is a language-based, observational measure of attachment used in the context of psychotherapy, described in Talia, Miller-Bottome, & Daniel (2017) in the journal Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy[56] and other publications.[57][58] The PACS has been defined as "leading to a paradigm shift in attachment-informed research and clinical practice.".[59] Rather than coding the form or content of patients' discourse, the PACS tracks the frequency of specific discourse markers that reflect how the patient regulates trust and connection with the therapist. In this way, the PACS contributes to our knowledge of specific types of communication and behaviors that distinguish patients of different attachment patterns in psychotherapy:

  • Secure: They tell clear, well-structured narratives that are easy to visualize; they describe relationships and experiences together with elaborating upon the impact they had on them; they offer interpretations about others' and their own behavior with tentativeness and curiosity
  • Avoidant: They downplay either their appraisal of relationships, or the effect these had on them; they are not likely to recount episodes, disclose feelings, or reflect on inner determinants of experiences
  • Preoccupied: They present narratives that are unclear, full of irrelevant details; they describe relationships and experiences with either excessive certainty, or excessive vagueness and lack of a clear stance.

The PACS identifies the same adult attachment groups as the AAI or the AAP, as well as yielding dimensional scores for "linguistic behaviors" such as Proximity seeking, Contact Maintaining, Exploring, Avoidance, Resistance. The strongest concurrent validation of the measure is the correspondence with the AAI (k = .82), and the Reflective Functioning Scale (r = .72).[60] The PACS is increasingly used in clinical settings.

Social psychology Self-report questionnaires edit

Hazan and Shaver created the first questionnaire to measure attachment in adults. [61] Their questionnaire was designed to classify adults into the three attachment styles identified by Ainsworth. The questionnaire consisted of three sets of statements, each set of statements describing an attachment style:

  • Secure - I find it relatively easy to get close to others and am comfortable depending on them and having them depend on me. I don't often worry about being abandoned or about someone getting too close to me.
  • Avoidant - I am somewhat uncomfortable being close to others; I find it difficult to trust them completely, difficult to allow myself to depend on them. I am nervous when anyone gets too close, and often, love partners want me to be more intimate than I feel comfortable being.
  • Anxious/Ambivalent - I find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I often worry that my partner doesn't really love me or won't want to stay with me. I want to merge completely with another person, and this desire sometimes scares people away.

People participating in their study were asked to choose which set of statements best described their feelings. The chosen set of statements indicated their attachment style. Later versions of this questionnaire presented scales so people could rate how well each set of statements described their feelings.

One important advance in the development of attachment questionnaires was the addition of a fourth style of attachment. Bartholomew and Horowitz presented a model that identified four categories or styles of adult attachment. [62] Their model was based on the idea attachment styles reflected people's thoughts about their partners and thought about themselves. Specifically, attachment styles depended on whether or not people judge their partners to be generally accessible and responsive to requests for support, and whether or not people judge themselves to be the kind of individuals towards which others want to respond and lend help. They proposed four categories based on positive or negative thoughts about partners and on positive or negative thoughts about self.

Attachment Theory Four Category Model
  Thoughts of Self
  Positive Negative
Thoughts
of
Partner
Positive Secure
Comfortable with intimacy and autonomy
Preoccupied
Preoccupied with relationships
Negative Dismissive
Dismissing of intimacy
Strongly independent
Fearful
Fearful of intimacy
Socially avoidant

Bartholomew and Horowitz used this model to create the Relationship Questionnaire (RQ-CV). The RQ-CV consisted of four sets of statements, each describing a category or style of attachment:

  • Secure - It is relatively easy for me to become emotionally close to others. I am comfortable depending on others and having others depend on me. I don't worry about being alone or having others not accept me.
  • Dismissive - I am comfortable without close emotional relationships. It is very important to me to feel independent and self-sufficient, and I prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me.
  • Preoccupied - I want to be completely emotionally intimate with others, but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I am uncomfortable being without close relationships, but I sometimes worry that others don't value me as much as I value them.
  • Fearful - I am somewhat uncomfortable getting close to others. I want emotionally close relationships, but I find it difficult to trust others completely, or to depend on them. I sometimes worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others.

Tests demonstrated the four attachment styles were distinct in how they related to other kinds of psychological variables. Adults indeed appeared to have four styles of attachment instead of three attachment styles.

David Schmitt, together with a large number of colleagues, validated the attachment questionnaire created by Bartholomew and Horowitz in 62 cultures. [63] The distinction of thoughts about self and thoughts about partners proved valid in nearly all cultures. However, the way these two kinds of thoughts interacted to form attachment styles varied somewhat across cultures. The four attachment styles had somewhat different meanings across cultures.

A second important advance in attachment questionnaires was the use of independent items to assess attachment. Instead of asking people to choose between three or four sets of statements, people rated how strongly they agreed with dozens of individual statements. The ratings for the individual statements were combined to provide an attachment score. Investigators have created several questionnaires using this strategy to measure adult attachment.

Two popular questionnaires of this type are the Experiences in Close Relationships (ECR) questionnaire and the Experiences in Close Relationships - Revised (ECR-R) questionnaire. The ECR was created by Brennan, Clark, and Shaver in 1998. [64] The ECR-R was created by Fraley, Waller, and Brennan in 2000. [65]

Analysis of the ECR and ECR-R reveal that the questionnaire items can be grouped into two dimensions of attachment. One group of questionnaire items deal with how anxious a person is about their relationship. These items serve as a scale for anxiety. The remaining items deal with how avoidant a person is in their relationship. These items serve as a scale for avoidance. Many researchers now use scores from the anxiety and avoidance scales to perform statistical analyses and test hypotheses.

Scores on the anxiety and avoidance scales can still be used to classify people into the four adult attachment styles.[64][66][67] The four styles of attachment defined in Bartholomew and Horowitz's model were based on thoughts about self and thoughts about partners. The anxiety scale in the ECR and ECR-R reflect thoughts about self. Attachment anxiety relates to beliefs about self-worth and whether or not one will be accepted or rejected by others. The avoidance scale in the ECR and ECR-R relates to thoughts about partners. Attachment avoidance relates to beliefs about taking risks in approaching or avoiding other people. Combinations of anxiety and avoidance can thus be used to define the four attachment styles. The secure style of attachment is characterized by low anxiety and low avoidance; the preoccupied style of attachment is characterized by high anxiety and low avoidance; the dismissive avoidant style of attachment is characterized by low anxiety and high avoidance; and the fearful avoidant style of attachment is characterized by high anxiety and high avoidance.

 
Two dimensional model of adult attachment related to the four styles of adult attachment.

Assessments using DMM theory and method edit

DMM-informed assessments offer a full lifespan set of assessments, many adapted from existing attachment assessments. Some assessments are validated and some are still in development or the validation process.

Infant CARE-Index (ICI) edit

This validated assessment, described above, primarily assess interaction rather than attachment in a 3-minute observation.[68][69][70][71]

Toddler CARE-Index (TCI) edit

The TCI is undergoing a 15+ year validation process. It assess the general attachment characteristics of a child 15-72 months old. The assessment is a shortened version of the Strange Situation Procedure and involves a 5-minute video recorded play interaction between a child and caregiver with a frustration task and post-frustration repair attempt.[72][18]

DMM-Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) edit

As described above but the DMM version covers a more narrow age range of 11-15 months.[15]

Preschool Assessment of Attachment (PAA) edit

As described above, this validated assessment is a slightly modified version of the SSP to cover a wider range of ages 2-5 years old.[73][74]

School-Age Assessment of Attachment (SAA) edit

Similar to the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System above and the CAPA below, it invites a free response task from a child 6-13 years old after seeing a picture stimuli. It uses a DMM discourse analysis technique to assess attachment.[75][76] The SAA was the subject of a special section of Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry in July 2017, and is a validated assessment.[77]

Meaning of the Child interview (MotC) edit

The MotC uses an interview and discourse technique similar to the AAI. It assesses a parent's general pattern of caregiving, sensitivity and responsiveness to their child.[78][79]

Child Attachment and Play Assessment (CAPA) edit

This assesses the attachment and exploration systems for children 7-11. It uses a process similar to the School-Age Assessment of Attachment and Adult Attachment Projective Picture System above.[80]

Transition to Adulthood Attachment Interview (TAAI) edit

This assessment is undergoing a development and validation process. It assess attachment patterns in people 14-25 years old, and is a version of the AAI modified to be age and development appropriate.[81][82]

DMM-Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) edit

As described above, this well validated assessment considers many aspects of the adult attachment system.[48]

Criticism edit

Existing measures have not necessarily been developed to a useful level. "Behavioral observation is a natural starting point for assessing attachment disorders because behavioral descriptions... have been central to the development of the concept... despite the fact that observations have figured prominently... no established observational protocol has been established".[83]

Also, questionable measures of attachment in school-age children have been presented. For example, a protocol for establishing attachment status was described by Sheperis and his colleagues.[84] Unfortunately, this protocol was validated against another technique, the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire, that was itself poorly validated and that is based on a nonconventional view of attachment.

Reception and development of SSP edit

Psychiatrist Michael Rutter described, relatively early in the development and use of the SSP, some limitations of the procedure in the following terms;[85]

 
Mother and child

"It is by no means free of limitations (see Lamb, Thompson, Gardener, Charnov & Estes, 1984).[86] To begin with, it is very dependent on brief separations and reunions having the same meaning for all children. This may be a major constraint when applying the procedure in cultures, such as that in Japan (see Miyake et al.,, 1985),[87] where infants are rarely separated from their mothers in ordinary circumstances. Also, because older children have a cognitive capacity to maintain relationships when the older person is not present, separation may not provide the same stress for them. Modified procedures based on the Strange Situation have been developed for older preschool children (see Belsky et al., 1994; Greenberg et al., 1990)[88][89] but it is much more dubious whether the same approach can be used in middle childhood. Also, despite its manifest strengths, the procedure is based on just 20 minutes of behaviour. It can be scarcely expected to tap all the relevant qualities of a child's attachment relationships. Q-sort procedures based on much longer naturalistic observations in the home, and interviews with the mothers have developed in order to extend the data base (see Vaughn & Waters, 1990).[90] A further constraint is that the coding procedure results in discrete categories rather than continuously distributed dimensions. Not only is this likely to provide boundary problems, but also it is not at all obvious that discrete categories best represent the concepts that are inherent in attachment security. It seems much more likely that infants vary in their degree of security and there is need for a measurement systems that can quantify individual variation".

Subsequent research has confirmed the SSP, and the DMM-PAA version, have value across cultures and may pick up cultural differences,[91] and the age range for which each is useful has been determined.[92] Attachment behaviors have been identified in nonhuman primates[91] and dogs.[93] The 21-minute procedure provides a massive amount of data, and Crittenden's TCI, a 5-minute version of the PAA, is a useful assessment. As with all other attachment assessments, these three are necessarily limited in what they can discover, and the information they can discover is powerful.[2] The DMM model is multi-dimensional in several ways.[48] Infant attachment assessments have been validated.[18]

Ecological validity and universality of Strange Situation attachment classification distributions edit

With respect to the ecological validity of the Strange Situation, a meta-analysis of 2,000 infant-parent dyads, including several from studies with non-Western language and/or cultural bases found the global distribution of attachment categorizations to be A (21%), B (65%), and C (14%).[94] This global distribution was generally consistent with Ainsworth et al.'s (1978) original attachment classification distributions.

