fbpx
Wikipedia

Two-alternative forced choice

Two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) is a method for measuring the sensitivity of a person or animal to some particular sensory input, stimulus, through that observer's pattern of choices and response times to two versions of the sensory input. For example, to determine a person's sensitivity to dim light, the observer would be presented with a series of trials in which a dim light was randomly either in the top or bottom of the display. After each trial, the observer responds "top" or "bottom". The observer is not allowed to say "I do not know", or "I am not sure", or "I did not see anything". In that sense the observer's choice is forced between the two alternatives.

Both options can be presented concurrently (as in the above example) or sequentially in two intervals (also known as two-interval forced choice, 2IFC). For example, to determine sensitivity to a dim light in a two-interval forced choice procedure, an observer could be presented with series of trials comprising two sub-trials (intervals) in which the dim light is presented randomly in the first or the second interval. After each trial, the observer responds only "first" or "second".

The term 2AFC is sometimes used to describe a task in which an observer is presented with a single stimulus and must choose between one of two alternatives. For example in a lexical decision task a participant observes a string of characters and must respond whether the string is a "word" or "non-word". Another example is the random dot kinetogram task, in which a participant must decide whether a group of moving dots are predominately moving "left" or "right". The results of a these tasks, sometimes called yes-no tasks, are much more likely to be affected by various response biases than 2AFC tasks. For example, with extremely dim lights, a person might respond, completely truthfully, "no" (i.e., "I did not see any light") on every trial, whereas the results of a 2AFC task will show the person can reliably determine the location (top or bottom) of the same, extremely dim light.

2AFC is a method of psychophysics developed by Gustav Theodor Fechner.[1]

Behavioural experiments edit

There are various manipulations in the design of the task, engineered to test specific behavioral dynamics of choice. In one well known experiment of attention that examines the attentional shift, the Posner Cueing Task uses a 2AFC design to present two stimuli representing two given locations.[2] In this design there is an arrow that cues which stimulus (location) to attend to. The person then has to make a response between the two stimuli (locations) when prompted. In animals, the 2AFC task has been used to test reinforcement probability learning, for example such as choices in pigeons after reinforcement of trials.[3] A 2AFC task has also been designed to test decision making and the interaction of reward and probability learning in monkeys.[4]

 
Example of a random dot kinetogram as used in a 2AFC task.

Monkeys were trained to look at a center stimulus and were then presented with two salient stimuli side by side. A response can then be made in the form of a saccade to the left or to the right stimulus. A juice reward is then administered after each response. The amount of juice reward is then varied to modulate choice.

In a different application, the 2AFC is designed to test discrimination of motion perception. The random dot motion coherence task, introduces a random dot kinetogram, with a percentage of net coherent motion distributed across the random dots.[5][6] The percentage of dots moving together in a given direction determines the coherence of motion towards the direction. In most experiments, the participant must make a choice response between two directions of motion (e.g. up or down), usually indicated by a motor response such as a saccade or pressing a button.

Biases in decision making edit

It is possible to introduce biases in decision making in the 2AFC task. For example, if one stimulus occurs with more frequency than the other, then the frequency of exposure to the stimuli may influence the participant's beliefs about the probability of the occurrence of the alternatives.[4][7] Introducing biases in the 2AFC task is used to modulate decision making and examine the underlying processes.

Models of decision making edit

The 2AFC task has yielded consistent behavioral results on decision-making, which lead to the development of theoretical and computational models of the dynamics and results of decision-making.[8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15][16][17]

Normal distribution model edit

 
The optimal strategy in a 2AFC task for univariate normal stimuli from categories   and   is to classify between the two joint bivariate normal distributions   and  .[18] The probability of the correct choice is 0.74 here.

Suppose the two stimuli   and   in the 2AFC task are random variables from two different categories   and  , and the task is to decide which was which. A common model is to assume that the stimuli came from normal distributions   and  . Under this normal model, the optimal decision strategy (of the ideal observer) is to decide which of two bivariate normal distributions is more likely to produce the tuple  : the joint distributions of   and  , or of   and  .[18]

The probability of error with this ideal decision strategy is given by the generalized chi-square distribution:  , where  

This model can also extend to the cases when each of the two stimuli is itself a multivariate normal vector, and also to the situations when the two categories have different prior probabilities, or the decisions are biased due to different values attached to the possible outcomes.[18]

Drift-diffusion model edit

There are typically three assumptions made by computational models using the 2AFC:

i) evidence favoring each alternative is integrated over time; ii) the process is subject to random fluctuations; and iii) the decision is made when sufficient evidence has accumulated favoring one alternative over the other.