However, controversy has been raised over a few cultural differences in these rates of 'global' attachment classification distributions. In particular, two studies diverged from the global distributions of attachment classifications noted above. One study was conducted in North Germany[95] in which more avoidant (A) infants were found than global norms would suggest, and the other in Sapporo, Japan[96] where more resistant (C) infants were found. Of these two studies, the Japanese findings have sparked the most controversy as to the meaning of individual differences in attachment behavior as originally identified by Ainsworth et al. (1978).

In a recent study conducted in Sapporo, Behrens, et al., 2007.[97] found attachment distributions consistent with global norms using the six-year Main & Cassidy scoring system for attachment classification.[98] In addition to these findings supporting the global distributions of attachment classifications in Sapporo, Behrens et al. also discuss the Japanese concept of amae and its relevance to questions concerning whether the insecure-resistant (C) style of interaction may be engendered in Japanese infants as a result of the cultural practice of amae.

Attachment measurement: discrete or continuous? edit

Regarding the issue of whether the breadth of infant attachment functioning can be captured by a categorical classification scheme, continuous measures of attachment security have been developed which have demonstrated adequate psychometric properties. These have been used either individually or in conjunction with discrete attachment classifications in many published reports.[99][100] The original Richter’s et al. (1998) scale is strongly related to secure versus insecure classifications, correctly predicting about 90% of cases.[100] Readers further interested in the categorical versus continuous nature of attachment classifications (and the debate surrounding this issue) should consult the paper by Fraley and Spieker[101] and the rejoinders in the same issue by many prominent attachment researchers including J. Cassidy, A. Sroufe, E. Waters & T. Beauchaine, and M. Cummings.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Ainsworth, Mary D.; Blehar, Mary C.; Waters, Everett; Wall, Sally (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation (1st ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum, original. Routledge Classic Editions, 2015. ISBN 9781848726826.
  2. ^ a b c d e f Duschinsky, Robbie (2020). Cornerstones of attachment research. Oxford New York, NY: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-884206-4.
  3. ^ George, Carol; Solomon, Judith (2008). Cassidy, Jude; Shaver, Phillip R. (eds.). The caregiving system: A behavioral systems approach to parenting (2nd ed.). Guilford Press. pp. 833–856. ISBN 9781606235843.
  4. ^ Granqvist, Pehr; Sroufe, L. Alan; Dozier, Mary; Hesse, Erik; Steele, Miriam; van Ijzendoorn, Marinus; Solomon, Judith; Main, Mary; Fearon, Pasco; Bakermans-Kranenburg, Marian; Steele, Howard; Cassidy, Jude; Carlson, Elizabeth; Madigan, Sheri; Duschinsky, Robbie (2017-11-02). "Disorganized attachment in infancy: a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy-makers". Attachment & Human Development. 19 (6): 534–558. doi:10.1080/14616734.2017.1354040. ISSN 1461-6734. PMC 5600694. PMID 28745146.
  5. ^ Ainsworth. Mary D. (1978) Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-89859-461-8.
  6. ^ Main, M. and Solomon, J. (1986) 'Discovery of an insecure disorganized/dioriented attachment pattern:procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior.' In t. Braxelton and M.Yogman (eds) Affective development in infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
  7. ^ Main, m. and Solomon, J. (1990) 'Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation' In M.Greenberg, D. Cicchetti and E. Cummings (eds) Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  8. ^ Bell, S. (1970). "The development of the concept of the object as related to infant-mother attachment". Child Development. 41 (2): 291–311. doi:10.2307/1127033. JSTOR 1127033.
  9. ^ Crittenden, P. (1985). "Social networks, quality of parenting, and child development". Child Development. 56 (5): 1299–1313. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1985.tb00198.x.
  10. ^ Radke-Yarrow, M.; Cummings, E.M.; Kuczynski, L.; Chapman, M. (1985). "Patterns of attachment in two and three year olds in normal families and families with parental depression". Child Development. 56 (4): 884–893. doi:10.2307/1130100. JSTOR 1130100. PMID 4042751.
  11. ^ Main, M.; Cassidy, J. (1988). "Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age six: predictability from infant attachment classifications stable across a one-month period". Developmental Psychology. 24 (3): 415–426. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.3.415.
  12. ^ a b Crittenden, P.M. (1992). "Quality of attachment in the preschool years". Development and Psychopathology. 4 (2): 209–241. doi:10.1017/s0954579400000110. S2CID 143894461.
  13. ^ Shah, P. E.; Fonagy, P.; Strathearn, L. (2010). "Exploring the mechanism of intergenerational transmission of attachment: The plot thickens". Clinical Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 15 (3): 329–346. doi:10.1177/1359104510365449. PMC 3060612. PMID 20603421.
  14. ^ Snyder, R.; Shapiro, S.; Treleaven, D. (2012). "Attachment Theory and Mindfulness". Journal of Child & Family Studies. 21 (5): 709–717. doi:10.1007/s10826-011-9522-8. S2CID 56009083.
  15. ^ a b Farnfield, Steve; Hautamäki, Airi; Nørbech, Peder; Sahhar, Nicola (2010). "DMM assessments of attachment and adaptation: Procedures, validity and utility". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 15 (3): 313–328. doi:10.1177/1359104510364315. ISSN 1359-1045. PMID 20603420. S2CID 206707459.
  16. ^ Ostler, Teresa (2010). "Assessing parenting risk within the context of severe and persistent mental illness: Validating an observational measure for families with child protective service involvement". Infant Mental Health Journal. 31 (5): 467–485. doi:10.1002/imhj.20267. ISSN 0163-9641. PMID 28543839.
  17. ^ Simo, S., Rauh, H., & Ziegenhain, U. (2000). Mother-infant interaction during the first eighteen months and attachment security at the end of the second year. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 47(2), 118-141.
  18. ^ a b c Tryphonopoulos, Panagiota D.; Letourneau, Nicole; Ditommaso, Enrico (2014-09-12). "Attachment and caregiver-infant interaction of observational -assessment tools". Infant Mental Health Journal. 35 (6): 642–656. doi:10.1002/imhj.21461. ISSN 0163-9641. PMID 25798513.
  19. ^ Svanberg, P.O.; Mennet, Lisa; Spieker, Susan (2010). "Promoting a secure attachment: A primary prevention practice model". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 15 (3): 363–378. doi:10.1177/1359104510367584. ISSN 1359-1045. PMID 20603424. S2CID 206707508.
  20. ^ Andreassen, C., & West, J. (2007). Measuring socioemotional functioning in a national birth cohort study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28(6), 627-646.
  21. ^ Lieberman, A. F. (1977). "Preschoolers' competence with a peer: Relations with attachment and peer experience". Child Development. 48 (4): 1277–1287. doi:10.2307/1128485. JSTOR 1128485.
  22. ^ a b Greenberg, M. T.; Cicchetti, D.; Cummings, E. M. (1993). Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. University of Chicago Press.
  23. ^ Solomon, J.; George, C. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 287–316.
  24. ^ Main, M. & Cassidy, J. (1988) "Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6: predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1-month period. Developmental Psychology 24, 415-426.
  25. ^ Crittenden PM (1992). . Development and Psychopathology. 4 (2): 209–41. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000110. S2CID 143894461. Archived from the original on 2008-03-14. Retrieved 2008-01-06.
  26. ^ Teti, Douglas M.; Gelfand, Donna M. (1997). "The Preschool Assessment of Attachment: Construct validity in a sample of depressed and nondepressed families". Development and Psychopathology. 9 (3): 517–536. doi:10.1017/s0954579497001284. ISSN 0954-5794. PMID 9327237. S2CID 42553006.
  27. ^ Crittenden, Patricia McKinsey; Claussen, Angelika Hartl; Kozlowska, Kasia (2007). "Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use: A Comparative Study". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy. 28 (2): 78–87. doi:10.1375/anft.28.2.78. ISSN 0814-723X.
  28. ^ "MIM Research".
  29. ^ a b Waters, E. and Deane, K. (1985) 'Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood.' In I. Bretherton and E. Waters (eds) Growing pains of attachment theory and research: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50, Serial No. 209 (1-2), 41-65
  30. ^ Vaughn, B. E.; Waters, E. (1990). "Attachment behavior at home and in the laboratory: Q-sort observations and strange situation classifications of one-year-olds". Child Development. 61 (6): 1965–1973. doi:10.2307/1130850. JSTOR 1130850. PMID 2083508.
  31. ^ Clements, M.; Barnett, D. (2002). "Parenting and attachment among toddlers with congenital anomalies: Examining the strange situation and attachment Q-sort". Infant Mental Health Journal. 23 (6): 625–642. doi:10.1002/imhj.10040.
  32. ^ Goodman, G; Pfeffer, CR (1998). "Attachment disorganization in prepubertal children with severe emotional disturbance". Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic. 62 (4): 490–525. PMID 9810111.
  33. ^ Solomon, J.; George, C. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 102–123.
  34. ^ Solomon, J., George, C., & De Jong, A. (1995). Children classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and school. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 447-464.
  35. ^ "MCAST (University of Manchester)".
  36. ^ Green, J.M., Stanley, C., Smith, V., & Goldwyn, R. (2000). A new method of evaluating attachment representations on young school age children - the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 42-64.
  37. ^ Goldwyn, R., Green, J.M, Stanley, C., & Smith V. (2000). The Manchester Child Attachment Story Task: Relationship with parental AAI, SAT and child behaviour. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 65-78.
  38. ^ Barone, L., Del Guidice, M., Fossati, A., Manaresi, F., Actis Perinitti, B., Colle, L., & Veglia, F. (2009). Psychometric Properties of the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task: an Italian multicentre study. International Journal of Behavioural Development. 33 (2) 185-190.
  39. ^ Wan, M.W. & Green, J. (2010). Negative and atypical story content themes depicted by children with behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1125-1131
  40. ^ Target, M., Fonagy, P. and Schmueli-Goetz, Y. (2003) 'Attachment representations in school-age children: the development of the Child Attachment Interview (CAI).' Journal of Child Psychotherapy 29, 2, 171-186
  41. ^ Smyke, A. and Zeanah, C. (1999)'Disturbances of Attachment Interview'. Available on the Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry website at www.jaacap.com
  42. ^ Shaver, P.A.; Fraley, R.C. (2004). "Self-report measures of adult attachment". Retrieved June 20, 2006.
  43. ^ George, Carol; Kaplan, Nancy; Main, Mary (1996). "Adult Attachment Interview" (PDF). Department of Psychology. Unpublished Manuscript (Third ed.). University of California, Berkeley.
  44. ^ a b c Duschinsky, Robbie (2020). Cornerstones of attachment research. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-884206-4. OCLC 1154774522.
  45. ^ Granqvist, Pehr; Sroufe, L. Alan; Main, Mary; Hesse, Erik; Steele, Miriam; van Ijzendoorn, Marinus; Solomon, Judith; Schuengel, Carlo; Fearon, Pasco; Bakermans-Kranenburg, Marian; and 33 other authors (2017-07-26). "Disorganized attachment in infancy: a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy-makers". Attachment & Human Development. 19 (6): 534–558. doi:10.1080/14616734.2017.1354040. ISSN 1461-6734. PMC 5600694. PMID 28745146.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  46. ^ a b Baldoni, Franco; Minghetti, Mattia; Craparo, Giuseppe; Facondini, Elisa; Cena, Loredana; Schimmenti, Adriano (2018-07-04). "Comparing Main, Goldwyn, and Hesse (Berkeley) and Crittenden (DMM) coding systems for classifying Adult Attachment Interview transcripts: an empirical report". Attachment & Human Development. 20 (4): 423–438. doi:10.1080/14616734.2017.1421979. hdl:11379/501534. ISSN 1461-6734. PMID 29308700. S2CID 205815474.
  47. ^ Shaver, P. R., Belsky, J., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). The adult attachment interview and self-reports of romantic attachment: Associations across domains and methods. Personal Relationships, 7, 25-43.
  48. ^ a b c d e Crittenden, Patricia McKinsey; Landini, Andrea (2011). Assessing adult attachment : a dynamic-maturational approach to discourse analysis. New York: W.W Norton & Co. ISBN 978-0-393-70667-3. OCLC 667877268.
  49. ^ Crittenden, Patricia M.; Newman, Louise (2010). "Comparing models of borderline personality disorder: Mothers' experience, self-protective strategies, and dispositional representations". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 15 (3): 433–451. doi:10.1177/1359104510368209. ISSN 1359-1045.
  50. ^ George, Carol; West, Malcolm L. (2001). "The development and preliminary validation of a new measure of adult attachment: the Adult Attachment Projective". Attachment & Human Development. 3 (1): 30–61. doi:10.1080/14616730010024771. ISSN 1461-6734.
  51. ^ George, Carol; West, Malcolm L., eds. (2012). The Adult Attachment Projective Picture System: Attachment theory and assessment in adults (Illustrated ed.). The Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1462504251.
  52. ^ George, Carol; Wargo Aikens, Julie; Lehmann, Melissa, eds. (2023). Working with Attachment Trauma: Clinical Application of the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System. New York: Routledge. ISBN 978-1-003-21543-1.
  53. ^ a b Gander, Manuela; George, Carol; Pokorny, Dan; Buchheim, Anna (2016-03-26). "Assessing Attachment Representations in Adolescents: Discriminant Validation of the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System". Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 48 (2): 270–282. doi:10.1007/s10578-016-0639-2. ISSN 0009-398X. PMC 5323491. PMID 27016937.
  54. ^ Solomon, Judith; George, Carol, eds. (2011). Disorganized attachment and caregiving. Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-60918-128-4. OCLC 912464422.
  55. ^ Buchheim, Anna; Erk, Susanne; George, Carol; Kächele, Horst; Kircher, Tilo; Martius, Philipp; Pokorny, Dan; Ruchsow, Martin; Spitzer, Manfred; Walter, Henrik (August 2008). "Neural correlates of attachment trauma in borderline personality disorder: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 163 (3): 223–235. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.07.001. ISSN 0925-4927. PMID 18635342. S2CID 5026031.
  56. ^ Talia, A.; Miller-Bottome, M.; Daniel, S. I. F. (2017). Assessing attachment in psychotherapy: Validation study of the Patient Attachment Coding System (PACS). Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 24(1), 149-161
  57. ^ Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., Katznelson, H., Pedersen, S. H., Steele, H., Schröder, P., ... & Lingiardi, V. (2019). Mentalizing in the presence of another: Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process. Psychotherapy Research, 29(5), 652-665.
  58. ^ Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., Wyner, R., Lilliengren, P., & Bate, J. (2019). Patients’ Adult Attachment Interview classification and their experience of the therapeutic relationship: are they associated?. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 22(2).
  59. ^ Slade, A. (2016). "Attachment and Adult Psychotherapy Theory, Research, and Practice". In: Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. (Eds) Handbook of Attachment Third edition
  60. ^ Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., Katznelson, H., Pedersen, S. H., Steele, H., Schröder, P., ... & Taubner, S. (2019). Mentalizing in the presence of another: Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process. Psychotherapy Research, 29(5), 652-665.
  61. ^ Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachmenpt process" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52, 511-524.
  62. ^ Bartholomew, K. & Horowitz, L.M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61, 226-244.
  63. ^ Schmitt, D.P., et al. (2004). Patterns and universals of adult romantic attachment across 62 cultural regions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 367-402.
  64. ^ a b Brennan, K.A., Clark, C.L., & Shaver, P.R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J.A. Simpson & W.S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford Press.
  65. ^ Fraley, R.C., Waller, N.G., & Brennan, K.A. (2000). An item-response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78, 350-365.
  66. ^ Bartholomew, K. & Shaver, P.R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult attachment. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships, pp. 25-45. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  67. ^ Collins, N.L. & Freeney, B.C. (2004). An Attachment Theory Perspective on Closeness and Intimacy. In D.J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of Closeness and Intimacy, pp. 163-188. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  68. ^ Crittenden, Patricia M.; DiLalla, David L. (1988). "Compulsive compliance: The development of an inhibitory coping strategy in infancy". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 16 (5): 585–599. doi:10.1007/bf00914268. ISSN 0091-0627. PMID 3235749. S2CID 30494458.
  69. ^ Hautamäki, Airi (2014). "Mothers and infants: Screening for maternal relationships at risk with the CARE-index". In Farnfield, Steve; Holmes, Paul (eds.). The Routledge handbook of attachment: assessment. London; New York: Routledge. pp. 37–52. ISBN 978-0-415-53825-1. OCLC 841895216.
  70. ^ Forcada-Guex, Margarita; Pierrehumbert, Blaise; Borghini, Ayala; Moessinger, Adrien; Muller-Nix, Carole (2006-07-01). "Early Dyadic Patterns of Mother–Infant Interactions and Outcomes of Prematurity at 18 Months". Pediatrics. 118 (1): e107–e114. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1145. ISSN 0031-4005. PMID 16818525. S2CID 9258568.
  71. ^ Parfitt, Ylva; Pike, Alison; Ayers, Susan (2013-11-21). "Infant Developmental Outcomes: A Family Systems Perspective". Infant and Child Development. 23 (4): 353–373. doi:10.1002/icd.1830. ISSN 1522-7227.
  72. ^ Crittenden, Patricia M. (2014). Toddler CARE-Index (TCI) Manual, unpublished training manual. Miami, FL: Family Relations Institute.
  73. ^ Crittenden, Patricia McKinsey; Claussen, Angelika Hartl; Kozlowska, Kasia (2007). "Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use: A Comparative Study". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy. 28 (2): 78–87. doi:10.1375/anft.28.2.78. ISSN 0814-723X.
  74. ^ The Routledge handbook of attachment : assessment. Steve Farnfield, Paul Holmes. London. 2014. ISBN 978-0-415-53824-4. OCLC 841895216.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) CS1 maint: others (link)
  75. ^ Crittenden, Patricia; Kozlowska, Kasia; Landini, Andrea (2010-02-22). "Assessing attachment in school-age children". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 15 (2): 185–208. doi:10.1177/1359104509356741. ISSN 1359-1045. PMID 20176770. S2CID 11582111.
  76. ^ Crittenden, Patricia M; Robson, Katrina; Tooby, Alison; Fleming, Charles (2017-04-21). "Are mothers' protective attachment strategies related to their children's strategies?". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 22 (3): 358–377. doi:10.1177/1359104517704027. ISSN 1359-1045. PMID 28429614. S2CID 4824676.
  77. ^ "Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry – Volume 22, Number 3, Jul 01, 2017". journals.sagepub.com.
  78. ^ Grey, Ben; Farnfield, Steve (2016-03-02). "The Meaning of the Child Interview: A new procedure for assessing and understanding parent–child relationships of 'at-risk' families". Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 22 (2): 204–218. doi:10.1177/1359104516633495. ISSN 1359-1045. PMID 26940120. S2CID 206708220.
  79. ^ Grey, Ben; Farnfield, Steve (2017-03-20). "The Meaning of the Child Interview (MotC) – the initial validation of a new procedure for assessing and understanding the parent-child relationships of "at risk" families". Journal of Children's Services. 12 (1): 16–31. doi:10.1108/jcs-03-2016-0006. ISSN 1746-6660.
  80. ^ Farnfield, Steve (2016). "The Child Attachment and Play Assessment (CAPA): Validation of a new approach to coding narrative stems with children ages 3–11 years". International Journal of Play Therapy. 25 (4): 217–229. doi:10.1037/a0038726. ISSN 1939-0629.
  81. ^ Crittenden, Patricia M. (2005). Transition to Adulthood Attachment Interview (TAAI), unpublished training manual. Miami, FL: Family Relations Institute.
  82. ^ Stokowy, Martin; Sahhar, Nicola, eds. (2012), "5 Adoleszenz und das TAAI (Landini/Kozlowska/Davies/Chudleigh", Bindung und Gefahr, Psychosozial-Verlag, pp. 113–140, doi:10.30820/9783837966534-113, ISBN 9783837966534, retrieved 2022-01-15
  83. ^ O'Connor, T., & Zeanah, C.H. (2003)."Attachment disorders: Assessment strategies and treatment approaches." Attachment & Human Development, 5(3):223-244, p. 229
  84. ^ Sheperis, C.J., Doggett, R.A., Hoda, N.E., Blanchard, T., Renfro-Michael, E.L., Holdiness, S.H., & Schlagheck, R. (2003). "The development of an assessment protocol for Reactive Attachment Disorder."Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 25(4):291-310
  85. ^ Rutter, Michael (1995). "The Clinical Implications of Attachment Concepts". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 36 (4): 552–553. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb02314.x. PMID 7650083.
  86. ^ Lamb, Thompson, Gardener, Charnov & Estes, (1984). Security of Infantile attachment as assessed in the 'Strange Situation'; its study and biological interpretations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 127-147
  87. ^ Miyake, Chen, & Campos (1985). Infant temperament and mother's mode of interaction and attachment in Japan; an interim report; In I. Bretherton & E Waters (Eds), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, Serial No 209, 276-297.
  88. ^ Belsky, J. & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment Theory and Evidence. In M. Rutter & D. Hay (Eds) Development Through Life; A Handbook For Clinicians (pp. 373-402). Oxford; Blackwell Scientific Publications.
  89. ^ Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D. & Cummings, M. (Eds), (1990). Attachment in the preschool years; theory research and intervention. Chicago; University of Chicago Press.
  90. ^ Vaughn, B. E. & Waters, E. (1990). Attachment behaviour at home and in the laboratory" Child Development 61, 1965-1973.
  91. ^ a b Cassidy, Jude; Shaver, Phillip Robert (2018). Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed.). New York (N.Y.): Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-4625-3664-1.
  92. ^ Crittenden, Patricia McKinsey; Claussen, Angelika Hartl; Kozlowska, Kasia (2007). "Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use: A Comparative Study". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy. 28 (2): 78–87. doi:10.1375/anft.28.2.78. ISSN 0814-723X.
  93. ^ Solomon, J.; Beetz, A.; Schöberl, I.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. (2019-07-04). "Attachment security in companion dogs: adaptation of Ainsworth's strange situation and classification procedures to dogs and their human caregivers". Attachment & Human Development. 21 (4): 389–417. doi:10.1080/14616734.2018.1517812. ISSN 1461-6734. PMC 6532729. PMID 30246604.
  94. ^ Van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Kroonenberg, P.M. (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the strange-situation" Child Development 59, 147-156.
  95. ^ Grossmann, K.E., Grossmann, K., Huber, F., & Wartner, U. (1981). German children's behavior toward their mothers at 12 months and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworth's strange situation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 157-184.
  96. ^ Takahashi, K. (1986). Examining the Strange-Situation procedure with Japanese mothers and 12-month old infants" Developmental Psychology 22, 265-270.
  97. ^ Behrens, K. Y., Main, M., & Hesse, E. (2007). Mothers’ Attachment Status as Determined by the Adult Attachment Interview Predicts Their 6-Year-Olds’ Reunion Responses: A Study Conducted in Japan" Developmental Psychology 43, 1553–1567.
  98. ^ Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6: Predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1-month period" Developmental Psychology 24, 415-426.
  99. ^ Richters, J. E., Waters, E., & Vaughn, B. E. (1988). Empirical classification of infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and crying during reunion" Child Development 59, 512-522.
  100. ^ a b Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (1990). Cross-cultural consistency of coding the strange situation. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 469-485.
  101. ^ Fraley, C. R., & Spieker, S. J. (2003). Are Infant Attachment Patterns Continuously or Categorically Distributed? A Taxometric Analysis of Strange Situation Behavior" Developmental Psychology 39, 387-404.