— Bogacz et al., The Physics of Optimal Decision Making[7]

It is typically assumed that the difference in evidence favoring each alternative is the quantity tracked over time and that which ultimately informs the decision; however, evidence for different alternatives could be tracked separately.[7]

 
Example of six evidence accumulation sequences from an unbiased (100% noise) source. The dotted lines indicate the thresholds for decision making for each of the two alternatives.

The drift-diffusion model (DDM) is a well defined[19] model, that is proposed to implement an optimal decision policy for 2AFC.[20] It is the continuous analog of a random walk model.[7] The DDM assumes that in a 2AFC task, the subject is accumulating evidence for one or other of the alternatives at each time step, and integrating that evidence until a decision threshold is reached. As the sensory input which constitutes the evidence is noisy, the accumulation to the threshold is stochastic rather than deterministic – this gives rise to the directed random-walk-like behavior. The DDM has been shown to describe accuracy and reaction times in human data for 2AFC tasks.[13][19]

Formal model edit

 
Example of ten evidence accumulation sequences for the DDM, where the true result is assigned to the upper threshold. Due to the addition of noise, two sequences have produced an inaccurate decision.

The accumulation of evidence in the DDM is governed according to the following formula:

 [7]

At time zero, the evidence accumulated, x, is set equal to zero. At each time step, some evidence, A, is accumulated for one of the two possibilities in the 2AFC. A is positive if the correct response is represented by the upper threshold, and negative if the lower. In addition, a noise term, cdW, is added to represent noise in input. On average, the noise will integrate to zero.[7] The extended DDM[13] allows for selection of   and the starting value of   from separate distributions – this provides a better fit to experimental data for both accuracy and reaction times.[21][22]

Other models edit

Ornstein–Uhlenbeck model edit

The Ornstein–Uhlenbeck model[14] extends the DDM by adding another term,  , to the accumulation that is dependent on the current accumulation of evidence – this has the net effect of increasing the rate of accumulation towards the initially preferred option.

 [7]

Race model edit

In the race model,[11][12][23] evidence for each alternative is accumulated separately, and a decision made either when one of the accumulators reaches a predetermined threshold, or when a decision is forced and then the decision associated with the accumulator with the highest evidence is chosen. This can be represented formally by:

 [7]

The race model is not mathematically reducible to the DDM,[7] and hence cannot be used to implement an optimal decision procedure.

Mutual inhibition model edit

The mutual inhibition model[16] also uses two accumulators to model the accumulation of evidence, as with the race model. In this model the two accumulators have an inhibitory effect on each other, so as evidence is accumulated in one, it dampens the accumulation of evidence in the other. In addition, leaky accumulators are used, so that over time evidence accumulated decays – this helps to prevent runaway accumulation towards one alternative based on a short run of evidence in one direction. Formally, this can be shown as:

 [7]

Where   is a shared decay rate of the accumulators, and   is the rate of mutual inhibition.

Feedforward inhibition model edit

The feedforward inhibition model[24] is similar to the mutual inhibition model, but instead of being inhibited by the current value of the other accumulator, each accumulator is inhibited by a fraction of the input to the other. It can be formally stated thus:

 [7]

Where   is the fraction of accumulator input that inhibits the alternate accumulator.

Pooled inhibition model edit

Wang[25] suggested the pooled inhibition model, where a third, decaying accumulator is driven by accumulation in both of the accumulators used for decision making, and in addition to the decay used in the mutual inhibition model, each of the decision driving accumulators self-reinforce based on their current value. It can be formally stated thus:

 [7]

The third accumulator has an independent decay coefficient,  , and increases based on the current values of the other two accumulators, at a rate modulated by  .