Further reading edit

  • Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P., (Eds). (1999) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. Guilford Press, NY.
  • Greenberg, MT, Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, EM., (Eds) (1990) Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research and Intervention University of Chicago, Chicago.
  • Greenspan, S. (1993) Infancy and Early Childhood. Madison, CT: International Universities Press. ISBN 0-8236-2633-4.
  • Holmes, J. (1993) John Bowlby and Attachment Theory. Routledge. ISBN 0-415-07730-3.
  • Holmes, J. (2001) The Search for the Secure Base: Attachment Theory and Psychotherapy. London: Brunner-Routledge. ISBN 1-58391-152-9.
  • Karen R (1998) Becoming Attached: First Relationships and How They Shape Our Capacity to Love. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-511501-5.
  • Parkes, CM, Stevenson-Hinde, J., Marris, P., (Eds.) (1991) Attachment Across The Life Cycle Routledge. NY. ISBN 0-415-05651-9
  • Siegler R., DeLoache, J. & Eisenberg, N. (2003) How Children develop. New York: Worth. ISBN 1-57259-249-4.

External links edit

  • AICAN - Australian Intercountry Adoption Network
  • Attachment Questionnaire

attachment, measures, attachment, assessments, various, procedures, used, assess, attachment, system, children, adults, these, procedures, assess, patterns, attachment, individual, self, protective, strategies, some, assessments, work, across, several, models,. Attachment measures or attachment assessments are the various procedures used to assess the attachment system in children and adults These procedures can assess patterns of attachment and individual self protective strategies Some assessments work across the several models of attachment and some are model specific Many assessments allow children and adults attachment strategies to be classified into three primary attachment pattern groups 1 B pattern balanced secure A pattern avoidant dismissive cognitive C pattern anxious ambivalent preoccupied affective In most models each pattern group is further broken down into several sub patterns 2 Some assessments are capable of finding additional information about an individual such as unresolved trauma depression history of family triangulation and lifespan changes in the attachment pattern Some assessments specifically or additionally look for caregiving behaviors as caregiving and attachment are widely considered two separate systems for organizing thoughts feelings and behavior 3 2 Some methods assess disorders of attachment or romantic attachment Attachment models are typically generated from the schools of developmental science or social psychology although both emanate from the Bowlby Ainsworth framework 2 Ainsworth s Strange Situation Procedure was the first formal attachment assessment and is still in wide use Each school while having the same basic foundation may be studying different phenomenon 2 467 Assessments are typically conducted by observing behavior in a structured setting by analyzing the transcript of a structured interview using technical discourse analysis methods or by self reports from a questionnaire Social psychology models primarily utilize self reports Some attachment models such as the Berkeley or ABC D model consider disorganized attachment to be a pattern or category The D classification was thought to represent a breakdown in the attachment caregiving partnership such that the child does not have an organized behavioral or representational strategy to achieve protection and care from the attachment figure However the disorganized concept has been determined to be invalid for people older than 20 months 4 Other models such as the Dynamic Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation DMM describe virtually all attachment behavior and patterns within or in a combination of the three primary A B C patterns The DMM considers all attachment behavior to be an organized effort to adapt within a given caregiving environment to optimize available caregiver protection and maximize survival Contents 1 Measures of attachment in infants 1 1 Strange Situation Procedure SSP 1 2 Infant CARE Index ICI 2 Measures of attachment in toddlerhood and early middle childhood 2 1 Controlled observational methods 2 1 1 Preschool strange situation 2 1 2 Main amp Cassidy attachment classification system 2 1 3 Preschool Assessment of Attachment PAA 2 1 4 Marschak Interaction Method MIM 2 2 Naturalistic observational methods 2 2 1 Attachment Q sort 2 3 Representational story stem narrative and picture methods 2 3 1 Attachment Story Completion Task ASCT 2 3 2 Attachment Doll Play Assessment ADPA 2 3 3 Manchester Child Attachment Story Task MCAST 2 3 4 Picture response techniques 2 4 Interview methods 2 4 1 Child Attachment Interview CAI 2 4 2 Disturbances of Attachment Interview DAI 3 Measures of attachment in adolescents 3 1 Attachment Interview for Childhood and Adolescence AICA 4 Measures in adults 4 1 Adult Attachment Interview AAI 4 2 Adult Attachment Projective Picture System AAP 4 3 Patient Attachment Coding System PACS 4 4 Social psychology Self report questionnaires 5 Assessments using DMM theory and method 5 1 Infant CARE Index ICI 5 2 Toddler CARE Index TCI 5 3 DMM Strange Situation Procedure SSP 5 4 Preschool Assessment of Attachment PAA 5 5 School Age Assessment of Attachment SAA 5 6 Meaning of the Child interview MotC 5 7 Child Attachment and Play Assessment CAPA 5 8 Transition to Adulthood Attachment Interview TAAI 5 9 DMM Adult Attachment Interview AAI 6 Criticism 6 1 Reception and development of SSP 6 2 Ecological validity and universality of Strange Situation attachment classification distributions 6 3 Attachment measurement discrete or continuous 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksMeasures of attachment in infants editMain articles Attachment in children and Attachment theory Attachment measures for infants rely on observing the infant with a caregiver typically videotaped either in a natural or contrived environment Measures for older children including toddlers are listed in subsequent sections Strange Situation Procedure SSP edit The Strange Situation procedure was formulated to observe attachment relationships between a caregiver and children between the age of nine and 18 months It was developed by Mary Ainsworth a developmental psychologist 5 Originally it was devised to enable children to be classified into the attachment styles known as secure anxious avoidant and anxious ambivalent As research accumulated and atypical patterns of attachment became more apparent it was further developed by Main and Solomon in 1986 and 1990 to include the new category of disorganized disoriented attachment 6 7 In this procedure the child is observed playing for 20 minutes while caregivers and strangers enter and leave the room recreating the flow of the familiar and unfamiliar presence in most children s lives The situation varies in stressfulness and the child s responses are observed The child experiences the following situations Mother or other familiar caregiver and baby enter room Mother sits quietly on a chair responding if the infant seeks attention A stranger enters talks to the mother then gradually approaches infant with a toy The mother leaves the room The stranger leaves the infant playing unless he she is inactive and then tries to interest the infant in toys If the infant becomes distressed this episode is ended Mother enters and waits to see how the infant greets her The stranger leaves quietly and the mother waits until the baby settles and then she leaves again The infant is alone This episode is curtailed if the infant appears to be distressed The stranger comes back and repeats episode 3 The mother returns and the stranger goes Reunion behaviour is noted and then the situation is ended Two aspects of the child s behaviour are observed The amount of exploration e g playing with new toys the child engages in throughout and The child s reactions to the departure and return of its caregiver Beginning in 1970 a series of expansions were added to Ainsworth s original patterns They include the following B4 1970 8 A C 1985 9 10 D disorganized 1986 B5 1988 1992 11 12 A C and Depressed 1992 2010 12 13 At later ages additional categories have been described Each of these patterns reflects a different kind of attachment relationship of the infant with the mother caregiver An infant may have a different pattern of attachment to each parent as well as to alternate caregivers Pattern of attachment is thus not a part of the infant but is characteristic of the protective and comforting quality of a specific relationship These attachment patterns are associated with behavioral patterns and can help further predict a child s future personality 14 Infant CARE Index ICI edit The Infant CARE Index ICI is procedure that assesses risk in parent infant relationships It was developed by Patricia Crittenden early in the development of the Dynamic Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation DMM and can be used from birth that is before infant s attachment strategies are established and up to 15 months of age The ICI involves a 3 minute video recording of a typical play interaction between a caregiver and infant without requiring a separation or other stressor The focus is on the dyadic synchrony that is the extent to which adult sensitivity to infant signals result in infant cooperation in play 15 Lower synchrony is indicative of higher risk In addition caregiver s interactions with the infant are assessed using three scales sensitive controlling and unresponsive and infant interactions are assessed from the perspective of their caregiver using four scales cooperative compulsive difficult and passive 16 The ICI does not conclude attachment strategies but it is highly correlated to the maternal scales in the infant Strange Situation assessment patterns of attachment 17 It can be used in screening to identify levels of risk and as a tool for clinical intervention and evaluation and has been used in numerous research projects The ICI is a statistically valid and reliable assessment and trained reliable coders can typically code an interaction in 15 20 minutes 18 Tryphonopoulos 2014 The ICI is often used within the context of child welfare in Britain and the wider UK It is also used as a developmental screening tool as a way to identify parents who may benefit from more support or specific interventions that support their ability to sensitively attune to their infant 19 Measures of attachment in toddlerhood and early middle childhood editAs the SSP is not suitable beyond 18 months of age other measures have been developed for older ages groups which include observational measures in a controlled or naturalistic environment representational methods and interview methods Some are developed for research purposes whereas others have been developed for clinical use Effective training of evaluators is essential as some items to be assessed require interpretation reliability e g child is suddenly aggressive toward mother for no reason 20 Controlled observational methods edit Preschool strange situation edit Although originally designed for 1 year old children Ainsworth s strange situation has been adapted to measure the attachment and exploratory behavior of children between the ages of 2 4 years old 21 A fundamental feature of the strange situation is that the situation the child is placed in must elicit stress If the strange situation fails to stress the child it cannot serve as an adequate environment for the measurement of attachment 22 The preschool strange situation features several alterations to facilitate the creation of stress in older children These modifications include a slightly longer separation changes in the role and or gender of the stranger and changes in the instructions to the caregiver Some versions of the preschool strange situations omit the stranger altogether thus leaving the child alone in the room throughout both separations 23 The coding system used to interpret the attachment style expressed by the child has also been modified Rather than focusing entirely on the expression of specific behaviors and emotions the revised coding system assesses ways in which a variety of behaviors such as talking are organized to maintain and negotiate proximity and contact Cassidy Marvin and the MacArthur Working group published a version of the Strange Situation procedure designed for children within the age group of 3 to 4 years old In addition to categorizing a child s attachment as secure insecure avoidant insecure ambivalent and insecure disorganized the measure includes a seven point avoidance scale and nine point security scale 22 Main amp Cassidy attachment classification system edit This system devised in 1988 analyses the reunion of child and parent after a 1 hour separation It is aimed at 6 year olds and classifies their attachment status 24 Preschool Assessment of Attachment PAA edit The PAA was devised by Patricia Crittenden for the purpose of assessing patterns of attachment in 18 month to 5 year old children It uses the coding method developed with the Dynamic Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation DMM Like the SSP it involves an 8 segment 21 minute procedure which is video recorded and then coded The classifications include all the SSP patterns plus patterns that begin to develop during the second year of life A3 A4 and C3 C4 The DMM maintains Ainsworth s ABC labeling scheme but renames the three basic strategies for negotiating interpersonal relationships Balanced for B patterns cognitive for A patterns and affective for C patterns It is also intended to be able to distinguish the unendangered from the endangered compulsive and obsessive subpatterns that may have implications for emotional and behavioral development 25 It has been determined scientifically validated and reliable 26 27 Marschak Interaction Method MIM edit The MIM is a structured observation of the interaction between parent and child The MIM was created by Marianne Marschak in the 1960s at the Yale Child Study Center Salo amp Makela 2006 of Finland have standardized and published a rating scale for the MIM for research purposes Anne Stewart has developed the MIM Behavior Rating Scale MIMBRS 28 Naturalistic observational methods edit Attachment Q sort edit This method devised by Waters and Deane in 1985 utilizes Q Sort methodology It is based on a set period of observation of children aged 1 5 in a number of environments It consists of nearly 100 items intended to cover the spectrum of attachment related behaviors including secure base and exploratory behaviors affective response and social cognition 29 The observer sorts the cards corresponding to the degree to which the child exhibits the item which is then scored 30 The overall score for each child will result in a variable ranging from 1 0 i e very secure to 1 0 i e very insecure 31 Despite its ability to classify secure attachment the score derived from the Q set measure does not classify the type of insecure attachment 29 Representational story stem narrative and picture methods edit This approach uses dolls and narrative to enact a story The dolls represent family members The interviewer enacts the beginning of the story and then hands the dolls over for the child to complete it with varying degrees of prompting and encouragement These techniques are designed to access the child s internal working models of their attachment relationships Methods include the MacArthur Story Stem Battery MSSB and the Attachment Story Completion Test developed in 1990 for children between the age of 3 to 8 years the Story Stem Assessment Profile SSAP developed in 1990 for children aged 4 8 the Attachment Doll Play Assessment developed in 1995 for children age 4 5 11 the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task MCAST developed in 2000 for children aged 4 5 8 5 Attachment Story Completion Task ASCT edit The Attachment Story Completion Task ASCT is a semi projective attachment measure designed by Inge Bretherton and colleagues to assess the internal working model of children between the age of 3 to 9 years old though it requires modification when used with older children The measure evaluates a child s attachment style by analyzing how a child resolves a stress inducing story In a 30 minute recorded interview five story stems are presented through the use of props such as small family figures The stories are designed to access how that child interacts with their primary caregiver in five situations separation confrontation fear reunion and pain 32 The Interviewer prompts the child to complete each story by saying show me or tell me what happens next The information derived from the interview is later coded according to the organization and content of the story completion Avoidant attachment for example can be disclosed by a child refusing to acknowledge the attachment issue presented in the story stem through claiming that the event did not take place A child may also avoid addressing attachment by focusing solely on minor details such as how the protagonist is dressed Secure attachment alternately is indicated when a child provides coherent and constructive resolutions to the stories 33 Attachment Doll Play Assessment ADPA edit The ADPA was developed by Judith Solomon and Carol George Solomon et al 1995 34 as a variation of the ACST The measure unlike some of the doll play measures was validated from concurrent mother child Strange Situation patterns at ages 5 7 years The assessment identified five attachment groups secure avoidant ambivalent and the punitive and caregiving patterns of middle childhood disorganization The ADPA has two unique features not found in other doll play assessments One is the instruction to the child to select their own family as compared with other assessments that create a standard family set of dolls This variation was found important for children in the D group because they frequently do not select a mother figure or they select an adult doll to represent the self Another is a coding system that is based on defensive processes rather than content or rating scales The focus on defense is unique to Solomon and George s work and served as the foundation for assessing defenses in the Adult Attachment Projective Picture adult attachment assessment The ADPA has been used cross culturally in Japan and with French speakers in Canada Manchester Child Attachment Story Task MCAST edit The MCAST 35 is a semi structured doll play story stem methodology developed by Jonathan Green Charlie Stanley Ruth Goldwyn and Vicky Smith 36 37 to evaluate and understand the internal mental representations of their attachment relationship with a specific primary caregiver in children of 4 to 8 5 years The concepts and procedures used have a basis in the Strange Situation Procedure and Adult Attachment Interview and involves 4 story stem vignettes involving two dolls representing the caregiver child dyad of interest and a dolls house presented with affective arousal to mobilise attachment representations in a way that children of this age range find accessible and engaging Responses are usually videotaped in order to reliably rate aspects of the child s represented narrative content and behaviour and the child s own behaviour to ascertain an attachment classification with a particular focus on disorganised attachment as well as providing other supporting ratings Clinical development of the MCAST started in 1992 validation was published in 2000 and it has been since used in a range of cultural contexts 38 and clinical and at risk groups 39 Training is required for its use Picture response techniques edit Like the stem stories these techniques are designed to access the child s internal working models of attachment relationships The child is shown attachment related pictures and asked to respond Methods include the Separation Anxiety Test SAT developed in 1972 for children aged between 11 and 17 Revised versions have been produced for 4 7 year olds The SAT was doctored Interview methods edit Child Attachment Interview CAI edit This is a semi structured interview designed by Target et al 2003 for children aged 7 to 11 It is based on the Adult Attachment Interview adapted for children by focusing on representations of relationships with parents and attachment related events Scores are based on both verbal and non verbal communications 40 Disturbances of Attachment Interview DAI edit Disturbances of Attachment Interview developed by Smyke and Zeanah 1999 is a semi structured interview designed to be administered by clinicians to caregivers This method is designed to pick up not only reactive attachment disorder but also Zeannah et al s 1993 suggested new alternative categories of disorders of attachment 41 It covers 12 items namely having a discriminated preferred adult seeking comfort when distressed responding to comfort when offered social and emotional reciprocity emotional regulation checking back after venturing away from the care giver reticence with unfamiliar adults willingness to go off with relative strangers self endangering behavior excessive clinging vigilance hypercompliance and role reversal Measures of attachment in adolescents editAttachment Interview for Childhood and Adolescence AICA edit This is a version of the Adult Attachment Interview AAI rendered age appropriate for adolescents The classifications of dismissing secure preoccupied and unresolved are the same as under the AAI described below Measures in adults editMain articles Attachment in adults and Attachment theory The three main ways of measuring attachment in adults include the Adult Attachment Interview AAI the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System AAP and self report questionnaires The AAI and AAP are based on a developmental perspective while the self report questionnaires are based on a social psychology perspective Each assessment was created with somewhat different aims in mind Shaver and Fraley coming from the social psychology perspective note If you are a novice in this research area what is most important for you to know is that self report measures of romantic attachment and the AAI were initially developed completely independently and for quite different purposes One asks about a person s feelings and behaviors in the context of romantic or other close relationships the other is used to make inferences about the defenses associated with an adult s current state of mind regarding childhood relationships with parents In principle these might have been substantially associated but in fact they seem to be only moderately related at least as currently assessed One kind of measure receives its construct validity mostly from studies of romantic relationships the other from prediction of a person s child s behavior in Ainsworth s Strange Situation Correlations of the two kinds of measures with other variables are likely to differ although a few studies have found the AAI to be related to marital relationship quality and a few have found self report romantic attachment measures to be related to parenting Shaver amp Fraley 2004 42 The AAI the AAP and the self report questionnaires offer distinct but equally useful perspectives on adult attachment 2 467 Adult Attachment Interview AAI edit Developed by Carol George Nancy Kaplan and Mary Main in 1984 43 this is a quasi clinical semi structured interview that takes about one hour to administer It utilizes either the ABC D or Berkeley or DMM model for discourse analysis 44 and originally involved about twenty questions It has since had extensive research validation to support it The AAI Berkeley model is discussed in Chapter 25 of Attachment Theory Research and Clinical Applications 2nd ed edited by J Cassidy and P R Shaver Guilford Press NY 2008 The chapter title is The Adult Attachment Interview Historical and Current Perspectives and is written by E Hesse The interview taps into adult representation of attachment i e internal working models by assessing general and specific recollections from their childhood The interview is coded based on quality of discourse especially coherence and content Categories are designed to predict parental stances on Berkeley infant data AAI Attachment status using the Berkeley or ABC D model originally utilized Ainsworth s patterns A1 and A2 B1 5 and C1 and C2 and also disorganization Disorganization was initially considered a pattern then seen as a status and then determined to be not usable as a concept for adults or children over 20 months 45 44 The Berkeley model developed to include over 20 patterns 46 Four broad categories have included Autonomous They value attachment relationships describe them in a balanced way and as influential Their discourse is coherent internally consistent and non defensive in nature Dismissing They show memory lapses Minimize negative aspects and deny personal impact on relationships Their positive descriptions are often contradicted or unsupported The discourse is defensive Preoccupied Experience continuing preoccupation with their own parents Incoherent discourse Have angry or ambivalent representations of the past Unresolved Disorganized Show trauma resulting from unresolved loss or abuse Some of the strongest external validation of the measures involves its demonstrated ability to predict interviewees children s classifications in the Strange Situation The measure also has been shown to have some overlap with attachment constructs measured by the less time intensive measures of the peer romantic attachment tradition Hazan amp Shaver Bartholomew as reported by Shaver P R Belsky J amp Brennan K A 2000 47 However there are important differences in what is measured by the AAI using the Berkeley model rather than being a measure of romantic attachment it taps primarily into a person s state of mind regarding their attachment in their family of origin The DMM AAI uses a slightly modified set of interview questions and uses principles and analysis techniques from attachment theory as described by Patricia Crittenden and the Dynamic Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation DMM The DMM method is more comprehensive assessing more self protective attachment strategies more types of trauma and more types of memory systems 46 48 Both the DMM AAI and Berkeley AAI methods assess information processing memory system use reflective integration and are able to identify issues such as unresolved trauma and depression The DMM AAI method can identify additional issues such as intrusions of negative affect disorientation and reorganizing or reorganized attachment strategies Assessing adult attachment A dynamic maturational approach to discourse analysis 2011 48 is the coding manual for the DMM AAI This manual is published and available at any bookseller and provides a comprehensive and detailed description of the AAI process DMM attachment theory and the attachment system s impact on aspects of humanity such as patterns of information processing memory system function and the effect of trauma It also offers a brief comparison of the two attachment models using the AAI The Berkeley ABC D model has never published a coding manual or comprehensive model of attachment 44 The DMM model of attachment also expands the range of non B patterns beyond Ainsworth s original patterns of A1 A2 B1 5 and C1 C2 patterns It identifies the additional patterns of A3 8 and C3 8 It also describes how A and C patterns can be combined by individuals such as A4 C5 6 49 48 It