Neural correlates of decision making edit

Brain areas edit

In the parietal lobe, lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP) neuron firing rate in monkeys predicted the choice response of direction of motion suggesting this area is involved in decision making in the 2AFC.[4][24][26]

Neural data recorded from LIP neurons in rhesus monkeys supports the DDM, as firing rates for the direction selective neuronal populations sensitive to the two directions used in the 2AFC task increase firing rates at stimulus onset, and average activity in the neuronal populations is biased in the direction of the correct response.[24][27][28][29] In addition, it appears that a fixed threshold of neuronal spiking rate is used as the decision boundary for each 2AFC task.[30]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Fechner, Gustav Theodor (1889). Elemente der Psychophysik (2 Volumes) (2nd ed.). Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel. Vol 2.
  2. ^ Posner, M I (February 1980). "Orienting of attention" (PDF). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 32 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1080/00335558008248231. ISSN 0033-555X. PMID 7367577. S2CID 2842391. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  3. ^ Shimp, Charles P. (July 1966). "Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in pigeons". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 9 (4): 443–455. doi:10.1901/jeab.1966.9-443. ISSN 0022-5002. PMC 1338246. PMID 5961513.
  4. ^ a b c Platt, Michael L.; Paul W. Glimcher (1999-07-15). "Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex". Nature. 400 (6741): 233–238. Bibcode:1999Natur.400..233P. doi:10.1038/22268. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 10421364. S2CID 4389701.
  5. ^ Britten, Kenneth H.; Michael N. Shadlen, William T. Newsome, J. Anthony Movshon (1993). "Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals". Visual Neuroscience. 10 (6): 1157–1169. doi:10.1017/S0952523800010269. PMID 8257671. S2CID 38024406.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ Gold, Joshua I.; Michael N. Shadlen (2000-03-23). "Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands". Nature. 404 (6776): 390–394. Bibcode:2000Natur.404..390G. doi:10.1038/35006062. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 10746726. S2CID 4410921.
  7. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Bogacz, Rafal; Eric Brown, Jeff Moehlis, Philip Holmes, Jonathan D. Cohen (October 2006). "The Physics of Optimal Decision Making: A Formal Analysis of Models of Performance in Two-Alternative Forced-Choice Tasks". Psychological Review. 113 (4): 700–765. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.212.9187. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.113.4.700. ISSN 0033-295X. PMID 17014301. Retrieved 2012-06-09.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Stone, M. (1960). "Models for choice-reaction time". Psychometrika. 25 (3): 251–260. doi:10.1007/bf02289729. S2CID 121644881.
  9. ^ Link, S. W.; R. A. Heath (1975). "A sequential theory of psychological discrimination". Psychometrika. 40 (1): 77–105. doi:10.1007/bf02291481. S2CID 49042143.
  10. ^ Link, S. W (1975). "The relative judgment theory of two choice response time". Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 12 (1): 114–135. doi:10.1016/0022-2496(75)90053-x.
  11. ^ a b Pike, A. R. (1966). "Stochastic Models of Choice Behaviour: Response Probabilities and Latencies of Finite Markov Chain Systems1". British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 19 (1): 15–32. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.1966.tb00351.x. PMID 5939142.
  12. ^ a b Vickers, D. (1970). "Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination". Ergonomics. 13 (1): 37–58. doi:10.1080/00140137008931117. PMID 5416868.