replaces the disorganized category with finer descriptions of the A and C attachment patterns The DMM also uses a different naming scheme for the general and specific patterns Adult Attachment Projective Picture System AAP edit Developed by Carol George and Malcolm West in 1999 this is a free response task that involves telling stories in response to eight picture stimuli 1 warm up amp 7 attachment scenes The advantage of using a picture free response system is that individuals are not asked to describe their own experiences a method that has been shown to be subject to social desirability and defensive processes especially for assessing attachment trauma This method is similar to the School Age Assessment of Attachment SAA below George and colleagues have described the AAP assessment in articles and books 50 51 and in a 2023 book how to implement the AAP in clinical practice for assessment client conceptualization treatment planning analysis and as a therapeutic guide 52 The AAP can identify three organized patterns of attachment secure insecure avoidant or deactivating defenses insecure preoccupied ambivalent or cognitive disconnection and also insecure unresolved disorganized attachment 53 54 The AAP identifies the same adult attachment groups as the early version of the AAI Berkeley model as described above In addition to providing adult group classifications the AAP is also used to code individual attachment defensive information processing patterns experiences of attachment trauma attachment synchrony and personal agency 53 The strongest concurrent validation of the measure is the correspondence between AAP and AAI classification agreement The AAP is demonstrated to be increasingly useful in clinical and neurobiological settings 55 Patient Attachment Coding System PACS edit Developed by Alessandro Talia and Madeleine Miller Bottome in 2012 it is a language based observational measure of attachment used in the context of psychotherapy described in Talia Miller Bottome amp Daniel 2017 in the journal Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 56 and other publications 57 58 The PACS has been defined as leading to a paradigm shift in attachment informed research and clinical practice 59 Rather than coding the form or content of patients discourse the PACS tracks the frequency of specific discourse markers that reflect how the patient regulates trust and connection with the therapist In this way the PACS contributes to our knowledge of specific types of communication and behaviors that distinguish patients of different attachment patterns in psychotherapy Secure They tell clear well structured narratives that are easy to visualize they describe relationships and experiences together with elaborating upon the impact they had on them they offer interpretations about others and their own behavior with tentativeness and curiosity Avoidant They downplay either their appraisal of relationships or the effect these had on them they are not likely to recount episodes disclose feelings or reflect on inner determinants of experiences Preoccupied They present narratives that are unclear full of irrelevant details they describe relationships and experiences with either excessive certainty or excessive vagueness and lack of a clear stance The PACS identifies the same adult attachment groups as the AAI or the AAP as well as yielding dimensional scores for linguistic behaviors such as Proximity seeking Contact Maintaining Exploring Avoidance Resistance The strongest concurrent validation of the measure is the correspondence with the AAI k 82 and the Reflective Functioning Scale r 72 60 The PACS is increasingly used in clinical settings Social psychology Self report questionnaires edit Hazan and Shaver created the first questionnaire to measure attachment in adults 61 Their questionnaire was designed to classify adults into the three attachment styles identified by Ainsworth The questionnaire consisted of three sets of statements each set of statements describing an attachment style Secure I find it relatively easy to get close to others and am comfortable depending on them and having them depend on me I don t often worry about being abandoned or about someone getting too close to me Avoidant I am somewhat uncomfortable being close to others I find it difficult to trust them completely difficult to allow myself to depend on them I am nervous when anyone gets too close and often love partners want me to be more intimate than I feel comfortable being Anxious Ambivalent I find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like I often worry that my partner doesn t really love me or won t want to stay with me I want to merge completely with another person and this desire sometimes scares people away People participating in their study were asked to choose which set of statements best described their feelings The chosen set of statements indicated their attachment style Later versions of this questionnaire presented scales so people could rate how well each set of statements described their feelings One important advance in the development of attachment questionnaires was the addition of a fourth style of attachment Bartholomew and Horowitz presented a model that identified four categories or styles of adult attachment 62 Their model was based on the idea attachment styles reflected people s thoughts about their partners and thought about themselves Specifically attachment styles depended on whether or not people judge their partners to be generally accessible and responsive to requests for support and whether or not people judge themselves to be the kind of individuals towards which others want to respond and lend help They proposed four categories based on positive or negative thoughts about partners and on positive or negative thoughts about self Attachment Theory Four Category Model Thoughts of Self Positive NegativeThoughtsofPartner Positive Secure Comfortable with intimacy and autonomy Preoccupied Preoccupied with relationshipsNegative Dismissive Dismissing of intimacy Strongly independent Fearful Fearful of intimacy Socially avoidantBartholomew and Horowitz used this model to create the Relationship Questionnaire RQ CV The RQ CV consisted of four sets of statements each describing a category or style of attachment Secure It is relatively easy for me to become emotionally close to others I am comfortable depending on others and having others depend on me I don t worry about being alone or having others not accept me Dismissive I am comfortable without close emotional relationships It is very important to me to feel independent and self sufficient and I prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me Preoccupied I want to be completely emotionally intimate with others but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like I am uncomfortable being without close relationships but I sometimes worry that others don t value me as much as I value them Fearful I am somewhat uncomfortable getting close to others I want emotionally close relationships but I find it difficult to trust others completely or to depend on them I sometimes worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others Tests demonstrated the four attachment styles were distinct in how they related to other kinds of psychological variables Adults indeed appeared to have four styles of attachment instead of three attachment styles David Schmitt together with a large number of colleagues validated the attachment questionnaire created by Bartholomew and Horowitz in 62 cultures 63 The distinction of thoughts about self and thoughts about partners proved valid in nearly all cultures However the way these two kinds of thoughts interacted to form attachment styles varied somewhat across cultures The four attachment styles had somewhat different meanings across cultures A second important advance in attachment questionnaires was the use of independent items to assess attachment Instead of asking people to choose between three or four sets of statements people rated how strongly they agreed with dozens of individual statements The ratings for the individual statements were combined to provide an attachment score Investigators have created several questionnaires using this strategy to measure adult attachment Two popular questionnaires of this type are the Experiences in Close Relationships ECR questionnaire and the Experiences in Close Relationships Revised ECR R questionnaire The ECR was created by Brennan Clark and Shaver in 1998 64 The ECR R was created by Fraley Waller and Brennan in 2000 65 Analysis of the ECR and ECR R reveal that the questionnaire items can be grouped into two dimensions of attachment One group of questionnaire items deal with how anxious a person is about their relationship These items serve as a scale for anxiety The remaining items deal with how avoidant a person is in their relationship These items serve as a scale for avoidance Many researchers now use scores from the anxiety and avoidance scales to perform statistical analyses and test hypotheses Scores on the anxiety and avoidance scales can still be used to classify people into the four adult attachment styles 64 66 67 The four styles of attachment defined in Bartholomew and Horowitz s model were based on thoughts about self and thoughts about partners The anxiety scale in the ECR and ECR R reflect thoughts about self Attachment anxiety relates to beliefs about self worth and whether or not one will be accepted or rejected by others The avoidance scale in the ECR and ECR R relates to thoughts about partners Attachment avoidance relates to beliefs about taking risks in approaching or avoiding other people Combinations of anxiety and avoidance can thus be used to define the four attachment styles The secure style of attachment is characterized by low anxiety and low avoidance the preoccupied style of attachment is characterized by high anxiety and low avoidance the dismissive avoidant style of attachment is characterized by low anxiety and high avoidance and the fearful avoidant style of attachment is characterized by high anxiety and high avoidance nbsp Two dimensional model of adult attachment related to the four styles of adult attachment Assessments using DMM theory and method editMain article Dynamic maturational model of attachment and adaptation DMM informed assessments offer a full lifespan set of assessments many adapted from existing attachment assessments Some assessments are validated and some are still in development or the validation process Infant CARE Index ICI edit This validated assessment described above primarily assess interaction rather than attachment in a 3 minute observation 68 69 70 71 Toddler CARE Index TCI edit The TCI is undergoing a 15 year validation process It assess the general attachment characteristics of a child 15 72 months old The assessment is a shortened version of the Strange Situation Procedure and involves a 5 minute video recorded play interaction between a child and caregiver with a frustration task and post frustration repair attempt 72 18 DMM Strange Situation Procedure SSP edit As described above but the DMM version covers a more narrow age range of 11 15 months 15 Preschool Assessment of Attachment PAA edit As described above this validated assessment is a slightly modified version of the SSP to cover a wider range of ages 2 5 years old 73 74 School Age Assessment of Attachment SAA edit Similar to the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System above and the CAPA below it invites a free response task from a child 6 13 years old after seeing a picture stimuli It uses a DMM discourse analysis technique to assess attachment 75 76 The SAA was the subject of a special section of Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry in July 2017 and is a validated assessment 77 Meaning of the Child interview MotC edit The MotC uses an interview and discourse technique similar to the AAI It assesses a parent s general pattern of caregiving sensitivity and responsiveness to their child 78 79 Child Attachment and Play Assessment CAPA edit This assesses the attachment and exploration systems for children 7 11 It uses a process similar to the School Age Assessment of Attachment and Adult Attachment Projective Picture System above 80 Transition to Adulthood Attachment Interview TAAI edit This assessment is undergoing a development and validation process It assess attachment patterns in people 14 25 years old and is a version of the AAI modified to be age and development appropriate 81 82 DMM Adult Attachment Interview AAI edit As described above this well validated assessment considers many aspects of the adult attachment system 48 Criticism editExisting measures have not necessarily been developed to a useful level Behavioral observation is a natural starting point for assessing attachment disorders because behavioral descriptions have been central to the development of the concept despite the fact that observations have figured prominently no established observational protocol has been established 83 Also questionable measures of attachment in school age children have been presented For example a protocol for establishing attachment status was described by Sheperis and his colleagues 84 Unfortunately this protocol was validated against another technique the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire that was itself poorly validated and that is based on a nonconventional view of attachment Reception and development of SSP edit Psychiatrist Michael Rutter described relatively early in the development and use of the SSP some limitations of the procedure in the following terms 85 nbsp Mother and child It is by no means free of limitations see Lamb Thompson Gardener Charnov amp Estes 1984 86 To begin with it is very dependent on brief separations and reunions having the same meaning for all children This may be a major constraint when applying the procedure in cultures such as that in Japan see Miyake et al 1985 87 where infants are rarely separated from their mothers in ordinary circumstances Also because older children have a cognitive capacity to maintain relationships when the older person is not present separation may not provide the same stress for them Modified procedures based on the Strange Situation have been developed for older preschool children see Belsky et al 1994 Greenberg et al 1990 88 89 but it is much more dubious whether the same approach can be used in middle childhood Also despite its manifest strengths the procedure is based on just 20 minutes of behaviour It can be scarcely expected to tap all the relevant qualities of a child s attachment relationships Q sort procedures based on much longer naturalistic observations in the home and interviews with the mothers have developed in order to extend the data base see Vaughn amp Waters 1990 90 A further constraint is that the coding procedure results in discrete categories rather than continuously distributed dimensions Not only is this likely to provide boundary problems but also it is not at all obvious that discrete categories best represent the concepts that are inherent in attachment security It seems much more likely that infants vary in their degree of security and there is need for a measurement systems that can quantify individual variation Subsequent research has confirmed the SSP and the DMM PAA version have value across cultures and may pick up cultural differences 91 and the age range for which each is useful has been determined 92 Attachment behaviors have been identified in nonhuman primates 91 and dogs 93 The 21 minute procedure provides a massive amount of data and Crittenden s TCI a 5 minute version of the PAA is a useful assessment As with all other attachment assessments these three are necessarily limited in what they can discover and the information they can discover is powerful 2 The DMM model is multi dimensional in several ways 48 Infant attachment assessments have been validated 18 Ecological validity and universality of Strange Situation attachment classification distributions edit With respect to the ecological validity of the Strange Situation a meta analysis of 2 000 infant parent dyads including several from studies with non Western language and or cultural bases found the global distribution of attachment categorizations to be A 21 B 65 and C 14 94 This global distribution was generally consistent with Ainsworth et al s 1978 original attachment classification distributions However controversy has been raised over a few cultural differences in these rates of global attachment classification distributions In particular two studies diverged from the global distributions of attachment classifications noted above One study was conducted in North Germany 95 in which more avoidant A infants were found than global norms would suggest and the other in Sapporo Japan 96 where more resistant C infants were found Of these two studies the Japanese findings have sparked the most controversy as to the meaning of individual differences in attachment behavior as originally identified by Ainsworth et al 1978 In a recent study conducted in Sapporo Behrens et al 2007 97 found attachment distributions consistent with global norms using the six year Main amp Cassidy scoring system for attachment classification 98 In addition to these findings supporting the global distributions of attachment classifications in Sapporo Behrens et al also discuss the Japanese concept of amae and its relevance to questions concerning whether the insecure resistant C style of interaction may be engendered in Japanese infants as a result of the cultural practice of amae Attachment measurement discrete or continuous edit Regarding the issue of whether the breadth of infant attachment functioning can be captured by a categorical classification scheme continuous measures of attachment security have been developed which have demonstrated adequate psychometric properties These have been used either individually or in conjunction with discrete attachment classifications in many published reports 99 100 The original Richter s et al 1998 scale is strongly related to secure versus insecure classifications correctly predicting about 90 of cases 100 Readers further interested in the categorical versus continuous nature of attachment classifications and the debate surrounding this issue should consult the paper by Fraley and Spieker 101 and the rejoinders in the same issue by many prominent attachment researchers including J Cassidy A Sroufe E Waters amp T Beauchaine and M Cummings See also editAffectional bond Attachment theory Attachment in adults Attachment in children Attachment disorder Attachment therapy Human bonding Object relations theory Reactive attachment disorder Mary Ainsworth John Bowlby Erik Erikson Sigmund Freud Jerome Kagan Melanie Klein Jean PiagetReferences edit Ainsworth Mary D Blehar Mary C Waters Everett Wall Sally 1978 Patterns of attachment A psychological study of the strange situation 1st ed Lawrence Erlbaum original Routledge Classic Editions 2015 ISBN 9781848726826 a b c d e f Duschinsky Robbie 2020 Cornerstones of attachment research Oxford New York NY Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 884206 4 George Carol Solomon Judith 2008 Cassidy Jude Shaver Phillip R eds The caregiving system A behavioral systems approach to parenting 2nd ed Guilford Press pp 833 856 ISBN 9781606235843 Granqvist Pehr Sroufe L Alan Dozier Mary Hesse Erik Steele Miriam van Ijzendoorn Marinus Solomon Judith Main Mary Fearon Pasco Bakermans Kranenburg Marian Steele Howard Cassidy Jude Carlson Elizabeth Madigan Sheri Duschinsky Robbie 2017 11 02 Disorganized attachment in infancy a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy makers Attachment amp Human Development 19 6 534 558 doi 10 1080 14616734 2017 1354040 ISSN 1461 6734 PMC 5600694 PMID 28745146 Ainsworth Mary D 1978 Patterns of Attachment A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation Lawrence Erlbaum Associates ISBN 0 89859 461 8 Main M and Solomon J 1986 Discovery of an insecure disorganized dioriented attachment pattern procedures findings and implications for the classification of behavior In t Braxelton and M Yogman eds Affective development in infancy Norwood NJ Ablex Main m and Solomon J 1990 Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation In M Greenberg D Cicchetti and E Cummings eds Attachment in the preschool years Theory research and intervention Chicago University of Chicago Press Bell S 1970 The development of the concept of the object as related to infant mother attachment Child Development 41 2 291 311 doi 10 2307 1127033 JSTOR 1127033 Crittenden P 1985 Social networks quality of parenting and child development Child Development 56 5 1299 1313 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 1985 tb00198 x Radke Yarrow M Cummings E M Kuczynski L Chapman M 1985 Patterns of attachment in two and three year olds in normal families and families with parental depression Child Development 56 4 884 893 doi 10 2307 1130100 JSTOR 1130100 PMID 4042751 Main M Cassidy J 1988 Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age six predictability from infant attachment classifications stable across a one month period Developmental Psychology 24 3 415 426 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 24 3 415 a b Crittenden P M 1992 Quality of attachment in the preschool years Development and Psychopathology 4 2 209 241 doi 10 1017 s0954579400000110 S2CID 143894461 Shah P E Fonagy P Strathearn L 2010 Exploring the mechanism of intergenerational transmission of attachment The plot thickens Clinical Child Psychology amp Psychiatry 15 3 329 346 doi 10 1177 1359104510365449 PMC 3060612 PMID 20603421 Snyder R Shapiro S Treleaven D 2012 Attachment Theory and Mindfulness Journal of Child amp Family Studies 21 5 709 717 doi 10 1007 s10826 011 9522 8 S2CID 56009083 a b Farnfield Steve Hautamaki Airi Norbech Peder Sahhar Nicola 2010 DMM assessments of attachment and adaptation Procedures validity and utility Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 15 3 313 328 doi 10 1177 1359104510364315 ISSN 1359 1045 PMID 20603420 S2CID 206707459 Ostler Teresa 2010 Assessing parenting risk within the context of severe and persistent mental illness Validating an observational measure for families with child protective service involvement Infant Mental Health Journal 31 5 467 485 doi 10 1002 imhj 20267 ISSN 0163 9641 PMID 28543839 Simo S Rauh H amp Ziegenhain U 2000 Mother infant interaction during the first eighteen months and attachment security at the end of the second year Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht 47 2 118 141 a b c Tryphonopoulos Panagiota D Letourneau Nicole Ditommaso Enrico 2014 09 12 Attachment and caregiver infant interaction of observational assessment tools Infant Mental Health Journal 35 6 642 656 doi 10 1002 imhj 21461 ISSN 0163 9641 PMID 25798513 Svanberg P O Mennet Lisa Spieker Susan 2010 Promoting a secure attachment A primary prevention practice model Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 15 3 363 378 doi 10 1177 1359104510367584 ISSN 1359 1045 PMID 20603424 S2CID 206707508 Andreassen C amp West J 2007 Measuring socioemotional functioning in a national birth cohort study Infant Mental Health Journal 28 6 627 646 Lieberman A F 1977 Preschoolers competence with a peer Relations with attachment and peer experience Child Development 48 4 1277 1287 doi 10 2307 1128485 JSTOR 1128485 a b Greenberg M T Cicchetti D Cummings E M 1993 Attachment in the preschool years Theory research and intervention University of Chicago Press Solomon J George C Handbook of attachment Theory research and clinical applications New York Guilford Press pp 287 316 Main M amp Cassidy J 1988 Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6 predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1 month period Developmental Psychology 24 415 426 Crittenden PM 1992 Quality of attachment in the preschool years Development and Psychopathology 4 2 209 41 doi 10 1017 S0954579400000110 S2CID 143894461 Archived from the original on 2008 03 14 Retrieved 2008 01 06 Teti Douglas M Gelfand Donna M 1997 The Preschool Assessment of Attachment Construct validity in a sample of depressed and nondepressed families Development and Psychopathology 9 3 517 536 doi 10 1017 s0954579497001284 ISSN 0954 5794 PMID 9327237 S2CID 42553006 Crittenden Patricia McKinsey Claussen Angelika Hartl Kozlowska Kasia 2007 Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use A Comparative Study Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy 28 2 78 87 doi 10 1375 anft 28 2 78 ISSN 0814 723X MIM Research a b Waters E and Deane K 1985 Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships Q methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood In I Bretherton and E Waters eds Growing pains of attachment theory and research Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50 Serial No 209 1 2 41 65 Vaughn B E Waters E 1990 Attachment behavior at home and in the laboratory Q sort observations and strange situation classifications of one year olds Child Development 61 6 1965 1973 doi 10 2307 1130850 JSTOR 1130850 PMID 2083508 Clements M Barnett D 2002 Parenting and attachment among toddlers with congenital anomalies Examining the strange situation and attachment Q sort Infant Mental Health Journal 23 6 625 642 doi 10 1002 imhj 10040 Goodman G Pfeffer CR 1998 Attachment disorganization in prepubertal children with severe emotional disturbance Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 62 4 490 525 PMID 9810111 Solomon J George C Handbook of attachment Theory research and clinical applications New York Guilford Press pp 102 123 Solomon J George C amp De Jong A 1995 Children classified as controlling at age six Evidence of disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and school Development and Psychopathology 7 447 464 MCAST University of Manchester Green J M Stanley C Smith V amp Goldwyn R 2000 A new method of evaluating attachment representations on young school age children the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task Attachment and Human Development 2 42 64 Goldwyn R Green J M Stanley C amp Smith V 2000 The Manchester Child Attachment Story Task Relationship with parental AAI SAT and child behaviour Attachment and Human Development 2 65 78 Barone L Del Guidice M Fossati A Manaresi F Actis Perinitti B Colle L amp Veglia F 2009 Psychometric Properties of the Manchester Child Attachment Story Task an Italian multicentre study International Journal of Behavioural Development 33 2 185 190 Wan M W amp Green J 2010 Negative and atypical story content themes depicted by children with behaviour problems Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51 1125 1131 Target M Fonagy P and Schmueli Goetz Y 2003 Attachment representations in school age children the development of the Child Attachment Interview CAI Journal of Child Psychotherapy 29 2 171 186 Smyke A and Zeanah C 1999 Disturbances of Attachment Interview Available on the Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry website at www jaacap com Shaver P A Fraley R C 2004 Self report measures of adult attachment Retrieved June 20 2006 George Carol Kaplan Nancy Main Mary 1996 Adult Attachment Interview PDF Department of Psychology Unpublished Manuscript Third ed University of California Berkeley a b c Duschinsky Robbie 2020 Cornerstones of attachment research Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 884206 4 OCLC 1154774522 Granqvist Pehr Sroufe L Alan Main Mary Hesse Erik