  13. ^ a b c Ratcliff, R. (1978). "A theory of memory retrieval". Psychological Review. 85 (2): 59–108. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.85.2.59. S2CID 1166147.
  14. ^ a b Busemeyer, J. R; J. T Townsend (1993). "Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment". Psychological Review. 100 (3): 432–459. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.100.3.432. PMID 8356185. S2CID 15604757.
  15. ^ Ratcliff, R.; T. Van Zandt, G. McKoon (1999). "Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time". Psychological Review. 106 (2): 261–300. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.106.2.261. PMID 10378014.
  16. ^ a b Usher, M.; J. L McClelland (2001). "The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model". Psychological Review. 108 (3): 550–592. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.550. PMID 11488378.
  17. ^ Ratcliff, R.; P. L Smith (2004). "A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time". Psychological Review. 111 (2): 333–367. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.111.2.333. PMC 1440925. PMID 15065913.
  18. ^ a b c Das, Abhranil; Geisler, Wilson (2021). "A method to integrate and classify normal distributions". Journal of Vision. 21 (10): 1. arXiv:2012.14331. doi:10.1167/jov.21.10.1. PMC 8419883. PMID 34468706. S2CID 229934512.
  19. ^ a b Smith, P. L (2000). "Stochastic dynamic models of response time and accuracy: A foundational primer". Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 44 (3): 408–463. doi:10.1006/jmps.1999.1260. PMID 10973778.
  20. ^ Laming, Donald Richard John (1968). Information theory of choice-reaction times. Academic P.
  21. ^ Ratcliff, R.; J. N Rouder (1998). "Modeling response times for two-choice decisions". Psychological Science. 9 (5): 347–356. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00067. S2CID 1045352.
  22. ^ Ratcliff, R.; J. N Rouder (2000). "A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 26 (1): 127–140. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.1.127. PMID 10696609.
  23. ^ LaBerge, D. (1962). "A recruitment theory of simple behavior". Psychometrika. 27 (4): 375–396. doi:10.1007/bf02289645. S2CID 120502141.
  24. ^ a b c Shadlen, M. N.; W. T. Newsome (1996-01-23). "Motion Perception: Seeing and Deciding". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 93 (2): 628–633. Bibcode:1996PNAS...93..628S. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.2.628. ISSN 1091-6490. PMC 40102. PMID 8570606.
  25. ^ Wang, X. J (2002). "Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits". Neuron. 36 (5): 955–968. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01092-9. PMID 12467598.
  26. ^ Shadlen, Michael N.; William T. Newsome (2001-10-01). "Neural Basis of a Perceptual Decision in the Parietal Cortex (Area LIP) of the Rhesus Monkey". Journal of Neurophysiology. 86 (4): 1916–1936. doi:10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916. ISSN 1522-1598. PMID 11600651. S2CID 272332.
  27. ^ Hanes, D. P; J. D Schall (1996). "Neural control of voluntary movement initiation". Science. 274 (5286): 427–430. Bibcode:1996Sci...274..427H. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.5678. doi:10.1126/science.274.5286.427. PMID 8832893. S2CID 11202961.
  28. ^ Schall, J. D; K. G Thompson (1999). "Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 22 (1): 241–259. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.241. PMID 10202539. S2CID 1851931.
  29. ^ Gold, J. I; M. N Shadlen (2002). "Banburismus and the brain: decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward". Neuron. 36 (2): 299–308. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00971-6. PMID 12383783.
  30. ^ Roitman, J. D; M. N Shadlen (2002). "Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task". The Journal of Neuroscience. 22 (21): 9475–9489. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09475.2002. PMC 6758024. PMID 12417672.