Steele Miriam van Ijzendoorn Marinus Solomon Judith Schuengel Carlo Fearon Pasco Bakermans Kranenburg Marian and 33 other authors 2017 07 26 Disorganized attachment in infancy a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy makers Attachment amp Human Development 19 6 534 558 doi 10 1080 14616734 2017 1354040 ISSN 1461 6734 PMC 5600694 PMID 28745146 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint numeric names authors list link a b Baldoni Franco Minghetti Mattia Craparo Giuseppe Facondini Elisa Cena Loredana Schimmenti Adriano 2018 07 04 Comparing Main Goldwyn and Hesse Berkeley and Crittenden DMM coding systems for classifying Adult Attachment Interview transcripts an empirical report Attachment amp Human Development 20 4 423 438 doi 10 1080 14616734 2017 1421979 hdl 11379 501534 ISSN 1461 6734 PMID 29308700 S2CID 205815474 Shaver P R Belsky J amp Brennan K A 2000 The adult attachment interview and self reports of romantic attachment Associations across domains and methods Personal Relationships 7 25 43 a b c d e Crittenden Patricia McKinsey Landini Andrea 2011 Assessing adult attachment a dynamic maturational approach to discourse analysis New York W W Norton amp Co ISBN 978 0 393 70667 3 OCLC 667877268 Crittenden Patricia M Newman Louise 2010 Comparing models of borderline personality disorder Mothers experience self protective strategies and dispositional representations Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 15 3 433 451 doi 10 1177 1359104510368209 ISSN 1359 1045 George Carol West Malcolm L 2001 The development and preliminary validation of a new measure of adult attachment the Adult Attachment Projective Attachment amp Human Development 3 1 30 61 doi 10 1080 14616730010024771 ISSN 1461 6734 George Carol West Malcolm L eds 2012 The Adult Attachment Projective Picture System Attachment theory and assessment in adults Illustrated ed The Guilford Press ISBN 978 1462504251 George Carol Wargo Aikens Julie Lehmann Melissa eds 2023 Working with Attachment Trauma Clinical Application of the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System New York Routledge ISBN 978 1 003 21543 1 a b Gander Manuela George Carol Pokorny Dan Buchheim Anna 2016 03 26 Assessing Attachment Representations in Adolescents Discriminant Validation of the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System Child Psychiatry amp Human Development 48 2 270 282 doi 10 1007 s10578 016 0639 2 ISSN 0009 398X PMC 5323491 PMID 27016937 Solomon Judith George Carol eds 2011 Disorganized attachment and caregiving Guilford Press ISBN 978 1 60918 128 4 OCLC 912464422 Buchheim Anna Erk Susanne George Carol Kachele Horst Kircher Tilo Martius Philipp Pokorny Dan Ruchsow Martin Spitzer Manfred Walter Henrik August 2008 Neural correlates of attachment trauma in borderline personality disorder A functional magnetic resonance imaging study Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging 163 3 223 235 doi 10 1016 j pscychresns 2007 07 001 ISSN 0925 4927 PMID 18635342 S2CID 5026031 Talia A Miller Bottome M Daniel S I F 2017 Assessing attachment in psychotherapy Validation study of the Patient Attachment Coding System PACS Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 24 1 149 161 Talia A Miller Bottome M Katznelson H Pedersen S H Steele H Schroder P amp Lingiardi V 2019 Mentalizing in the presence of another Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process Psychotherapy Research 29 5 652 665 Talia A Miller Bottome M Wyner R Lilliengren P amp Bate J 2019 Patients Adult Attachment Interview classification and their experience of the therapeutic relationship are they associated Research in Psychotherapy Psychopathology Process and Outcome 22 2 Slade A 2016 Attachment and Adult Psychotherapy Theory Research and Practice In Cassidy J amp Shaver P Eds Handbook of Attachment Third edition Talia A Miller Bottome M Katznelson H Pedersen S H Steele H Schroder P amp Taubner S 2019 Mentalizing in the presence of another Measuring reflective functioning and attachment in the therapy process Psychotherapy Research 29 5 652 665 Hazan C amp Shaver P 1987 Romantic love conceptualized as an attachmenpt process Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 511 524 Bartholomew K amp Horowitz L M 1991 Attachment styles among young adults A test of a four category model Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61 226 244 Schmitt D P et al 2004 Patterns and universals of adult romantic attachment across 62 cultural regions Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 35 367 402 a b Brennan K A Clark C L amp Shaver P R 1998 Self report measurement of adult romantic attachment An integrative overview In J A Simpson amp W S Rholes Eds Attachment theory and close relationships pp 46 76 New York Guilford Press Fraley R C Waller N G amp Brennan K A 2000 An item response theory analysis of self report measures of adult attachment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 350 365 Bartholomew K amp Shaver P R 1998 Methods of assessing adult attachment In J A Simpson amp W S Rholes Eds Attachment theory and close relationships pp 25 45 New York NY Guilford Press Collins N L amp Freeney B C 2004 An Attachment Theory Perspective on Closeness and Intimacy In D J Mashek amp A Aron Eds Handbook of Closeness and Intimacy pp 163 188 Mahwah NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Crittenden Patricia M DiLalla David L 1988 Compulsive compliance The development of an inhibitory coping strategy in infancy Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 16 5 585 599 doi 10 1007 bf00914268 ISSN 0091 0627 PMID 3235749 S2CID 30494458 Hautamaki Airi 2014 Mothers and infants Screening for maternal relationships at risk with the CARE index In Farnfield Steve Holmes Paul eds The Routledge handbook of attachment assessment London New York Routledge pp 37 52 ISBN 978 0 415 53825 1 OCLC 841895216 Forcada Guex Margarita Pierrehumbert Blaise Borghini Ayala Moessinger Adrien Muller Nix Carole 2006 07 01 Early Dyadic Patterns of Mother Infant Interactions and Outcomes of Prematurity at 18 Months Pediatrics 118 1 e107 e114 doi 10 1542 peds 2005 1145 ISSN 0031 4005 PMID 16818525 S2CID 9258568 Parfitt Ylva Pike Alison Ayers Susan 2013 11 21 Infant Developmental Outcomes A Family Systems Perspective Infant and Child Development 23 4 353 373 doi 10 1002 icd 1830 ISSN 1522 7227 Crittenden Patricia M 2014 Toddler CARE Index TCI Manual unpublished training manual Miami FL Family Relations Institute Crittenden Patricia McKinsey Claussen Angelika Hartl Kozlowska Kasia 2007 Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use A Comparative Study Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy 28 2 78 87 doi 10 1375 anft 28 2 78 ISSN 0814 723X The Routledge handbook of attachment assessment Steve Farnfield Paul Holmes London 2014 ISBN 978 0 415 53824 4 OCLC 841895216 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link CS1 maint others link Crittenden Patricia Kozlowska Kasia Landini Andrea 2010 02 22 Assessing attachment in school age children Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 15 2 185 208 doi 10 1177 1359104509356741 ISSN 1359 1045 PMID 20176770 S2CID 11582111 Crittenden Patricia M Robson Katrina Tooby Alison Fleming Charles 2017 04 21 Are mothers protective attachment strategies related to their children s strategies Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 22 3 358 377 doi 10 1177 1359104517704027 ISSN 1359 1045 PMID 28429614 S2CID 4824676 Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry Volume 22 Number 3 Jul 01 2017 journals sagepub com Grey Ben Farnfield Steve 2016 03 02 The Meaning of the Child Interview A new procedure for assessing and understanding parent child relationships of at risk families Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 22 2 204 218 doi 10 1177 1359104516633495 ISSN 1359 1045 PMID 26940120 S2CID 206708220 Grey Ben Farnfield Steve 2017 03 20 The Meaning of the Child Interview MotC the initial validation of a new procedure for assessing and understanding the parent child relationships of at risk families Journal of Children s Services 12 1 16 31 doi 10 1108 jcs 03 2016 0006 ISSN 1746 6660 Farnfield Steve 2016 The Child Attachment and Play Assessment CAPA Validation of a new approach to coding narrative stems with children ages 3 11 years International Journal of Play Therapy 25 4 217 229 doi 10 1037 a0038726 ISSN 1939 0629 Crittenden Patricia M 2005 Transition to Adulthood Attachment Interview TAAI unpublished training manual Miami FL Family Relations Institute Stokowy Martin Sahhar Nicola eds 2012 5 Adoleszenz und das TAAI Landini Kozlowska Davies Chudleigh Bindung und Gefahr Psychosozial Verlag pp 113 140 doi 10 30820 9783837966534 113 ISBN 9783837966534 retrieved 2022 01 15 O Connor T amp Zeanah C H 2003 Attachment disorders Assessment strategies and treatment approaches Attachment amp Human Development 5 3 223 244 p 229 Sheperis C J Doggett R A Hoda N E Blanchard T Renfro Michael E L Holdiness S H amp Schlagheck R 2003 The development of an assessment protocol for Reactive Attachment Disorder Journal of Mental Health Counseling 25 4 291 310 Rutter Michael 1995 The Clinical Implications of Attachment Concepts Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 36 4 552 553 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 1995 tb02314 x PMID 7650083 Lamb Thompson Gardener Charnov amp Estes 1984 Security of Infantile attachment as assessed in the Strange Situation its study and biological interpretations Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 127 147 Miyake Chen amp Campos 1985 Infant temperament and mother s mode of interaction and attachment in Japan an interim report In I Bretherton amp E Waters Eds Growing points of attachment theory and research Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50 Serial No 209 276 297 Belsky J amp Cassidy J 1994 Attachment Theory and Evidence In M Rutter amp D Hay Eds Development Through Life A Handbook For Clinicians pp 373 402 Oxford Blackwell Scientific Publications Greenberg M T Cicchetti D amp Cummings M Eds 1990 Attachment in the preschool years theory research and intervention Chicago University of Chicago Press Vaughn B E amp Waters E 1990 Attachment behaviour at home and in the laboratory Child Development 61 1965 1973 a b Cassidy Jude Shaver Phillip Robert 2018 Handbook of attachment theory research and clinical applications 3rd ed New York N Y Guilford Press ISBN 978 1 4625 3664 1 Crittenden Patricia McKinsey Claussen Angelika Hartl Kozlowska Kasia 2007 Choosing a Valid Assessment of Attachment for Clinical Use A Comparative Study Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy 28 2 78 87 doi 10 1375 anft 28 2 78 ISSN 0814 723X Solomon J Beetz A Schoberl I Gee N Kotrschal K 2019 07 04 Attachment security in companion dogs adaptation of Ainsworth s strange situation and classification procedures to dogs and their human caregivers Attachment amp Human Development 21 4 389 417 doi 10 1080 14616734 2018 1517812 ISSN 1461 6734 PMC 6532729 PMID 30246604 Van IJzendoorn M H amp Kroonenberg P M 1988 Cross cultural patterns of attachment A meta analysis of the strange situation Child Development 59 147 156 Grossmann K E Grossmann K Huber F amp Wartner U 1981 German children s behavior toward their mothers at 12 months and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworth s strange situation International Journal of Behavioral Development 4 157 184 Takahashi K 1986 Examining the Strange Situation procedure with Japanese mothers and 12 month old infants Developmental Psychology 22 265 270 Behrens K Y Main M amp Hesse E 2007 Mothers Attachment Status as Determined by the Adult Attachment Interview Predicts Their 6 Year Olds Reunion Responses A Study Conducted in Japan Developmental Psychology 43 1553 1567 Main M amp Cassidy J 1988 Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6 Predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1 month period Developmental Psychology 24 415 426 Richters J E Waters E amp Vaughn B E 1988 Empirical classification of infant mother relationships from interactive behavior and crying during reunion Child Development 59 512 522 a b Van IJzendoorn M H amp Kroonenberg P M 1990 Cross cultural consistency of coding the strange situation Infant Behavior and Development 13 469 485 Fraley C R amp Spieker S J 2003 Are Infant Attachment Patterns Continuously or Categorically Distributed A Taxometric Analysis of Strange Situation Behavior Developmental Psychology 39 387 404 Further reading editCassidy J amp Shaver P Eds 1999 Handbook of Attachment Theory Research and Clinical Applications Guilford Press NY Greenberg MT Cicchetti D amp Cummings EM Eds 1990 Attachment in the Preschool Years Theory Research and Intervention University of Chicago Chicago Greenspan S 1993 Infancy and Early Childhood Madison CT International Universities Press ISBN 0 8236 2633 4 Holmes J 1993 John Bowlby and Attachment Theory Routledge ISBN 0 415 07730 3 Holmes J 2001 The Search for the Secure Base Attachment Theory and Psychotherapy London Brunner Routledge ISBN 1 58391 152 9 Karen R 1998 Becoming Attached First Relationships and How They Shape Our Capacity to Love Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 511501 5 Parkes CM Stevenson Hinde J Marris P Eds 1991 Attachment Across The Life Cycle Routledge NY ISBN 0 415 05651 9 Siegler R DeLoache J amp Eisenberg N 2003 How Children develop New York Worth ISBN 1 57259 249 4 External links editAICAN Australian Intercountry Adoption Network Attachment Questionnaire Articles on attachment measures including 11 self report measures with scoring instructions Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Attachment measures amp oldid 1188160450, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.