alternative, forced, choice, 2afc, method, measuring, sensitivity, person, animal, some, particular, sensory, input, stimulus, through, that, observer, pattern, choices, response, times, versions, sensory, input, example, determine, person, sensitivity, light,. Two alternative forced choice 2AFC is a method for measuring the sensitivity of a person or animal to some particular sensory input stimulus through that observer s pattern of choices and response times to two versions of the sensory input For example to determine a person s sensitivity to dim light the observer would be presented with a series of trials in which a dim light was randomly either in the top or bottom of the display After each trial the observer responds top or bottom The observer is not allowed to say I do not know or I am not sure or I did not see anything In that sense the observer s choice is forced between the two alternatives Both options can be presented concurrently as in the above example or sequentially in two intervals also known as two interval forced choice 2IFC For example to determine sensitivity to a dim light in a two interval forced choice procedure an observer could be presented with series of trials comprising two sub trials intervals in which the dim light is presented randomly in the first or the second interval After each trial the observer responds only first or second The term 2AFC is sometimes used to describe a task in which an observer is presented with a single stimulus and must choose between one of two alternatives For example in a lexical decision task a participant observes a string of characters and must respond whether the string is a word or non word Another example is the random dot kinetogram task in which a participant must decide whether a group of moving dots are predominately moving left or right The results of a these tasks sometimes called yes no tasks are much more likely to be affected by various response biases than 2AFC tasks For example with extremely dim lights a person might respond completely truthfully no i e I did not see any light on every trial whereas the results of a 2AFC task will show the person can reliably determine the location top or bottom of the same extremely dim light 2AFC is a method of psychophysics developed by Gustav Theodor Fechner 1 Contents 1 Behavioural experiments 1 1 Biases in decision making 2 Models of decision making 2 1 Normal distribution model 2 2 Drift diffusion model 2 2 1 Formal model 2 3 Other models 2 3 1 Ornstein Uhlenbeck model 2 3 2 Race model 2 3 3 Mutual inhibition model 2 3 4 Feedforward inhibition model 2 3 5 Pooled inhibition model 3 Neural correlates of decision making 3 1 Brain areas 4 See also 5 ReferencesBehavioural experiments editThere are various manipulations in the design of the task engineered to test specific behavioral dynamics of choice In one well known experiment of attention that examines the attentional shift the Posner Cueing Task uses a 2AFC design to present two stimuli representing two given locations 2 In this design there is an arrow that cues which stimulus location to attend to The person then has to make a response between the two stimuli locations when prompted In animals the 2AFC task has been used to test reinforcement probability learning for example such as choices in pigeons after reinforcement of trials 3 A 2AFC task has also been designed to test decision making and the interaction of reward and probability learning in monkeys 4 nbsp Example of a random dot kinetogram as used in a 2AFC task Monkeys were trained to look at a center stimulus and were then presented with two salient stimuli side by side A response can then be made in the form of a saccade to the left or to the right stimulus A juice reward is then administered after each response The amount of juice reward is then varied to modulate choice In a different application the 2AFC is designed to test discrimination of motion perception The random dot motion coherence task introduces a random dot kinetogram with a percentage of net coherent motion distributed across the random dots 5 6 The percentage of dots moving together in a given direction determines the coherence of motion towards the direction In most experiments the participant must make a choice response between two directions of motion e g up or down usually indicated by a motor response such as a saccade or pressing a button Biases in decision making edit It is possible to introduce biases in decision making in the 2AFC task For example if one stimulus occurs with more frequency than the other then the frequency of exposure to the stimuli may influence the participant s beliefs about the probability of the occurrence of the alternatives 4 7 Introducing biases in the 2AFC task is used to modulate decision making and examine the underlying processes Models of decision making editThe 2AFC task has yielded consistent behavioral results on decision making which lead to the development of theoretical and computational models of the dynamics and results of decision making 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Normal distribution model edit nbsp The optimal strategy in a 2AFC task for univariate normal stimuli from categories a displaystyle a nbsp and b displaystyle b nbsp is to classify between the two joint bivariate normal distributions a b displaystyle ab nbsp and b a displaystyle ba nbsp 18 The probability of the correct choice is 0 74 here Suppose the two stimuli x 1 displaystyle x 1 nbsp and x 2 displaystyle x 2 nbsp in the 2AFC task are random variables from two different categories a displaystyle a nbsp and b displaystyle b nbsp and the task is to decide which was which A common model is to assume that the stimuli came from normal distributions N m a s a displaystyle N mu a sigma a nbsp and N m b s b displaystyle N mu b sigma b nbsp Under this normal model the optimal decision strategy of the ideal observer is to decide which of two bivariate normal distributions is more likely to produce the tuple x 1 x 2 displaystyle x 1 x 2 nbsp the joint distributions of a displaystyle a nbsp and b displaystyle b nbsp or of b displaystyle b nbsp and a displaystyle a nbsp 18 The probability of error with this ideal decision strategy is given by the generalized chi square distribution p e p x w k l 0 0 2 lt 0 displaystyle p e p left tilde chi boldsymbol w boldsymbol k boldsymbol lambda 0 0 2 right lt 0 nbsp where w s a 2 s b 2 k 1 1 l m a m b s a 2 s b 2 s a 2 s b 2 displaystyle boldsymbol w begin bmatrix sigma a 2 amp sigma b 2 end bmatrix boldsymbol k begin bmatrix 1 amp 1 end bmatrix boldsymbol lambda frac mu a mu b sigma a 2 sigma b 2 begin bmatrix sigma a 2 amp sigma b 2 end bmatrix nbsp This model can also extend to the cases when each of the two stimuli is itself a multivariate normal vector and also to the situations when the two categories have different prior probabilities or the decisions are biased due to different values attached to the possible outcomes 18 Drift diffusion model editThere are typically three assumptions made by computational models using the 2AFC i evidence favoring each alternative is integrated over time ii the process is subject to random fluctuations and iii the decision is made when sufficient evidence has accumulated favoring one alternative over the other Bogacz et al The Physics of Optimal Decision Making 7 It is typically assumed that the difference in evidence favoring each alternative is the quantity tracked over time and that which ultimately informs the decision however evidence for different alternatives could be tracked separately 7 nbsp Example of six evidence accumulation sequences from an unbiased 100 noise source The dotted lines indicate the thresholds for decision making for each of the two alternatives The drift diffusion model DDM is a well defined 19 model that is proposed to implement an optimal decision policy for 2AFC 20 It is the continuous analog of a random walk model 7 The DDM assumes that in a 2AFC task the subject is accumulating evidence for one or other of the alternatives at each time step and integrating that evidence until a decision threshold is reached As the sensory input which constitutes the evidence is noisy the accumulation to the threshold is stochastic rather than deterministic this gives rise to the directed random walk like behavior The DDM has been shown to describe accuracy and reaction times in human data for 2AFC tasks 13 19 Formal model edit nbsp Example of ten evidence accumulation sequences for the DDM where the true result is assigned to the upper threshold Due to the addition of noise two sequences have produced an inaccurate decision The accumulation of evidence in the DDM is governed according to the following formula d x A d t c d W x 0 0 displaystyle dx Adt cdW x 0 0 nbsp 7 At time zero the evidence accumulated x is set equal to zero At each time step some evidence A is accumulated for one of the two possibilities in the 2AFC A is positive if the correct response is represented by the upper threshold and negative if the lower In addition a noise term cdW is added to represent noise in input On average the noise will integrate to zero 7 The extended DDM 13 allows for selection of A displaystyle A nbsp and the starting value of x 0 displaystyle x 0 nbsp from separate distributions this provides a better fit to experimental data for both accuracy and reaction times 21 22 Other models edit Ornstein Uhlenbeck model edit The Ornstein Uhlenbeck model 14 extends the DDM by adding another term l displaystyle lambda nbsp to the accumulation that is dependent on the current accumulation of evidence this has the net effect of increasing the rate of accumulation towards the initially preferred option d x l x A d t c d W displaystyle dx lambda x A dt cdW nbsp 7 Race model edit In the race model 11 12 23 evidence for each alternative is accumulated separately and a decision made either when one of the accumulators reaches a predetermined threshold or when a decision is forced and then the decision associated with the accumulator with the highest evidence is chosen This can be represented formally by d y 1 I 1 d t c d W 1 d y 2 I 2 d t c d W 2 y 1 0 y 2 0 0 displaystyle begin aligned dy text 1 I text 1 dt cdW text 1 dy text 2 I text 2 dt cdW text 2 end aligned quad y text 1 0 y text 2 0 0 nbsp 7 The race model is not mathematically reducible to the DDM 7 and hence cannot be used to implement an optimal decision procedure Mutual inhibition model edit The mutual inhibition model 16 also uses two accumulators to model the accumulation of evidence as with the race model In this model the two accumulators have an inhibitory effect on each other so as evidence is accumulated in one it dampens the accumulation of evidence in the other In addition leaky accumulators are used so that over time evidence accumulated decays this helps to prevent runaway accumulation towards one alternative based on a short run of evidence in one direction Formally this can be shown as d y 1 k y 1 w y 2 I 1 d t c d W 1 d y 2 k y 2 w y 1 I 2 d t c d W 2 y 1 0 y 2 0 0 displaystyle begin aligned dy text 1 ky text 1 wy text 2 I text 1 dt cdW text 1 dy text 2 ky text 2 wy text 1 I text 2 dt cdW text 2 end aligned quad y text 1 0 y text 2 0 0 nbsp 7 Where k displaystyle k nbsp is a shared decay rate of the accumulators and w displaystyle w nbsp is the rate of mutual inhibition Feedforward inhibition model edit The feedforward inhibition model 24 is similar to the mutual inhibition model but instead of being inhibited by the current value of the other accumulator each accumulator is inhibited by a fraction of the input to the other It can be formally stated thus d y 1 I 1 d t c d W 1 u I 2 d t c d W 2 d y 2 I 2 d t c d W 2 u I 1 d t c d W 1 y 1 0 y 2 0 0 displaystyle begin aligned dy text 1 I text 1 dt cdW text 1 u I text 2 dt cdW text 2 dy text 2 I text 2 dt cdW text 2 u I text 1 dt cdW text 1 end aligned quad y text 1 0 y text 2 0 0 nbsp 7 Where u displaystyle u nbsp is the fraction of accumulator input that inhibits the alternate accumulator Pooled inhibition model edit Wang 25 suggested the pooled inhibition model where a third decaying accumulator is driven by accumulation in both of the accumulators used for decision making and in addition to the decay used in the mutual inhibition model each of the decision driving accumulators self reinforce based on their current value It can be formally stated thus d y 1 k y 1 w y 3 v y 1 I 1 d t c d W 1 d y 2 k y 2 w y 3 v y 2 I 2 d t c d W 2 d y 3 k inh y 3 w y 1 y 2 d t displaystyle begin aligned dy text 1 ky text 1 wy text 3 vy text 1 I text 1 dt cdW text 1 dy text 2 ky text 2 wy text 3 vy text 2 I text 2 dt cdW text 2 dy text 3 k text inh y text 3 w y text 1 y text 2 dt end aligned nbsp 7 The third accumulator has an independent decay coefficient k inh displaystyle k text inh nbsp and increases based on the current values of the other two accumulators at a rate modulated by w displaystyle w nbsp Neural correlates of decision making editBrain areas edit In the parietal lobe lateral intraparietal cortex LIP neuron firing rate in monkeys predicted the choice response of direction of motion suggesting this area is involved in decision making in the 2AFC 4 24 26 Neural data recorded from LIP neurons in rhesus monkeys supports the DDM as firing rates for the direction selective neuronal populations sensitive to the two directions used in the 2AFC task increase firing rates at stimulus onset and average activity in the neuronal populations is biased in the direction of the correct response 24 27 28 29 In addition it appears that a fixed threshold of neuronal spiking rate is used as the decision boundary for each 2AFC task 30 See also editChoice modelling Choice set Julian RotterReferences edit Fechner Gustav Theodor 1889 Elemente der Psychophysik 2 Volumes 2nd ed Leipzig Breitkopf amp Hartel Vol 2 Posner M I February 1980 Orienting of attention PDF Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32 1 3 25 doi 10 1080 00335558008248231 ISSN 0033 555X PMID 7367577 S2CID 2842391 Retrieved 2012 06 12 Shimp Charles P July 1966 Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in pigeons Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9 4 443 455 doi 10 1901 jeab 1966 9 443 ISSN 0022 5002 PMC 1338246 PMID 5961513 a b c Platt Michael L Paul W Glimcher 1999 07 15 Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex Nature 400 6741 233 238 Bibcode 1999Natur 400 233P doi 10 1038 22268 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 10421364 S2CID 4389701 Britten Kenneth H Michael N Shadlen William T Newsome J Anthony Movshon 1993 Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals Visual Neuroscience 10 6 1157 1169 doi 10 1017 S0952523800010269 PMID 8257671 S2CID 38024406 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Gold Joshua I Michael N Shadlen 2000 03 23 Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands Nature 404 6776 390 394 Bibcode 2000Natur 404 390G doi 10 1038 35006062 ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 10746726 S2CID 4410921 a b c d e f g h i j k l Bogacz Rafal Eric Brown Jeff Moehlis Philip Holmes Jonathan D Cohen October 2006 The Physics of Optimal Decision Making A Formal Analysis of Models of Performance in Two Alternative Forced Choice Tasks Psychological Review 113 4 700 765 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 212 9187 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 113 4 700 ISSN 0033 295X PMID 17014301 Retrieved 2012 06 09 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Stone M 1960 Models for choice reaction time Psychometrika 25 3 251 260 doi 10 1007 bf02289729 S2CID 121644881 Link S W R A Heath 1975 A sequential theory of psychological discrimination Psychometrika 40 1 77 105 doi 10 1007 bf02291481 S2CID 49042143 Link S W 1975 The relative judgment theory of two choice response time Journal of Mathematical Psychology 12 1 114 135 doi 10 1016 0022 2496 75 90053 x a b Pike A R 1966 Stochastic Models of Choice Behaviour Response Probabilities and Latencies of Finite Markov Chain Systems1 British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 19 1 15 32 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8317 1966 tb00351 x PMID 5939142 a b Vickers D 1970 Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination Ergonomics 13 1 37 58 doi 10 1080 00140137008931117 PMID 5416868 a b c Ratcliff R 1978 A theory of memory retrieval Psychological Review 85 2 59 108 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 85 2 59 S2CID 1166147 a b Busemeyer J R J T Townsend 1993 Decision field theory a dynamic cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment Psychological Review 100 3 432 459 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 100 3 432 PMID 8356185 S2CID 15604757 Ratcliff R T Van Zandt G McKoon 1999 Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time Psychological Review 106 2 261 300 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 106 2 261 PMID 10378014 a b Usher M J L McClelland 2001 The time course of perceptual choice the leaky competing accumulator model Psychological Review 108 3 550 592 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 108 3 550 PMID 11488378 Ratcliff R P L Smith 2004 A comparison of sequential sampling models for two choice reaction time Psychological Review 111 2 333 367 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 111 2 333 PMC 1440925 PMID 15065913 a b c Das Abhranil Geisler Wilson 2021 A method to integrate and classify normal distributions Journal of Vision 21 10 1 arXiv 2012 14331 doi 10 1167 jov 21 10 1 PMC 8419883 PMID 34468706 S2CID 229934512 a b Smith P L 2000 Stochastic dynamic models of response time and accuracy A foundational primer Journal of Mathematical Psychology 44 3 408 463 doi 10 1006 jmps 1999 1260 PMID 10973778 Laming Donald Richard John 1968 Information theory of choice reaction times Academic P Ratcliff R J N Rouder 1998 Modeling response times for two choice decisions Psychological Science 9 5 347 356 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 00067 S2CID 1045352 Ratcliff R J N Rouder 2000 A diffusion model account of masking in two choice letter identification Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 26 1 127 140 doi 10 1037 0096 1523 26 1 127 PMID 10696609 LaBerge D 1962 A recruitment theory of simple behavior Psychometrika 27 4 375 396 doi 10 1007 bf02289645 S2CID 120502141 a b c Shadlen M N W T Newsome 1996 01 23 Motion Perception Seeing and Deciding Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93 2 628 633 Bibcode 1996PNAS 93 628S doi 10 1073 pnas 93 2 628 ISSN 1091 6490 PMC 40102 PMID 8570606 Wang X J 2002 Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits Neuron 36 5 955 968 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 02 01092 9 PMID 12467598 Shadlen Michael N William T Newsome 2001 10 01 Neural Basis of a Perceptual Decision in the Parietal Cortex Area LIP of the Rhesus Monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 86 4 1916 1936 doi 10 1152 jn 2001 86 4 1916 ISSN 1522 1598 PMID 11600651 S2CID 272332 Hanes D P J D Schall 1996 Neural control of voluntary movement initiation Science 274 5286 427 430 Bibcode 1996Sci 274 427H CiteSeerX 10 1 1 408 5678 doi 10 1126 science 274 5286 427 PMID 8832893 S2CID 11202961 Schall J D K G Thompson 1999 Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements Annual Review of Neuroscience 22 1 241 259 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 22 1 241 PMID 10202539 S2CID 1851931 Gold J I M N Shadlen 2002 Banburismus and the brain decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli decisions and reward Neuron 36 2 299 308 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 02 00971 6 PMID 12383783 Roitman J D M N Shadlen 2002 Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task The Journal of Neuroscience 22 21 9475 9489 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 22 21 09475 2002 PMC 6758024 PMID 12417672 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Two alternative forced choice amp oldid 1221000674, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.